Sensation and perception overview The senses (sensory modalities) Vision Audition (hearing) Olfaction (smell) Gustation (taste) Touch (pressure, temperature [warmth and cold], pain) Proprioception Vestibular sense (body’s position in space) Kinesthesia (movement and position of parts of the body in relation to one another) Sensation and Perception Sensation and perception are two stages in the process whereby we construct our internal representation of the environment. Our goal as biological organisms is to respond to, interact with, and adapt to our environments. Our goal is to extract meaning from the objects that exist and events that occur around us. Sensation: physical energy is converted into neural signals Perception: signals are selected, organized, and interpreted Sensation to attention to perception Transduction: sensory receptors translate physical stimulation (light, sound waves, heat, molecules) into neural signals 2) Neural pathways transmit this information to the brain 3) Rudimentary neural/mental codes, representing features in the environment, are constructed in the brain/mind 4) Features (properties and parts) are combined (bound) to form objects (wholes); may require attention 1) In the case of object recognition, a feature is any property of an object, such as it’s shape (as determined by it’s edges or boundaries), color, size, and movement a) i. b) The letter A has three features: /, \, and – In the case of categorization, some features of the bird category are feathers, beaks, lays eggs, builds nests, etc. Illusory conjunctions Sensation to attention to perception cont. 5) Objects are matched with prior knowledge and pattern categorization and recognition occurs 6) Some objects are selected for further processing (another job for attention) 7) Names of objects are accessed 8) Meanings of objects are accessed 9) Objects are organized into scenes 10) The scene emerges into subjective awareness (i. e., consciousness); total time = 100 ms Sensation to attention to perception cont. Low-level vs. high level processes Sensory processes are also known as low-level or peripheral processes Perceptual processes are also known as high-level or central processes As information processing progresses, the representations/codes become more abstract and less modality-specific. For example, if the goal of sensation and perception is to extract meaning from our world, does it really matter whether you learned something by reading it (a visual stimulus) or by hearing it (an auditory stimulus)? The information processing progression described in steps 1-10 is mostly a bottom-up processing view of sensation and perception In bottom-up processing, the conscious percept is determined primarily by the information received via sensory receptors One can also identify top-down processing influences on sensation and perception In top-down processing, memory and general knowledge take part in determining the conscious percept See next slide Sensation to attention to perception cont. To function effectively, our minds need both quantitative information and qualitative information Quantitative information The intensity of a stimulus is coded in the brain by the frequency with which neurons fire – the higher the intensity, the greater the frequency of firing For example, if someone lightly touches your back, the frequency of firing would be relatively low Qualitative information The difference between two different stimuli is coded in the brain by different patterns of neurons firing Coarse coding: a single neuron fires when your back is touched anywhere in a relatively large patch of skin Then how do you know precisely where you’re being touched? Because each particular spot is defined in terms of a unique set of several overlapping patches Thus, a touch on any particular spot is coded as a unique set (or pattern) of neurons firing The cycle of perception: Sensation and perception are active processes. Psychophysics Psychophysics Psychophysics The study of the correspondence between physical reality and psychological reality There is no one-to-one correspondence between physical reality and psychological reality. 3 examples There are physical energies that exist in our environment that fall outside the normal range of human sensation, but that other animals can detect (i.e., represent psychologically) Some bees can detect ultraviolet radiation Some snakes can detect infrared radiation Elephants can detect very low frequency sounds Dogs can detect very high frequency sounds Absolute thresholds Difference thresholds Psychophysics cont. Within the normal range of human sensation, sometimes the intensity of a stimulus is so faint that we do not report that we perceive it, even though the physical energy from that stimulus (e.g., light waves bouncing off an object) is striking our sensory apparatus (e.g., our retina). Absolute Threshold The smallest amount of stimulation that can be accurately detected 50% of the time is the absolute threshold. “Limen” is the German word for theshold. Subliminal means “below the limen”. Subliminal perception: Does stimulation that falls below the absolute threshold influence behavior? Psychophysics cont. Within the normal range of human sensation, sometimes a change in the intensity of a stimulus occurs, but we do not report that the stimulus has changed. Difference Thresholds The lowest level of stimulation required to sense a change in stimulation Weber’s Law In order to perceive a change in stimulation, that change must be greater as the magnitude of the stimulation becomes greater The ratio of the change in stimulation (delta I) to the magnitude of the stimulation (I) is a constant (k) delta I/I = k For judging the weight of objects, k = 1/50 Thus, if an object weighs 50 grams, an increase of 1 gram will be noticeable. However, if an object weighs 100 grams, the weight will need to increase by 2 grams in order for you to report that you’ve noticed a difference. Psychophysics cont. Signal-Detection Theory Reporting that a stimulus is present is jointly determined by the signal and the subject’s response criterion. Separates subject’s sensitivity from subject’s propensity to guess. Hits – false alarms. Yea sayer Yes No On 89% 11% Off 41% 59% Nay sayer Stimulus Response Yes No On Hit Miss Off False alarm Correct rejection On Yes 65% No 35% Off 8% 92% Sensory modalities Taste (Gustation) Photograph of tongue surface (top), magnified 75 times 500 to 10,000 taste buds line the tongue and mouth Taste receptors (~ 50) are down inside each “bud” Children have more taste buds than adults Older adults have less intense taste sensations than younger adults Taste preferences begin in the womb Supertasters have more taste buds, but are often more picky eaters Smell (Olfaction) Touch (Haptic sense) Skin is the largest organ in the body (18 sq ft) Pressure, temperature, and pain Different receptors in the skin transduce different types of physical information that, in the end, is routed to the somatosensory cortex (by way of the thalamus) Sensitivity to Touch The Heat Grill The brain interprets simultaneous firing of both receptor types in interesting ways. When a person grasps two braided water pipes – one with cold water running through it and one with warm water – the sensation is “very hot” or “wet”. Pain Pain in the skin When cells are damaged, they release substance P, that stimulates free nerve endings Two types of pain sense neuronal axons Myelinated L-fibers transmit all types of information very quickly to the spinal cord Adaptive because this type of pain causes us to recoil quickly Unmyelinated S-fibers transmit pain information relatively slowly to the spinal cord Adaptive because this type of pain forces us to allow injured tissue to recuperate Gate control theory L-fibers inhibit neural transmission in S-fibers This is why rubbing the skin near an injury can lessen the pain. Hearing (Audition) Sound Sound waves are vibrations of molecules in the air Frequency (perceived as pitch) What is white noise? Amplitude (determines loudness) Measured in decibels (dB), which is a logarithmic scale An increase of 10 dB corresponds to a change in sound power of 10 times the comparison stimulus Complexity (determines timbre or tonal quality) Determines the difference between middle C played by a trumpet vs. a flute The Human Ear Auditory transduction When sound waves hit the fluid of the inner ear, the fluid causes the basilar membrane to move up and down, activating electrical potentials in the hair cells (the receptor cells for hearing). Temporal coding The entire basilar membrane vibrates slowly for low frequencies and more rapidly for high frequencies Place theory Auditory Localization Sounds from different directions are not identical as they arrive at left and right ears Loudness Timing Phase The brain calculates a sound’s location by using these differences Visual sensation The Electromagnetic Spectrum Structures of the Human Eye The Retina Receptive Fields I -- a place or spot in your visual environment for which an individual neuron is “responsible” -- allows for the detection of edges Receptive Fields II -- the cell fires more when light illuminates the center and less when the center is dark -- the cell fires more when the periphery is dark and less when the periphery is illuminated Hermann Grid Color Vision Trichromatic theory: T. Young (1802) & H. von Helmholtz (1852) both proposed that the eye detects 3 primary colors: red, blue, & green There are three types of cones, each of which is most maximally sensitive to a different wavelength All other colors can be derived by combining these three Color Vision cont. Opponent process theory Color opponent cells in the LGN are activated by one color and inhibited by the other: blackwhite, blue-yellow, red-green Color “blindness” Afterimages Proprioceptive senses Vestibular sense: position of the body in space Vestibular sacs and semicircular canals are located in the inner ear, above the cochlea Vestibular sacs sense gravity and the position of the head in space Semicircular canals sense acceleration as the head moves Kinesthesia: movement and position of muscles and limbs relative to each other Receptors located in joints, tendons, and muscles. Extrasensory Perception Extrasensory Perception (ESP): The ability to perceive something without ordinary sensory information Three types of ESP: Telepathy – Mind-to-mind communication Clairvoyance – Perception of remote events Precognition – Ability to see future events 57% of Americans believe in ESP CBS News (2002) But ESP has not been scientifically demonstrated Parapsychology J. B. Rhine conducted many experiments on ESP using stimuli such as these Guess the symbol on the card In a 25-card deck, with 5 different cards, participants correctly identified 7.1 (5 correct guesses would be expected by chance) Rhine believed that his evidence supported the existence of ESP, but his findings were flawed Experimental procedures were faulty Cards had been handled by the participants and were marked in various subtle ways Experimental findings could not be replicated Perception The topographic nature of primary perceptual processing Neurons in the primary sensory areas of the cerebral cortex have topographic organizational structures. Neurons in the primary auditory cortex have a tonotopic organization. Different parts of the primary auditory cortex are differentially sensitive to different frequencies, just like the basilar membrane. Neurons in the somatosensory cortex have a somatotopic organization. Connected areas of the body are represented next to each other in the cortex (see next slide). Somatopic organization in cortex Visual Perception The organization of features into objects and scenes, the location of those objects in space, and the interpretation (beginning with classification) of those objects. Retinotopic maps in the brain Macaque monkeys were injected with a glucoselinked radioactive substance After viewing an image, the monkeys were sacrificed and their striate cortex was flat mounted Then the radiation was measured Those cells firing the most will use the most glucose and will show the most radiation Hierarchical processing of features Simple cells: respond to lines of a particular orientation Complex cells: may respond to lines that terminate or to corners; may respond to lines anywhere in their receptive field Hypercomplex cells: respond to a combination of features Visual Pathways Form perception (or perceptual organization) We must be able to segregate the stimulation the visual system receives into separate objects. “Parsing” our visual world. Gestalt maxim The whole is greater than the sum of it’s parts. Figure-ground People inherently distinguish between figure (the object they are viewing) and ground (background) Ambiguous or reversible figures Ambiguous (reversible) figures Ambiguous (reversible) figures Ambiguous (reversible) figures Ambiguous (reversible) figures Similarity Objects that are of a similar color, size, or shape are usually perceived as part of a pattern Proximity When objects are close to one another, we tend to perceive them together rather than separately Good continuation Items that continue a pattern or direction tend to be grouped together as part of a pattern Closure We are inclined to overlook incompleteness in sensory information and to perceive a whole object even where none really exists. Subjective Contour Geon Theory Geons (geometric icons) are simple 3D component shapes An “alphabet” of 36 are stored in memory Geons are combined to identify essential contours of objects Face perception In human beings, facial expression and the perception of faces are an important mode of communication A specific part of the brain is dedicated to face perception The fusiform gyrus in the right hemisphere Located on the ventral portion of the right temporal lobe Damage to the fusiform gyrus results in prosopagnosia – an inability to recognize faces Thatcher illusion Same race effect in face recognition Face memory is better for faces from one’s own race And this is more true of whites, than blacks. There is also more activation in the fusiform gyrus when perceiving a same race face than when perceiving a different race face. Depth Perception How far away is a particular object? Binocular cues Information integrated from both eyes Monocular cues Information from a single eye Artists working in two-dimensional media rely on monocular depth cues to represent a 3-D world Binocular Cues Oculomotor cues Our eyes converge (turn inward towards the nose) to focus on objects that are closer and diverge to focus on objects that are farther away Receptors in the oculomotor muscles send this information to the brain Retinal disparity The difference between images captured in the two retinas This difference diminishes as depth increases Binocular cells in visual cortex are retinal disparity detectors, firing to specific retinal disparities Monocular Cues Relative size When looking at two objects known to be of similar size, the object that looks smaller (because it projects a smaller image on the retina) will be perceived as farther away Texture gradient At close distances, patterns appear coarse and less closely packed, while at farther distances they appear finer and more densely packed Linear perspective Parallel lines converge at a distance Monocular Cues cont. Interposition When one object blocks our view of another object, the obstructed object is perceived as more distant Elevation As objects in our plane of vision that are below the horizon get closer to the horizon (and, if above the horizon, they get farther from the horizon), they are perceived as more distant Motion parallax When a person is moving, nearby objects speed by whereas far objects move by more slowly Perceptual Constancy The perception of objects stays constant despite changes in sensory stimulation. Size constancy Even though two objects of the same size but at different depths (distances) create differently sized retinal images, we still perceive them as the same size. Color constancy The color of an object remains constant despite changes in illumination. Shape constancy Despite the fact that the same object produces a different retinal image nearly every time we encounter it, we still recognize it as that object. Size perception (and size constancy) depends on depth perception Ames Box (Ames Room) When one child stands in a near corner and another (of similar height) stands in a far corner, the room creates the illusion that they are both equidistant from the viewer Therefore perceptual processing does not adjust for distance and we interpret the small retinal image as coming from a smaller person and the larger retinal image as coming from a large person. The Ponzo Illusion The lines project the same size retinal image because they are the same size. However, linear perspective cues tell us that the top line is farther away. However, in order for something farther away to project the same size retinal image, it must be larger. So, we perceive the top line as larger. The moon illusion The moon produces retinal images of the same size no matter where it is in the sky However, we perceive it as closer when it is not on the horizon In order for it to produce the same size retinal image and be closer, it must be smaller So, we perceive it as smaller. The Muller-Lyer Illusion Again, the lines project the same size retinal image because they are the same size. However, linear perspective cues tell us that the righthand line is farther away. Therefore, in order for something that is farther away to project the same-size retinal image, it must be larger! So we perceive it as larger. Shape Constancy Even though these images cast shadows of different shapes, we still see the quarter as round Two systems for motion perception