Introduction Outline of Presentation Quick Review of Attachment Styles/Patterns o Bowlby and Ainworth Zeanah & Smyke (2008) - The most important breakthrough in attachment research came when Mary Ainsworth and her colleagues developed a laboratory paradigm for the assessment of attachment in infancy known as the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP;Ainsworth, Blehar,Waters,&Wall, 1978). Important work by Sroufe (2005) and colleagues (Weinfield, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, 1999) established its construct validity for the assessment of the quality of parent–child attachment in young children. Main and colleagues’ (Main&Hesse, 1990; Main&Solomon, 1990) description of disorganized attachment extended the value of SSP classifications to clinical populations of young children, enhancing the interest of clinicians. The SSP has been used in hundreds of studies of attachment around the world, and although often maligned, it is still widely considered the gold standard for assessing quality of attachment in the early years. It has been emphasized repeatedly that SSP classifications of secure, avoidant, resistant, and disorganized are best considered risk and protective factors for disorders rather than diagnostic entities themselves. Important work has established increased risk for anxiety disorders, disruptive behavior disorders, dissociative disorders, substance use, delinquency, and personality disorders among children with insecure, and especially, disorganized attachments, to their primary caregivers (Hauser&BormanSpurrell, 1996; Carlson, 1998; DeKlyen, 1996; Lyons-Ruth, 1996; Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996; Warren, Huston, Egeland, & Sroufe, 1997). Hardy (2007) – Attachment theory (AT) was developed by John Bowlby in the 1960’s. Bowlby was a psychoanalyst who recognized the importance of a child’s early relationship with the primary caregiver in the future development of the child’s personality. AT basically suggests that infants are evolutionarily primed to form a close, enduring, dependent bond on a primary caregiver beginning in the first moments of life. HARDY (2007) - The vulnerability of the infant requires that care be provided by an adult, and the infant’s behaviors and inherent faculties ensure that a bond will be created. Infants attend to human voices, recognize human faces, and gaze into parents’ eyes when being fed. They look to the attachment object for cues when faced with novel stimuli. The fulfillment of their physiological needs requires close and frequent physical contact throughout infancy (Carlson, Sampson, & Sroufe, 2003). As they develop the capacity for locomotion and intentional movement, they attempt to maintain physical proximity to the caregiver and frequently return for “refueling” when they are involved in an individual activity (Ainsworth et al., 1978). Since infants are intrinsically driven to form attachments, they will attach to o o the primary caregiver regardless of the type of interactions that occur. Thus, attachment status is classified according to quality rather thanquantity (Main, 1996). HARDY (2007)Four infant attachment styles have been identified: secure, avoidant, resistant-ambivalent, and disorganizeddisoriented. Schwartz(2006) - Attachment theory provides a useful lens through which to view the development of early relationships and the potential trajectory for later interpersonal functioning. Bowlby (1982) defined attachment from a bioevolutionary perspective, concluding that attachment is a fundamental human need based in biology and intimately related to the survival of the species. More specifically, attachment is the organization of a set of specific behaviors designed to achieve four primary goals: proximity, security, safety, and regulation of a child’s affective states (Bowlby, 1982; Schore, 2001). Recently, Schore (2001) reiterated Bowlby’s hypothesis, notingthat an infant’s adaptive and coping capacities play a prominent and central role in their mental health and that these domains of functioning cannot be separated or understood apart from the child’s attachment and relationship with his or her caregiver. Secure Attachment HARDY (2007) - Infants with a secure pattern of attachment typically protest when they are separated from their caregiver, and they attempt to regain closeness to the caregiver upon reunion. Schwartz (2006) - In the development of a secure attachment, the infant is able to draw upon the mother for responsive, nurturing, and reliable care for regulating distress (Stern, 1985). Essentially, the presence of a responsive, nurturing caregiver helps regulate the infant’s arousal state and emotions, resulting in the infant’s expanding and growing capacity to learn the skills necessary for self-regulation, ability to cope with stress, and the development of a secure attachment. In the context of a secure attachment, children are thus likely to be better equipped to regulate their own affect. In turn, children who can better regulate their emotions and behavior are more likely to be ready to enter school and function effectively once in school. On the other hand, children with early relational trauma, histories of maltreatment, and disruptions in their earliest relationships have been shown to be at significant risk for both internalizing and externalizing behavior problems (Kennedy & Kennedy, 2004). Disorganized Attachment HARDY (2007) - The disorganized style of attachment is typically seen in infants who have been maltreated by their attachment figure. They exhibit conflicted behaviors such as simultaneously reaching for and turning away from the caregiver. This is most likely related to the inherent conflict between the attachment object being both the cause of distress and the infant’s only potential source of comfort from distress. The disorganized attachment style is thought to be most correlated with psychopathology (Main, 1996). o Resistant-Ambivalent Attachment HARDY (2007) The resistant-ambivalent pattern is characterized by a preoccupation or fixation on the caregiver in which the caregiver is alternately sought for comfort and rejected. o Avoidant Attachment HARDY (2007 - The avoidant attachment style involves behaviors that resemble rejection. Infants with this pattern tend to ignore the caregiver’s departure and return and actively avoid the caregiver’s attempts to regain contact. Attachment Disorders o RAD o Schwartz (2006) - Children with early relational trauma and disruptions in the primary infant–parent relationship, resulting from grossly pathological care and maltreatment, may be diagnosed with attachment disorders and, in some cases, RAD; however, RAD, as a psychiatric diagnosis, extends well beyond the narrow scope of the infant–parent relationship and reflects broad social abnormalities across multiple contexts (Richters & Volkmar, 1994). In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition, text revision (DSM IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000), the predominant feature of the disorder is the presence of “markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate social relatedness in most contexts beginning before age 5 years and is associated with grossly pathogenic care” (p. 127). Grossly pathogenic care is characterized by (a) a persistent disregard for the child’s emotional needs for comfort, stimulation, and affection; (b) persistent disregard for the child’s physical needs; and (c) repeated changes of primary caregivers (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). o Similar to the behaviors associated with maltreatment, the descriptive symptomatology of RAD suggests maladaptive patterns of interpersonal functioning and emotional regulation that have far-reaching consequences for children (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). For example, it has been found that children with RAD demonstrate maladaptive attachment behaviours specifically with regard to how they cope with exploration, fear, and wariness (O’Connor, Brendenkamp, & Rutter, 1999). Aber, Allen, Carlson, and Cicchetti (1989) found that children who are RAD maltreated exhibit poor self-esteem and self-regulation, poor peer relations, and developmental and cognitive delays. They concluded that the predominant issue for these maltreated children was a concern for security and reflected an expectation of adults as unresponsive, unavailable, and rejecting (Aber et al., 1989). Children diagnosed with RAD have similar difficulties selfregulating and modulating their emotions and behaviors (Kay-Hall & Geher, 2003). o Children with attachment disorders such as RAD struggle to form typical, reciprocal relationships with others (Reber, 1996). According to a number of researchers, o children with RAD display a wide variety of maladaptive behaviors including property destruction, aggression, hoarding food, stealing, lying, bullying, and cruelty to animals and people (Parker & Forrest, 1993; Reber, 1996). Additionally, children diagnosed with RAD may seek out strangers at inappropriate times, and experience difficulties both giving and receiving affection (Reber, 1996). Kirschner (1992) found these children tend to seek out other children with similar emotional and behavioral disturbances. Other behaviors reportedly seen in children diagnosed with RAD include poor impulse control, poor self-regulation, hyperactivity, low frustration tolerance, and seeking out and associating with other children with behavioral problems and superficiality (Kirschner, 1992; Reber, 1996). Kay-Hall and Geher (2003) found that children diagnosed with RAD evidenced significantly more violent behaviors, were less empathic, and had more personality difficulties than did children who were not diagnosed with RAD. The problems typically seen in children with RAD are typically intense and pervasive; children with problems related to attachment struggle to find joy, mutuality, and reciprocity in their interactions with others (Hughes, 1997). These children have not experienced the necessary sense of reciprocity and security of a responsive, attuned parent. They have grown up in a world that is often chaotic, neglectful, and frightening. These experiences are reflected in their inability to regulate and modulate their emotions and behaviors, often appearing out of control. HISTORY OF RAD o ZEANAH & SMYKE(2008) – The clinical perspective on attachment derives from descriptive studies of young children raised in extreme caregiving environments, such as those who have been maltreated or those who have been reared in institutional settings (Goldfarb, 1945; Main & George, 1979; Spitz, 1945; Wolkind, 1974). Drawing upon these studies, Tizard and Rees (1975) reported that at age 4 years, a majority of young children (18/26) who had been raised in residential nurseries in the United Kingdom since birth exhibited aberrant attachment behavior. A group of 8 children were described as emotionally withdrawn and unresponsive, and another group of 10 children were indiscriminate, attention seeking, and socially superficial. These two groups became the basis for RAD, which was first described in the DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980) and later revised in subsequent editions. The criteria for RAD in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and the ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1992) are notably similar, although DSM-IV criteria include inhibited/emotionally withdrawn and disinhibited/indiscriminately social subtypes whereas ICD-10 criteria define the inhibited type as Reactive Attachment Disorder and the disinhibited type as Disinhibited Attachment Disorder. o In addition to the disturbed social behaviors that form the core of contemporary descriptions of attachment disorders, both the DSM and the ICD specify that the etiology of the disorder is extremely poor caregiving. The DSM-IV criteria require “pathogenic care,” and the ICD-10 description cautions against making the diagnosis in the absence of maltreatment. Both also noted that the signs and symptoms of the disorder are not due to developmental delay or to pervasive developmental disorder. DSM Definition HARDY (2007) In them Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition-Text Revision (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), the only pathology that is officially related to attachment is reactive attachment disorder of infancy or early childhood (RAD) (see Table 1). The diagnostic criteria for this disorder include: a pattern of disturbed and developmentally inappropriate social relationships prior to age five, a history of pathogenic care that predates the presentation of the disturbances, and the assumption that the disturbances are not better accounted for by other diagnoses (p. 130). There are two subtypes of RAD, inhibited type and disinhibited type. The DSM-IV-TR (2000) criteria do not include any descriptors of associated behaviors, and there are no validated instruments for assessing or diagnosing RAD (Minde, 2003). A wide variety of behaviors have been attributed to RAD, but there is limited empirical research in this area (Hanson & Spratt, 2000; Minde). While diagnosis and treatment of disordered attachment patterns are vitally important, limiting the conception of attachmentrelated psychopathology to the narrow diagnostic criteria of RAD significantly understates the importance of other attachment-related conditions and may limit the use of potentially effective interventions. Identifying the links between these disorders will guide interventions to impact the attachment problems by targeting underlying issues rather than focusing on just the symptoms. Diagnostic Criteria Schwartz (2006) – In differentiating RAD from other developmental disorders, the DSM IV-TR emphasizes that the diagnosis is predicated upon an etiology of documented grossly pathogenic care, and may include the experience of frequent and repeated changes in caregivers. For RAD to develop, grossly pathogenic care also must include limited opportunities to develop selective attachments. Examples include children brought up in institutions with multiple caregivers or children who have a history of being reared in atypical environments characterized by extreme neglect and abuse. Children raised in these types of environments have been shown to manifest abnormal social behaviors such as lack of responsiveness, excessive inhibition, hypervigilance, indiscriminate sociability, or pervasively disorganized attachment behaviors (O’Connor & Rutter, 2000). These symptoms suggest a growing link between children with major attachment disruptions and the presence of a disorganized attachment status. Children classified with a disorganized attachment or diagnosed with RAD are seen as controlling and punishing, and engage in role-reversal behavioural patterns when reunited with the parent (Hughes, 1997; Main & Cassidy, 1988; Solomon & George, 1996). Research also has shown that the disorganized attachment pattern is linked to aggressive externalizing behavior (Lyons-Ruth, Repacholi, McLeod, & Silva, 1991; Shaw, Owens, Vondra, & Keenan, 1996). Hanson and Spratt (2000) suggested that “the purpose of the RAD diagnosis is to provide a clinical description that includes the problems in the child’s ability to relate to people and the context in which this behavior develops” (p. 138). SCHWARTZ (2006) - children diagnosed with RAD must have a documented history of parenting characterized by grossly pathogenic care. Hardy (2007) - Since the diagnostic criteria for RAD are relatively nonspecific, the diagnosis may be given to children who come from a range of different backgrounds. Research has focused on children who have been institutionalized as well as on children who have had a primary caregiver but developed a disordered attachment due to maltreatment. In another study, Zeanah et al. (2004) found evidence of disordered attachment in maltreated toddlers who were removed from their primary caregiver and placed in foster care. Marvin and Whelan (2003) further emphasize some of the difficulties associated with the RAD diagnosis and with the assessment of attachment disturbances. Because attachment has typically been studied by applying Ainsworth’s original model of qualitative differences between attachment typology, focusing on the RAD criteria and associated behaviors presents a difficult transition. The RAD diagnosis does not fit neatly into the categorization of the four attachment styles that have been identified by Ainsworth and Main. Core Symptoms HARDY (2007) - Haugaard and Hazan (2004) describe the inhibited type of RAD as a pattern resulting from experience with caregivers who do not provide emotional support and comfort when needed. Because children who have been treated in this way expect to be rejected by others, they avoid social contact. Behaviors that are typically associated with this pattern include withdrawal from others, avoidance of comforting gestures, self-soothing behaviors, vigilance, aggression, and awkwardness in social situations. In the disinhibited type, behaviors are believed to be related to experience with caregivers who are not responsive but can be coerced into providing affection. This results in behaviours such as inappropriate familiarity and comfort-seeking with strangers, exaggeration of needs for assistance, chronic anxious appearance, and inappropriate childishness (p. 158). Subtypes o ZEANAH & SMYKE (2008) – EXCELLENT TABLE ON PAGE 223 Schwartz (2006) RAD is divided into two subtypes: the Inhibited subtype and the Disinhibited subtype. The Inhibited subtype refers to children who persistently and pervasively fail to initiate and to respond to social interactions in a developmentally acceptable way. The Disinhibited subtype describes children who are indiscriminately sociable or demonstrate lack of selectivity in their attachments. o Disinhibited ZEANAH & SMYKE (2008) - Indiscriminate/Disinhibited Pattern of RAD The essence of this pattern of RAD is the failure to exhibit developmentally expectable reticence around unfamiliar adults. This ismanifest by the child’s lack of reticence about engaging socially with them, failure of the child to check back with the caregiver in unfamiliar settings and instead tending to wander off, and the child’s willingness to approach, interact with, and “go off” with a stranger. Developmentally, stranger wariness appears early in the second half of the first year of life. Though individual differences are evident, some degree of stranger wariness is evident in all typically developing children. In the indiscriminate/disinhibited pattern, wariness around strangers is absent or substantially diminished. o Inhibited ZEANAH & SMYKE (2008) - Emotionally Withdrawn/Inhibited RAD The essence of this pattern of RAD is a young child who exhibits minimal or no discriminated attachment behavior, even at times when the child’s attachment behaviors should be activated. Phenomenologically, it is characterized by the absence of organized attachment behaviors, impaired social engagement and reciprocity, emotion regulation difficulties (i.e., low levels of positive affect, outbursts of irritability, unexplained fear and hypervigilance). Children with this pattern do not seek comfort consistently or at all even when distressed and are not easily soothed when they do become distressed. Individual differences in soothability are rightfully considered a temperamental disposition, but what distinguishes RAD is the degree and pervasiveness of the child’s unresponsiveness in the context of minimal or no attachment behaviors. **** THIS ARTICLE ALSO PROVIDES A GREAT DISTINCTION BETWEEN RAD AND PDD ON PAGE 223. Theoretical Conceptualization o HARDY (2007) - There are two major theoretical perspectives that inform our understanding of the process of attachment. According to the developmental psychology perspective, the early relationship with the attachment object causes an infant to form internal working models for relationships that will influence interpersonal relationships throughout life. These working models consist of representational structures that define one’s perception of self and others and contribute to the internal processes that define one’s selective experience of the external world (Pietromonaco & Barrett, 2000). The psychoanalytic perspective maintains that self and object representations form as a result of early childhood experiences and that these representations will influence all future affective exchanges. Similarly, these processes function from the unconscious mind (Schore, 2002). Although there are some differences in the construction and application of the developmental psychology and psychoanalytic perspectives, they seem to differ primarily in semantics. o Neuroanalytic Perspective explained on page 2 of the HARDY article ****** Major Issues in Conceptualizing and Identifying RAD o ZEANAH & SMYKE (2008) - Studies of Children in Institutions Signs of RAD have been readily identified among young children living in institutions. Smyke, Dumitrescu, and Zeanah (2002) reported signs of both emotionally withdrawn/inhibited and indiscriminately social/disinhibited attachment in a substantial number of young children being raised in a large institution for young children in Bucharest, Romania. In another sample, Zeanah et al. (2005b) similarly found signs of both types of RAD among young children living in institutions. o Quality of Caregiving and RAD Given that pathogenic care is required as a diagnostic criterion for RAD, it is important to know if the care-giving environment is related to signs of the disorder. It is clear that RAD has been reported only in children with histories of either maltreatment or institutional rearing, but this may be because the pathogenic care criterion is required. Furthermore, the pathogenic care criterion is particularly important in distinguishing indiscriminate/disinhibited RAD from conditions such as Williams syndrome and fetal alcohol syndrome, both of which have been reported to be associated with indiscriminate social behavior (Jacobson &Jacobson, 2003; Jones et al., 2000). o MEASUREMENT ISSUES As with most psychiatric disorders, RAD is most often measured by caregiver report, but there has been some attention to convergent validity through behavioral observations. Boris et al. (2004), for example, used a combination of a structured interview and behavioral assessment to determine if young children met criteria for RAD. This study used a behavioral assessment of attachment behavior designed for use in clinical settings. o HARDY (2007) speaks about assessing RAD and the challenges inherent to it – page 31, 32, and top of 33. Treatment Implications o HARDY (2007) - Treatment of Attachment Disturbances. Since attachment disturbances have been linked to a variety of different outcomes in children and adults, using attachment theory to inform psychiatric treatment in general is warranted. In children who show significantly maladapted attachment patterns and/or those who meet the criteria for RAD, more directed interventions may be required. Empirical evidence in this area is limited. Furthermore, interventions may differ for children who have been adopted into a stable home, those who are in foster care, those who still reside in institutions, those who reside with their original attachment figure, those who developed attachment disturbances at varying ages, etc. The connection between attachment insecurity and other non-attachmentspecific disorders is also far from clear, and the existence of co-occuring disorders is likely to increase the complexity required for effective treatment. Thus far, treatment of disordered attachment and related behaviors tends to be focused in several areas: enhancing current attachment relationships, creating new attachment o o relationships, and reducing problematic symptoms and behaviors. Due to the current propensity towards diagnosis-focused research, most of the related recommendations are focused on children who meet criteria for RAD. Hanson and Spratt (2000) identify treatment strategies based on interventions that have been effective in populations of children who have been abused. Their rationale is that RAD is more common in children who have been maltreated and that it involves similar symptom profiles. Their preferred interventions include cognitive behavioral management of mood symptoms, behavioral modification, and psychoeducation. Since RAD is characterized by impaired social relationships, the addition of social support and coaching may enhance peer relationships (Haugaard & Hazan, 2004; Minde, 2003). Interventions designed to enhance self-esteem and increase self-efficacy could also improve functioning (Haugaard & Hazan Children who have a healthy and supportive relationship with an adult (not necessarily a caregiver) are less likely to be negatively affected by insecure attachments. In addition, caregiver involvement in the treatment process, providing the caregiver is psychologically healthy enough to participate appropriately, is believed to be an important contributor to positive treatment outcomes. Treatment of caregivers’ own attachment difficulties and current psychopathology may also be indicated (Hanson & Spratt; Haugaard & Hazan). Other psychotherapeutic approaches to the treatment of RAD include encouraging the development of a healthy attachment relationship to the therapist and processing traumatic events through play therapy (Haugaard & Hazan, 2004). Interventions designed to enhance caregivers’ ability to understand the meaning behind the behaviors of children with RAD are also believed to be an important part of treatment in children with severely disturbed attachment-related behavior (Minde, 2003; Marvin & Whelan, 2003). This approach goes beyond traditional psychoeducational or supportive therapies in that it addresses children whose symptoms have not been reduced by the formation of a healthy attachment. Particularly for children who have been adopted or are in foster care, caregivers may already have the knowledge and sensitivity required to form healthy attachments, but the children’s behaviours may be extremely complex and contradictory. Marvin and Whelan state that the interpretation of these behaviors is difficult even for clinicians. They suggest that if caregivers are taught to interpret and address the meaning behind the behaviors, they may be better able to assist the children in developing more adaptive relational patterns. Schwatz (2006) - Specific therapeutic interventions for children diagnosed with RAD have not been well validated in peer-reviewed journals; however, if RAD is linked to maltreatment (by fulfilling the criteria for grossly pathogenic care), then interventions that have been used with children who have been maltreated may offer guidelines for interventions with children with RAD (Hanson & Spratt, 2000). Perry (2001) outlined the following interventions for maltreated children: (a) Nurture the child; (b) understand behaviors before punishing; (c) interact with these children based on emotional age; (d) be consistent, predictable, and repetitive; (e) model and teach appropriate social behaviors; (f ) listen, talk, and play with these children; (g) maintain realistic expectations; (h) be patient with the child and yourself; (i) take care of yourself; and ( j) use other resources. Considering the four basic goals of attachment, (proximity, security, safety, and self-regulation), it is important to provide children with a sense of stability. Environmental and relational stability provides children with an opportunity to feel safe and secure, and must include caregivers who are sensitive to the child’s need for proximity while maintaining awareness that closeness in the past has meant danger. The school psychologist, knowing the crucial role of stability, can take a prominent role in guiding and directing teachers and other school staff in ways to enhance the child’s sense of security. Teachers and school personnel, such as counselors and school psychologists, must help children learn how to regulate and modulate their affect and behavior, and have the unique opportunity to act as a secure base for the child (Kobak et al., 2001). In attempting to aid children who are unable to effectively regulate emotions and behaviors, it is critical for those who intervene to be cognizant of their own capacity to modulate and regulate their own emotions and behaviors. o Schwartz (2006) - Lieberman and Zeanah (1999) summarized the basic ideas behind interventions for children with attachment disorders. They pointed to the need for interventions to be child specific, to be developmentally appropriate, and to minimize the use of negative practices such as scolding, embarrassment, and condemning. Interventions which rely on emotional pressure invariably lead to empathic failures which reinforce the child’s existing impaired internal working models and increase the likelihood of escalating behaviors. Limitations o Zeanah & Smyle(2008) – bottom of pg 221 and top of 222. o HARDY (2007) - There are two subtypes of RAD, inhibited type and disinhibited type. The DSM-IV-TR (2000) criteria do not include any descriptors of associated behaviors, and there are no validated instruments for assessing or diagnosing RAD (Minde, 2003). A wide variety of behaviors have been attributed to RAD, but there is limited empirical research in this area (Hanson & Spratt, 2000; Minde). While diagnosis and treatment of disordered attachment patterns are vitally important, limiting the conception of attachment related psychopathology to the narrow diagnostic criteria of RAD significantly understates the importance of other attachment-related conditions and may limit the use of potentially effective interventions. Identifying the links between these disorders will guide interventions to impact the attachment problems by targeting underlying issues rather than focusing on just the symptoms. Pg 31 challenge to the current disinhibited pattern. *** Numerous limitation and cautions discussed in the HARDY (2007) ARTICLE. Conclusion Q&A REFERENCES