The temporal trajectory of age-related decreases in executive function Doreen Nessler¹, Ray Johnson, Jr.², Michael Bersick¹ and David Friedman¹ Task-switching studies indicate an age-related decrease in working memory (or episodic memory [1]): Older adults’ reaction times (RTs) show a larger increase when maintaining two task sets (in a mixed block) vs. one (mixing costs). In contrast, age has a smaller impact on task-set reconfiguration: The RT difference in mixed blocks between task-set repetitions and switches to an alternative task (switch costs) is age-invariant. However, these results vary with the following task characteristics: switch frequency [2], informative cue occurrence [3] and predictability [4]. Here, we clarify the link between these characteristics and agerelated changes, and examine whether some aspects of cognitive control, such as working memory and task-set reconfiguration, are compromised at an earlier age than others. Methods Participants: 15 Young (age 18 to 30); 15 Young-old (age 60 to 68); 15 Old-old (age 72 to 87) Design: Digit (not 5) required response: more/less than 5? or odd/even? pure blocks: one task; mixed blocks: two tasks. Factors: (1) Ratio of switch (S) to stay or repeat (R) trials equiprobable; switch after 0, 1, or 2 trials: a-bb-aaa rare switch 1:3; switch after 2, 3, or 4 trials: aaa-bbbb-aaaaa (2) Cue-status: informative, uninformative; (3) Predictability Status: predictable, unpredictable Mixing costs = pre-switch RTs in mixed blocks - RTs in pure blocks Switch costs = switch RTs – pre-switch RTs mixed blocks Discussion 1 Target onset 300 800 1300 0 Cue onset Baseline for target-related ERPs Informative Cue 400 300 200 100 0 2a Young Young-old 600 Old-old 400 300 200 Switch costs 100 400 300 200 100 0 0 2b Young-old Informative Cue Old-old 3a Young 400 300 200 100 0 3b Young Young-old Old-old 1000 ms Behavioral and ERP results indicate that the ability to maintain two task sets in memory declines early in the aging process (young-old), independent of task characteristics. However, the ability to use working memory for advanced task preparation when the upcoming task is predictable remains intact until later in the aging process. Task-set reconfiguration seems to be intact when task demands are low, but compromised for rare, uninformed switches for both groups of older adults. The age-related decrease in the ability to reconfigure the task set could be due to difficulties in 1) detecting the increased conflict and/or 2) upregulating and increasing cognitive control. Fz 7 Old-old Cuerelated P3 Cz 4b Old-old target P3 Cz Pure task Pre-switch Switch Cz Uninformative Cue Cue -1000 Cz Cz Target 1000 ms Cz Fig. 4a, b: The age-related increase in mixing cost (Fig. 2a, b) was associated with a larger decrease in target P3 amplitude, most likely reflecting task-set configuration processes that were not prepared before target onset. (a) Despite age-invariant P3 amplitudes to the cue, only older adults show a decrease in target P3 for pre-switch and switch trials compared to pure trials. (b) Only the young show a smaller decrease in the target P3 for pre-switch than switch trials. Uninformative Cue Fig. 3a, b: Similarly, for young-old and old-old adults, differences in switch costs compared to the young were only present for rare switches after uninformative cues (see arrows), which is when task demands were highest. Fz Young-old Young-old 4a Fig. 2b: Young and youngold, but not old-old, adults used predictability to decrease mixing costs in the equiprobable, uninformative switch condition (see arrows). 500 Young Young Fig. 2a,b: For all manipulations, young-old and old-old adults similarly showed larger mixing costs than the young. Uninformative Cue Young 500 Informative Cue Equiprobable, predictable Equiprobable, unpredictable Rare switch, predictable Rare switch, unpredictable EEG recording: 62 sintered Ag/AgCl electrodes; ref.: averaged mastoids; continuous DC100Hz; 500 Hz sampling rate; Fig. 1: ERP epochs. Baseline for cuerelated ERPs Mixing costs References Introduction ¹ Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory, NY Psychiatric Institute, NY, NY; ² Department of Psychology, Queens College/ CUNY, Flushing, NY Fz baseline -200 200 400 ms Response MFN Equiprobable, uninformative cue Equiprobable, informative cue Rare switch, uninformative cue Rare switch, informative cue Young-old Old-old Fig. 7: Response conflict for older adults, as indicated by MFN amplitude, was high regardless of task demands. The older adults’ impairment in detecting increased conflict induced by rare, uninformed switches may be due to their having a limit on the amount of conflict that they can detect (Fig. 3b). [1] Mayr & Kliegl, 2000, JEP: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1124-1140. [2] Friedman, Nessler, Johnson, Ritter & Bersick, in revision. [3] Kray, Li & Lindenberger , 2002, Brain and Cognition, 49, 363-381. [4] Van Asselen & Ridderinkhof, 2000, Psychologica Belgica, 40, 259–273. Fig. 5: For equiprobable, uninformed items, the decrease in target P3 was smaller for predictable than for unpredictable trials, most likely reflecting advanced preparation (Fig. 2b), for young and young-old, but not old-old adults. Young FCz 6 Young Cz Cz 0 5 0 Old-old Cz Pure task Predictable trials Unpredictable trials 500 1000 ms (collapsed over pre-switch, switch, post-switch) Young-old FCz Young-old Old-old FCz 500 1000 ms Pure task Equiprobable, uninformative cue Rare switch, uninformative cue Fig. 6: The agerelated increase in switch costs for rare uninformed switches (Fig. 3b) was associated with the lack of a significant decrease in target P3.