A COMPARISON OF THREE MODELS FOR A HUMPHREYS-TYPE CONDITIONING SITUATION BY RICHARD C. ATKINSON TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 5 NOVEMBER 20, 1956 PREPARED Ul'IDER CONTRACT Nonr 225(17) (NR 171-034) FOR OFFICE OF NAVAL RESEARCH REPRODUCTION IN WHOLE OR IN PART IS PERMITTED FOR ANY PURPOSE OF THE UNITED STATES GOVERNMENT APPLIED MATHEMATICS AND STATISTICS LABORATORY STANFORD UNIVERSITY STANFORD, CALIFORNIA A COMPARISON OF THREE MODELS FOR A HUMPHREYS-TYPE SITUATION~/ CONDITIONING by Richard C. Atkinsong/ Summary. Three models for a Humphreys-type conditioning situation are presented. In model I experimental trials are viewed as discrete units, and the possible influence of trace stimuli on behavior is not considered. Models II and III are members ofa class of representations which incorporates a concept of trace stimuli as determining components of SUbsequent behavior. Functions expressing the expected probabilities of responses are derived and predictions for the three models compared. The purpose of this paper is to provide an analysis ofa Humphreys-type conditioning situation in terms of statistical learning theory [3,4,6]. We consider an experimental situation in which each trial begins with the presentation of a signal. events, E l series being or Following the signal, one or the other of two reinforcing E , occurs; the probability of 2 rr and (l-rr) respectively. dict on each trial which event, E l or E l E l E 2 during a given The subject is instructed to pre- E , will occur. 2 to the subject are categorized into two classes, A l is a prediction by the subject that and The behaviors available and A ; an 2 will occur, and an A 2 A l response response is a ~/ This research was supported the Sciences Division of the Ford Foundation and in part by the Office of Naval Research under Contract NR 171g/ The author wishes to thank ProfessorP. Suppes for suggestions in carrying out this research. -2- prediction that E 2 will occur. In analyzing the situation the experimental psychologist is primarily interested in two questions: (a) what is the relation between asymptotic probability of an between 2. Al ~ and the response and (b) what is the relation and the rate of approach to the asymptote. ~ Model L Several investigators [1,2,5,7] have,provided the following interpretation of the situation in terms of statistical learning theory. They suggest that the stimulus governing the subjects response on each trial is the signal. The signal is conceptualized as a population, S , of stimulus c elements which is sampled by the subject on each presentation of the signal; the probability of any given element being sampled is ... principles [4] an element sampled from ditioned to response an E trial Al event occurs. 2 n if an E l S c e. By association on a trial will become con- event occurs and to response The probability of an Al A 2 response at the end of is defined in the model as the proportion of elements in are conditioned to if AI' and similarly for the probability of an A2· S c ')j We can then define the probability, p(n), that a given element in is conditioned to (1) Al at the start of trial pen) = (l-e)p(n-l) + pen) =: (l-e)p(n-l) e n that S c as if an E l occurred on trial n-l , if an E 2 occurred on trial n-l or 1/ The reader is referred to Estes and Burke rationale underlying these assumptions. . [4] for a statement of the -3- This leads to an expected difference equation pen) = (1-9)p(n-l) + 91C, whose solution is pen) pea) where A l = 1C - [1C - p(a)](1_9)n , is the probability that the given element is conditioned to an response at the start of the first trial. The mean value of pen) portion of elements conditioned to for all elements in of an A l S over all elements in A . l is the expected pro- c We have assumed that S , and may therefore interpret c response at the start of trial pen) 9 is the same as the probability n. By inspection of equation (3) we see model I predicts that (a) the probability of an rate of A l response approaches approach~/ is independent of 1C as n becomes large, and (b) the 1C. In the remaining part of this paper we develop alternative formalizations of the stimulus governing the subject's response and investigate the relationships between these models and the above model. 3. Model II. We assume that the stimulus governing the elicitation of a response on each trial is a compound of both (a) the signal stimulus and (b) the reinforcing stimulus of the previous trial. Let ~7 power n. S c represent the set of stimulus elements associated with the Rate of approach, in this paper, refers to the term raised to the For example in equation (3), the term (1-8). -4- signal and 8. 1 the set associated with the occurrence ofE. (i=1,2); assume 1 the three sets are pairwise disjoint. 8 c is 8', with 8 1 is The sampling parameter associated with 8 , and with 1 arrangements it is natural to assume Then on trial n 8 and c 2 is 8 , 2 = 82 ; 8 1 For most experimental hence, to simplify notation, the stimulus governing the probability of response is composed of (a) samples from (b) samples from 8 8 c and if 8 1 if E l occurred on trial occurred on trial n-l n-l. We define the following probabilities. p (n): c probability that a given element in at the start of trial Pl(n): P2(n): 8 1 is conditioned to Al 8 is conditioned to ~ n. probability that a given element in at the start of trial Al c n. probability that a given element in at the start of trial is conditioned to 8 2 n. By the same development employed in model I, p (n) = ~ - [~ - p (O)](1_8,)n . c c (4) For Pl(n), however, we have a probability ~ on each trial that is available for sampling and, in addition, a probability element is sampled. that an element in That is, on any trial 8 1 is sampled. Hence n e 8 1 that a given there is a probability 8~ and -5- P (n) = (1-811: )Pl (n-l) + 8 11: l if an E l occurs on trial n-l, if an E 2 occurs on trial n-l . or The expected difference equation is then (6) A similar argument leads to the following expression for P2(n). Solving equations (6) and (7) we obtain ( 8) where Pl(l) and conditioned to Next define ditioned to the P2(1) A at the start of the second trial. l p.[nIE.] l A l l as the probability that an element in response at the start of trial event occurred on trial (10) represent the probability that a given element is n-l. S. l n, given that an is conE. By conditional probability considerations l -6- and (11) One final definition is required before we can write the probability of an A l response associated with the compound stimulus of S c and of Sl is Sl the effect of (1-0: ) . 1 S c Similarly, 0: 2 S is defined for and S c 0: = 0: 1 2 We can now write the expected probability of an A l In the presence S.l ' on response probability is experimental arrangements, it is natural to assume of trial and c 0: 1 S2' and the effect Again, in most and hence we let response at the start n, Substituting equations (8) and (9) into equations (10) and (11) and, in turn, substituting the results into equations (12) yields the fOllowing expression, - o:[rr-p (O)](l_e,)n c - (l-o:)rr[ rr - Pl (1)] (l_err)n-l The function is defined for n=1,2, "0 For the first trial (n=O) we -7- let p(O) = Pc (0). An inspection of equation (13) indicates that for a < 1, pen) proaches an asymptote above for For ~ for ~ ::::: 0, 1 2 ' or 21 < ~ < 1 and an asymptote below the asymptote is 1 e, e approach to the asymptote is a function of ap- i and ~. Further, the a = 1, equa- For ~. ~ tion (13) reduces to equation (3). 4. Model III. We assume that the stimulus which determines response proba- bility on each trial is a compound of the reinforcing stimuli of the two previous trials. More specifically, there are four stimuli, one of which is present on each trial, that determine response probability. We define the following fourpaiFwise disjoint sets of stimulus elements. 8 .. : lJ set available for sampling on trial ing event occurred on trial occurred on trial n-2 n-l, where n given that an and an i=1,2 l reinforc- reinforcing event E. J and E. j:::::l,2. Again we assume the sampling constants associated with the four sets are equal and denoted by Next define 6. Pij(n) is conditioned to the as the probability that a given element in set A l response at the start of trial 8 .. lJ n. By considerations similar to those for equation (5) we obtain for an element in 8 11 a probability ~2 that the set ing on a given trial and, hence, a probability 8 11 (14) is sampled on the trial. 2 8 11 6 ~2 is available for sampthat a given element Therefore ::::: (1-611: )Pll (n-l) + e ~ 2 if occurred on trial n-l -8- or Pll(n) = 2 (1-81C )P ll (n-l) if E 2 occurred on trial n-l. This leads to the expected difference expression By identical considerations we obtain (16) (18) Next define p .. [nIE.E.] lJ l J is conditioned to Al occurred on trial n-2 as the probability that an element in at the start of trial and E. J on trial n n-l. given that an Ei S.. lJ event By conditional probability considerations 2 2 2 [Pll(n-2)(1-8) + 8(1-8) + 8] + [1-1C ]Pll(n-2) , = 1C -9- We can now define the expected probability of an n A l response on trial as p(n) Solving recursive expressions (15)-(18), substituting the results in equations (19)-(22), and in turn substituting these results in equation (23) we obtain for the probability of an A l response at the start of trial n n-2 2 2 2 - ~ [1 - ~ (28 - 8 )]711~11 where 2 2 7ij = ~ - Pij(2), ~ll = 1 - 8 ~ , ~22 = 1 - 8(1-~) , and = 1 - 8 ~ (l-~). The function is defined for n=2,3,... . ~12 = ~21 In dealing with most experimental situations where no initial preference exists between and A 2 5. i t would be reasonable to assume Comparison of Model I and Model III. with a comparison between model I and III. 1 p(O) = 2 and 1 p(l) = (1-8)2 + 8~. In this section we are concerned But it should be noted that for all comparisons the result obtained by model II, for any by the results of models I and III. A l 0, will be bounded -10- Let and (24). PI(n) PIII(n) be the probability of an A l response defined in equation (3) be the probability of the same response as defined in equation Further, for simplicity let is not defined, let PI(l) = 21 PI(O) =PIII(O) and, since PIII(l) = PIII(l). An inspection of equations (3) and (24) indicates that the asymptotic values for model I and model III are equal for interval 0 < :rr <~, P ( 00) > P I ( 00) lrr :rr = 0, 21 ' ~ < :rr < while for or 1. In the 1, P ( 00) r < P rrI ( 00 Next, define the functions N-l X (N,11:) = N 11: r L. Pr(i) , i=O and (26) For :rr = 1 and lim N Using the value of XIII(N,:rr) 1 X(N,l) - 28 ~ 00 e obtained in equation (27) we can compute Xr(N,:rr) and for any value of 11:. Xr(N,:rr) = XrrI (N,11:); for :rr = 0'21 ' or 1; X (N,11:) < X (N,11:), for all other values of I rrr :rr. Stated differently, the rate of approach to the asymptote for or 1 I and rII, but for other values of :rr = 0 is identical for models :rr, the rate predicted by model I is greater than the prediction by model III. ). -11- References 1. Atkinson, R. C. An analysis of the effect of non-reinforced trials in ~' terms of statistical learning theory. expo Psychol., 1956, 51, in press. 2. Burke, C.J., Estes, W. K., and Hellyer, S. J. expo Psychol., 1954, 48, 153-161. in relation to stimulus variability. 3. Estes, W. K. Rate of verbal conditioning Toward a statistical theory of learning. Psychol. Rev., 1950, 57, 94-107· 4. Estes, W. K., and Burke, C. J. ing. A theory of stimulus variability in learn- Psychol. Rev., 1953, 60, 276-286. 5. Estes, W. K., and Straughan, J. H. Analysis of a verbal conditioning situation in terms of statistical learning theory. ~' expo Psychol., 1954, 47, 225-234. 6. Suppes, P., and Estes, W. K. I. 7. Foundations of statistical learning theory, Forthcoming as a technical report. Neimark, Edith D. Effects of type of non-reinforcement and number of alternative responses in two verbal conditioning situations. Psychol., 1956, 52, 209-220. ~. expo STANFORD UNIVERSITY Technical Reports Distribution List Contract Nonr 225(17) (NR 171-034) ASTIA Documents Service Knott Building Dayton 2, Ohio Center Commanding Officer Office of Naval Research Branch Office Navy #100, Fleet Post Office New York, New York Director, Naval Research Laboratory Attn~ Technical Information Officer Washington 25, D. C. 5 35 5 Office of Naval Research Branch Office 346 Broadway New York 13, New York 1 Office of Naval Research Branch Office 1030 Green Street Pasadena 1, Calif. 1 Office of Naval Research Logistics Branch, Code 436 Department of the Navy Washington 25, D. C. 1 Operations Resea~ch Office 7100 Connecticut Avenue Chevy Chase, Maryland Attn: The Library 1 The Rand Corporation 1700 Main Street Santa Monica, Calif. Attn: Dr. John Kennedy 1 The Logistics Research Project The George Washington Univ. 707 - 22nd Street, N. W. Washington 7, D. C. 1 Dr. R. F. Bales Department of Social Relations Harvard University Cambridge, Massachusetts 1 Dr. Alex Bavelas Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge, Massachusetts 1 Dr. Donald Campbell Department of Psychology Northwestern University Evanston; Illinois 1 6 Office of Naval Research Group Psychology Branch Code 452 Department of the Navy Washington 25, D. C. Office of Naval Research Branch Office 1000 Geary Street San Francisco 9, Calif. Office of Naval Research Mathematics Division, Code 430 Department of the Navy Washington 25, D. C. 1 1 Offic~ of Naval Research Branch Office Tenth Floor The John Crerar Library Building 86 East Randolph Street Chicago 1, Illinois 1 Office of Technical Services Department of Commerce Washington 25, D. C. 1 Dr. Clyde H. Coombs Bureau of Psychological Services University of Michigan 1027 E. Huron Street Ann Arbor, Michigan 1 -ii- Dr. Mort Deutsch Graduate School of Arts ~ Sciences New York University Washington Square New York 3, New York 1 Dr. Leon Festinger Department of Psychology Stanford University 1 Dr. Leo A. Goodman Statistical Research Center University of Chicago Chicago 37, Illinois Dr. Harry Helson Department of Psychology University of Texas Austin, Texas Dr. William E. Kappauf Department of Psychology University of Illinois Urbana, Illinois Dr. Leo Katz Department of Mathematics Michigan State College East Lansing, Michigan 1 Dr. Theodore M. Newcomb Department of Psychology University of Michigan Ann Arbor, Michigan 1 Dr. Helen Peak Department of Psychology University of Michigan Ann Arbor, Michigan 1 Dr. George Saslow Department of Neuropsychiatry Washington University 640 South Kingshighway St. Louis, Missouri 1 Dr. C. P. Seitz Special Devices Center Office of Naval Research Sands Point Port Washington Long Island, New York 1 Dr. Marvin Shaw The Johns Hopkins University Mergenthaler Hall Baltimore, Maryland 1 Dr. Herbert Solomon Teachers College Columbia University New York, New York 1 Dr. F. F. Stephan Box 337 Princeton University Princeton, New Jersey 1 Dr. Dewey B. Stuit 108 Schaeffer Hall State University of Iowa Iowa City, Iowa 1 1 Dr. Francis J. DiVesta Department of Psychology Syracuse University 123 College Place Syracuse, New York Dr. Murray Gerstenhaber University of Pennsylvania Philadelphia, Pennsylvania Dr. O. K. Moore Department of Sociology Box 1965 Yale Station New Haven, Conn. 1 1 1 1 1 Dr. Duncan Luce Bureau of Applied Social Research Colu~bia Universit~ New York 27~ New York Dr. Nathan Maccoby Boston University Graduate School Boston 15, Massachusetts 1 1 -iii- Dr. Robert L. Thorndike Teachers College Columbia University New York, New York Dr. E. Paul Torrance Survival Research Field Unit Crew Research Laboratory AFP & TRC Stead Air Force Base Reno, Nevada Dr. John T. Wilson National Science Foundation 1520 H Street, N. W. Washington 25, D. C. ProfessorK. J. Arrow Department of Economics Stanford University Stanford, Calif. Professor M. Flood Department of Industrial Engineering Columbia University New York 27, New York Professor Jacob Marschak Behavioral Science Center Stanford University Stanford, Calif. Professor OskarMorgenstern Department of Economics & Social Institutions Princeton University Princeton, New Jersey Professor Nicholos Rashevsky University of Chicago Chicago 37, Illinois Professor David Rosenblatt American University Washington 6, D. C. Professor Tsunehiko Watanabe Economics Department Stanford University Stanford, California 1 ::professor Alan J.Rowe Manage~ent Sciences Research Project University of California Los Angeles 24, California 1 Professor L. J. Savage Committee on Statistics University of Chicago Chicago, Illinois 1 1 Professor Herbert Simon Carnegie Institute of Technology Schenley Park Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 1 1 Professor R. M. Thrall University of Michigan Engineering Research Institute Ann Arbor, Michigan 1 Professor A. W. Tucker Department of Mathematics Princeton University, Fine Hall Princeton, New Jersey 1 Professor J. Wolfowitz Department of Mathematics Cornell University Ithaca, New York 1 Professor Maurice Allais 15 Rue des Gates-Ceps Saint-Cloud, (S.-O.) France 1 ProfessorE. W. Beth Bern, Zweerskade 23, I Amsterdam Z., The Netherlands 1 Professor R. B. Braithwaite King's College Cambridge, England 1 Professor Maurice Fr~chet Institut H. Poincare 11 Rue P. Curie Paris 5, France 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 -iv- Dr. Ian P. Howard Department of Psychology University of Durham 7, Kepier Terrace Gilesgate Durham, England 1 Dr. T. T. ten Have Sociaal - Paed. Instituut Singel 453 Amsterdam, Netherlands 1 Additional copies for project leader and assistants and reserve for future requirements 25