Title: Where have they all gone?: classroom attention patterns after Acquired Brain Injury Author: Siân A. Rees Affiliation: The SHIPS Project, Contact details: 43/45 Stanley Road Warmley Bristol BS15 4NX Email: sian@shipsproject.org.uk Abstract Certain groups of pupils who have sustained an Acquired Brain Injury (ABI) have a different pattern of attention within the classroom which interferes with learning and social interactions. The delineation of these groups is suggested. By looking in detail at the classroom behaviour of eight pupils, a common account for classroom behaviour post-ABI is questioned, and an alternative explanation put forward. Practical ways to promote learning for this group are explored. Key Words: attention, concentration, acquired brain injury, classroom interaction, instruction Introduction A growing number of pupils are returning to our mainstream classrooms having sustained an Acquired Brain Injury (ABI) (Sharples et al 1990, Royal College of Surgeons 1999, Hawley et al 2002, Hawley et al 2003, Petit-Zeman 2001). In the classroom young people are required to give attention to tasks and teachers in different ways. They may be asked to switch attention from one task to another quickly, divide attention between concurrent tasks, focus attention on particular aspects of a task and sustain attention to complete a task (Walker & Wicks 2005). Most importantly, pupils are expected to move into and out of the different kinds of attention rapidly and efficiently, as the lesson demands. For those who have sustained an ABI this may be difficult (paper in preparation). Sustaining a brain injury is a major event in any life, but if it occurs while the brain is developing, recovery becomes intertwined with the normal process of development can take many years and may not be complete (Hawley et al. 2004). A rapid physical recovery can mask underlying cognitive difficulties (Lord-Maes & Obrzut 1996, Johnson 1992). Older 1 children with ABI may act like younger ones (Dennis et al. 1996, 1998) and further development of the brain after injury may not proceed as expected (Johnson 1992). ABI and attention Attention is the process of admitting and holding information in consciousness (Ylvisaker & Szekeres 1998), but the term tends to have a wider definition in the educational world. Concentration is the selecting and maintaining aspects of attention. It is affected by motivation and deliberate control of attention is necessary under less than ideal conditions, such as a classroom. In a classroom, and life in general, it is also necessary to divide attention between two or more tasks while focussed attention refers to the ability to ignore distraction while focussing on an activity. However in the classroom it will always be necessary to be aware of distractions and to judge whether they are important or not; a teacher providing additional information about a talk is important, while a peer wanting to talk about social matters is more likely not to be important for the task. It is suggested that speed of processing is a limiting function of attentional capacity and poor working memory could also be an impairment of attentional control (Mateer et al. 1996), leading to impulsivity and poor judgement. Attention problems could arise from the over processing of redundant stimuli. The ability to divide or change the focus of attention allows flexibility in thinking, ability to appreciate different points of view and reasoning (Mateer et al. 1996). Difficulties with attention after an ABI are well documented in the literature (Begali 1992, Ewing-Cobbs et al. 1998, Kehle et al. 1996, Semrud-Clikeman 2001, Mateer et al. 1996, Ylvisaker & Szekeres 1998, Walker & Wicks 2005). People, things and their own thoughts will distract learners after an ABI and a programme of gradual re-entry to school is usually advised to avoid attentional overload (Telzrow 1987). Dennis et al. (1995) found that young people with an ABI process both the distractor and the target. Such pupils are more easily distracted in lessons which they find difficult, where many ideas are new and much information is provided. New learning is then adversely affected. Often these children can concentrate on activities of their own choosing but have problems when the activity is chosen for them (Baldwin et al. 1998). They experience difficulties when asked to switch from one activity to another or to maintain attention on a particular task (sustained attention). The effect of difficulties with attentional control in the classroom has not been considered widely, although it has been found that good attention skills are a prerequisite for the successful return to schools in the USA (Semrud-Clikeman 2001, Telzrow 1987). Although it is suggested that difficulties with attention in general may be secondary to other cognitive disabilities (Ylvisaker & Szekeres 1998), many commentators (e.g. Walker & Wicks 2005) note that after an ABI, young people experience difficulties with focussed attention. It is possible for pupils who have sustained an ABI to switch, divide, sustain and focus attention under ideal conditions (paper in preparation). But the ability to become absorbed in a task is essential for learning. Method Pupils were recruited to this study as they were referred to The SHIPS Project (Supporting Head Injured Pupils in Schools), a charity which helps schools and families to manage the education of young people who have sustained an Acquired Brain Injury and who are educated in mainstream or MLD schools. The young people had all sustained severe traumatic injuries, or were in intensive care for a significant amount of time where the injury 2 was non-traumatic. Full consent was obtained from pupils, parents and teachers, and names have been anonymised. Because of the paucity of existing evidence about the performance of ABI students following re-integration into the school setting, and the opportunistic nature of recruitment to the project, a qualitative, naturalistic case study strategy was adopted, seeking to illuminate the difficulties experienced by this group of pupils. Participant observation within the pupils' normal classrooms was undertaken, recording observations as fieldnotes in a range of classes, over several years for some pupils, while on a single session for others. Data from 92 pupils was collected with aetiologies as in Table 1 Aetiology Anoxic injury Encephalitis Traumatic brain injury Tumour Stroke/haemorrhage Meningitis Epilepsy Congenital Heart Defect Table 1 Number of pupils in study 2 6 48 11 11 4 8 2 Table 2 provides further details about Louise, Nasser, William, Joanne, Ruth, Julie, Vicky and Simon whose observations are used as examples Name (Alias) William Ruth Simon Vicky Joanne Nasser Louise Julie Aetiology encephalitis tbi tbi tbi tbi tbi tbi tbi Stage at injury KS4 Infant Lower KS2 Upper KS2 KS4 Early Years KS3 KS3 Stage at observation KS4 Upper KS2 KS3 KS3 KS4 KS3 KS3 KS4 Table 2 Data from observations were subjected to analysis within NVivo (www.qsinternational.com). 3 Classroom Attention Requirements Typically developing pupils seem to be able to move in and out of the different kinds of attention required in the classroom, graphically represented in Figure 1: Sustained attention Into lesson Switching attention Focused attention = absorbed Divided attention Figure 1: typical classroom attention pattern This study was provoked as William’s Maths teacher commented that he was continually disrupted and distracted in class, but could become absorbed when the researcher (SR) was there. William did not have a teaching assistant (TA), he was considered too able to qualify for personal assistance, but the maths class does have TA help once a week. It was not the presence of more adults in the room that made William settle more quickly. This TA is unaware of the needs of ABI pupils, but as a specialist teacher SR is aware of his difficulties. SR seemed to be doing something specific. On examination it appeared that SR was pushing William through his distractions into a period of focussed attention, by gently, and wordlessly, keeping his attention on his work touching the task and bringing his attention to bear on it long enough for him to be able to focus and do it for himself. This could be classed as an aspect of scaffolding, namely direction maintenance where 'The tutor has the role of keeping [learners] in pursuit of a particular objective.' (Wood et al. 1976 p98) Classroom Observations Through the way that they time their instruction, classroom teachers can help pupils with ABI to focus, and in time pupils do learn that they need to focus and not allow themselves to be distracted by others. Simon was four years post injury at this point, in a science lesson about electromagnets. The class are standing or sitting around the front bench with the teacher. Previously they have been working from a text book. The teacher conducted a quick revision of the exercise with questions and answers, accepting some together. There followed a didactic electromagnetic bell works. Simon looked at the 4 answers of the previous lots of ideas and linking explanation of how an teacher and the board as appropriate. The teacher asked them to copy the diagram into their exercise books and annotate it. They were sent back to their seats. Simon immediately gets out his book and starts. The rest of the group chatter and fiddle with equipment. Simon draws in pen. The rest keep mucking around. The teacher admonishes them, but Simon takes no notice. When I checked later, even though there had not been a break in the chatter all had done the work to the teacher's specification. At times Simon was clearly distracted by the mucking about but did not speak or join in. The teacher arrives with the apparatus for them to have a closer look. She explains the process (third time through at least) and others make comments, but again nothing from Simon, though he looks very closely. On completion of the lesson, he had explained what had happened in his book clearly and succinctly, including the vital role of the spring which the teacher had not mentioned. It was in the book. Most pupils can manage to move in and out of being absorbed as in figure 1, getting on with the task while chattering but pupils with ABI do not appear able to do so. This excludes them from the group and makes them appear different. Simon, who was several years post injury at this point, seems to know that he must not join in, but must focus on his work. The arrival of the teacher was at just the right time for Simon to remind him of the explanation she had given and although he again said nothing he looked intently. Notably the teacher did not disrupt his train o f thought by requiring an oral/verbal response. The exercise involved a diagram and annotation, a more iconic representation of the concept rather than the symbolic exercise of writing about it. He had managed to link the teacher’s explanation with that in the textbook. Deep focusing after ABI Further from injury, some pupils are able to move themselves into a deeply focussed state. Once these pupils with an ABI settle there is a period of deeply focussed attention, during which they ignore all around them, including the teacher’s indications that they are moving onto the next part of the lesson. Where the teacher makes the transition less obvious the ABI pupil remains engrossed. Maths Y11 class. Louise and the rest of the class had been working at exercises 'finding the mean' Suddenly the teacher asks the class to give him some numbers. Louise was concentrating on her graph. Everyone else stops their task and provides a number without prompting - this seemed to be a regular feature of lessons. The teacher had to ask her specifically to give a number and had to repeat her name before she was aware. Louise was very deeply engrossed in the task before her and did not respond to her name on the first occasion. Once the teacher had collected the numbers offered and asked the class to multiply them by 10 and 100, they were expected to return to the initial task. Louise did not settle well to this, having been disturbed, and was unable to regain her focussed state. The teacher commented to me that Louise likes her routine and did not come with the ‘starter’ 5 activity being moved to the middle of the session. However it could equally have been a difficulty with the attention cycle having been broken. This seems to be a pattern which occurs in many lessons with several different pupils. Non-injured pupils seem to be able become absorbed in a task much more quickly and then not to focus their attention quite so exclusively and are able to notice the teacher’s indication that the lesson is moving on, or a new task is being set, but this seems rather more difficult for some pupils with ABI, as in this excerpt from a Geography lesson. Louise read the task from the board, the TA read the relevant passage from the book. The teacher then started to read a different passage from the book, but Louise ignored the teacher. She had not reached that part of the task. P2: Where are the atlases? T: There TA: Shall I get you one? Louise nodded (Key for extracts: T is teacher, TA teaching assistant, pupils are identified by their initial, P1, 2, 3 are other pupils, SR is the researcher) Louise was so engrossed in her current task, that she did not attend to the teacher’s next set of instructions or assistance or was not able to divide her attention as the task was so complicated. It may be that she made a decision that she did not need this information as she had not yet reached that part of the task. If so then this is evidence of poor planning! In this class this is overcome by the prompting of the TA to fetch an atlas, and subsequently the TA showed Louise what to do, but not why. That part of the lesson has been lost. This deep focusing can mean pupils get a lot done, but there are also disadvantages to being lost in the task. Disadvantages of deep focusing Pupils may be left behind in previous classes and try to catch up, their focus being on that activity, missing even the beginning of the session Previously in tutor time the class had been asked to make sure their timetables were stuck into their planners. Nasser had not finished but arrived in his Maths class before the teacher so he carried on. T: Nasser He looks up and then down again. He is cutting out his timetable and sticking it in his planner. The teacher has started writing on the board and the class are just settling down T: So if you could write a little grid and a little diagram to help you understand. If you call it Place Value..... Nasser continues his cutting and sticking T: What are you doing? You need to write this down. You need to stop. Put today’s date like this (she writes on the board). I’ll sort that out later. He carried on with it. P1: Do you have to write this? T: Yes please. (reading from the board) The value of a digit depends on its place in the number. N: Shall I write that? 6 T: Yes please. I’ll put units, tens, hundreds, thousands. What shall I put after the line? Other pupils answer but not Nasser. She continues teaching and Nasser continues copying. He copies one letter at a time, which means the copying is very slow. Nasser was so engrossed in the task from tutor-time that he missed the start of the maths lesson. The teacher tried twice to get his attention, but not successfully, perhaps because she use the conditional tense (Rees 2007) and he was only alerted by the question from another pupil, who had arrived at the lesson rather late. Pupils with ABI frequently miss homework being given and there is a constant cry from parents for this group of pupils to be given assistance with writing tasks in their planners. However in the example below the difficulty is seen to be deeper than just the amount of time given in class for the homework task to be recorded. Maths lesson, Nasser is well into his work T: I’m trying to decide whether to make you work on this for homework or give you something else.... Get your planners out Nasser does nothing T: Keep this pink sheet you have now. You can work on this sheet. I want you to do question 3. She writes it on the board. Nasser does not respond. He is deeply into his work. The teacher explains what question 3 is asking them to do and writes it all on the board. Then a few boys who have been very disruptive all lesson decide to run around the class, hitting and throwing things, they run in and out of the door. Nasser does not take any notice whatsoever. T: I want my counters back. The class starts packing up. T: You can stay sitting down to the end of the lesson Nasser is still working on the classwork. The bell went and there was a rush for the door. Nasser looked at me. N: Where have they all gone? Nasser is so deeply focussed on his work that he does not notice the embedded instruction to get out planners, the teacher writing on the board or her explanation of the homework. Certainly her indication that the lesson was over, ‘I want my counters back’ was rather elliptical, but others understood it, particularly in the context of homework being set. Nasser did not even notice the boys running around the room, however they had been rather disruptive and Nasser may have chosen to block out their behaviour. In the general literature several authors hold that ABI students are unable to block out distractions (Ylvisaker & Szekeres 1998, Semrud-Clikemann 2001, Walker & Wicks 2005), but it seems to be that pupils in this study completely block both useful and unhelpful interruptions once they are deeply focused, unlike typically developing pupils. Some pupils do not seem to be aware of transitions at all, which probably contributes to the air of elusiveness they have in the classroom. If pupils have hearing difficulties as a result of their ABI this is frequently given as the reason that they do not follow lesson patterns. But what is more striking is that once focused none of the pupils seem aware of transitions. What 7 eventually arouses Nasser is the strident school bell and it is then a rush to clear away and move to the next class. Vicky's class were offered Sudoku or reading as a fill in activity before the session started. Vicky chose reading poetry V: I’ll get a poem book SR: You like poems? V: Yes they rhyme She read a couple, we discussed them. She read more, silently. T: It’s time for Mrs H’s group to go She did not hear. I touched her arm and pointed at the other pupils. We went to literacy. Like Nasser, Vicky was deeply focused on her task and did not hear the transition to a new lesson. Without a TA, she would have been left in the wrong class, possibly missing the start of the next lesson and having to catch up. Most of these pupils, including Vicky, want to feel 'normal' and anything which points out that they react differently from their peers tends to provoke an outburst. My presence in this class prevented this. Part of the issue for these pupils is that the transitions are frequently in the wrong place. This can be difficult for other groups of pupils too, but in these examples teachers took their timing from the rest of the class, those without an ABI, becoming restless. My presence, specifically looking at the pupil who had sustained an ABI, may have meant that they gave more time than normal. But taking longer to settle, pupils after ABI frequently have not completed one part of the task before the teacher wants to move them on. Vicky was in the literacy class The class had been told to work through a two sided A3 sheet planning and developing their superhero. Vicky was distracted by others mucking around, but gradually settled with the help of SR. Every now and then the teacher read out the next bit, moving the class to the next part of the task as she noticed that they were ready. But Vicky was still completing the section she was on and carried on with the work she was doing. SR had to touch Vicky’s arm and point at the teacher. Vicky looked up and listened. The teacher's timings, taken from those of the rest of the class, did not coincide with Vicky’s timings. Teachers commented that Vicky did not get much work done and was easily distracted, but it was clear from this dynamic assessment session that Vicky was able to focus on work when helped to settle, but that her timings were not the same as those of her peers. Some pupils do notice when the class has moved on, but after the teacher has started talking, as happened to Joanne. Joanne was in a Maths lesson where the class was working through a GCSE practice paper, working on their own, but with guidance from the teacher. T: Now the bearings question. He drew on the board. Suddenly Joanne looked up and asked J: What are we doing? TA: (quietly) Next page. Joanne turned over and carried on. 8 Joanne missed the start of the guidance having to try to make sense of the explanation without the introduction. This then becomes doubly difficult for these pupils. The rest of the class were working at a similar pace, but Joanne took longer on each question to settle and then become focused missing the transition to the next question. This issue of timing is of significant concern to those who teach pupils after an ABI. Having concentrated hard for some time in a period of focussed attention, a pupil who has an ABI is often overcome with fatigue, and finds it difficult to follow the next part of the lesson. During a History lesson, comparing the way of life of the Native Americans and settlers, William listened attentively to the lesson and knew what to do when the activity arrived. P1 made a very loud and persistent comment about gay priests in the Church of England. William smiled and made a brief comment. He then got back to work. A little while later T: OK, look the sheets. A class question and answer session followed which led to the setting of homework. William did not look up. T: Write it in your planner P2: I haven’t got mine T: Those without planners, write it in the front of your books William continued working SR: Where’s your planner? William looked in his bag and looked at me SR: Oh dear! Where will you write it then? W: Don’t know He looked at the back and then the next page of his book SR: In the front? He could not see where to write it. The teacher meant on the cover This is an example of the very subtle difficulties of the ABI pupil. At the beginning of the lesson William was able to divide his attention between his work and the comments of another pupil, but when focussed on the task in hand he completely missed the teaching for the homework task. During the lesson he had produced some good work and by this time he was quite tired. He was then very confused by the instruction to write it in the front of his book. This may have been because he would not usually write the homework in the front, preferring to write it on his hand, and to him writing it in the front of his exercise book was my suggestion, as he had completely missed the teacher’s instructions. His teacher’s comment was that William was doing well in class, but homework was completely disorganised. This excerpt may indicate a possible reason, if intense focus on the classwork task means he misses the teaching for the homework and then is too disorganised to write down the homework clearly. Thus in classroom observations an unexpected attention pattern has been seen. This causes several difficulties for some pupils with ABI. Firstly they do not get as much work done as others as they are unable to settle as quickly. This means they do not get the practice or that they do not have as many notes in their books for revision purposes. Later they miss the transition to a different task or to homework being set. If this is not understood, or not written down, they are then unable to reinforce the learning while studying on their own. 9 Thus much of the lesson has been lost to the pupil. Deep focusing is a significant barrier to understanding what the teacher is trying to communicate to the class. Delineation of the Attention Cycle While for most children attention is under ‘automatic control’ (Walker & Wicks 2005 p36), after an ABI pupils have to work hard to maintain their interest in a task. Under suitable conditions, pupils with an ABI are able to focus, switch, sustain and divide their attention, but some pupils find this a much harder task than others. The findings of our study indicate that, in the classroom, the pattern of attention for young people with an ABI seems to be different from that of other classmates. At the start of an activity pupils with ABI are easily distracted and take a long time to settle. Some pupils with ABI notice everything and seem unable to choose where to focus, as they process all stimuli (Dennis et al. 1995). There follows a period when they are able to dual task, answering other pupils or the teacher while writing. If an adult then helps identify a target for attention, the young person enters a period of deeply focussed attention, when they seem unable to hear anything else in the classroom, including the teacher calling the class to attention for the next part of the task. As one of the TAs in Ruth’s school pointed out: SR: What about in class? TA: She is always good in class. She concentrates really well, in fact sometimes she concentrates so much that she misses things. You see the rest of the class lift their heads when the teacher wants to say something and she carries on with what she’s doing. Later after injury, the young people are able to identify targets for attention and choose where to focus; they seem to need to consciously choose what will engage their attention. However if the teacher breaks their concentration, an unsettled period follows before being able to focus again, graphically represented in Figure 2 distracted Into lesson divided attention disrupted deeply focussed miss next instruction Figure 2: attention pattern of a pupil with ABI 10 Typically developing pupils seem to be able to enter and leave a state of focussed attention which is not as deep, at will; they become absorbed in a task rather than deeply focussed. The cycle followed by the pupil after an ABI takes longer, and hence these pupils frequently achieve less than they would have done had they not sustained their injury, as illustrated in Figure 3 distracted divided attention absorbed disrupted deeply focussed Key Pupils with ABI Other pupils miss next instruction Figure 3: comparison of the attention patterns of a typical pupil and a pupil with ABI Which ABI groups do this? An examination of the data from all the young people in the study has shown that not all pupils with an ABI are subject to the tyranny of deep focusing (Table 3). Aetiology Number of pupils in study Unable to focus Over focus No opportunity to observe Anoxic injury Encephalitis Traumatic brain injury Tumour Stroke/ haemorrhage Meningitis Epilepsy Congenital Heart Defects Table 3 2 6 48 0 0 6 2 5 29 0 1 8 11 11 6 0 4 2 4 8 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 11 No difficulties in this area Percentage observed with overfocus Percentage observed with no focus 100 100 73 0 0 15 1 0 40 18 60 0 0 0 0 0 4 100 100 100 While all the pupils in this small sample with anoxic injuries and encephalitis have this changed attention pattern, only 73% , 40% and 18% of pupils who had sustained injuries traumatically, via a brain tumour or a stroke showed this attention cycle. What is notable is that there are also pupils within these three groups who could not control their focus of attention at all. Perhaps William had been in this group when SR was not present and this is what the Maths teacher had noticed. However there were 4 pupils with a traumatic brain injury who were able to manage their attention, becoming absorbed in tasks and noticing transitions without assistance. Three of these four sustained moderate injuries and the fourth was a non-accidental injury after which she was adopted. There is little documentation available for her. Pupils with an ABI through meningitis, epilepsy or congenital heart defects, at least in this study, did not show this pattern at all. As deep-focussing has been shown above to be helpful in some circumstances, further work could ascertain whether these skills could be taught to those whose do not show this tendency and whether young people could develop control to ameliorate the disadvantages. What can teachers do to help This cluster of pupils who have sustained a brain injury due to trauma, tumour, encephalitis or anoxia does not have a typical learning pattern. Most of these pupils are able to achieve a state of focussed attention in the classroom when they are further in time from their injury, but this may be assisted by other adults in the classroom for those who have just returned to school, in fact this may be the most important task for a TA supporting a pupil with an ABI. However once in this state all the pupils seem to find it hard to return to a normal level of attention and to notice what is happening around them, which means that they may miss vital learning and teaching. TAs will require additional training when supporting a pupil in this group to emphasise the importance of intervening to assist the young person when shifts in attention are called for. Pupils who are affected by an a-typical attention cycle need to learn to become focused on a task. Within the project it has been found that the most successful way to do this is to touch the work without uttering a word, drawing the pupil back to the task time and again until they are focused. This does take more time, and teachers will need to be aware of this when considering the tasks to be undertaken by different pupils. This presumes a differentiated class which is essential for the inclusion of pupils with an ABI. Teachers also need to signal transition points explicitly between lesson activities, forewarning the pupil with the ABI who will probably be in a state of deep focus. Pupils may be alerted to new instructions verbally when they are not in deep focus but they will need to be explicit, along the lines of ‘stop what you are doing and pay attention to my instructions’. However if TAs do too much for the pupil they may not allow the pupil to develop strategies to focus, as they interrupt the process. In many schools homework is recorded by the TA rather than the pupil with an ABI, but frequently this is not enough information when the pupil has not focused on the instructions from the teacher. Teachers or TAs could be aware of this difficulty with homework. His teacher’s comment was that William was doing well in class, but homework is completely disorganised. If pupils focus too intently on the classwork task, miss the teaching for the homework and then lacks the organisational skills to write down the homework clearly, it is no wonder that homework is not done, done inadequately and causes family rows. 12 Homework instructions would be better provided as written handouts or by email for ABI students. While the young person with ABI does not need minute-by-minute assistance in the classroom, they do need transitions to be signalled, as well as ensuring that the homework task is in their planner in sufficient detail. With the general help of the parents/carers, pupils may then find homework a less onerous task. When pupils have been focusing deeply they often complain of a headache or fatigue; this may contribute the need for extra rest periods, or a partial time-table for pupils with ABI. All the pupils I observed had hour-long lessons. Some schools have shorter lessons and the problems of fatigue may be different. Moving around the cycle It seems that most of these pupils can achieve a deeply focused state, sometimes on their own, and sometimes with assistance. At times this deep focusing can be helpful, in that young people may then be able to complete tasks. However, it does not happen all the time and it is not always beneficial in the classroom if the pupils then miss out on learning opportunities. It is clear that on return to school, and for a few years after, pupils with ABI do not manage to move into this cycle without help. They then remain distractible for the whole lesson and achieve very little. Where a teaching assistant is in the class, they could be instructed to assist the pupil with ABI to settle and then watch as they become focussed. When the teacher wants to introduce the next task it will be necessary for the assistant to bring the pupil out of their focussed state in order to be ready to learn with the rest of the class. This rigid attention cycle causes several difficulties for these pupils. Firstly they do not get as much work done as others as they are unable to settle as quickly. This means they do not get the practice or that they do not have as many notes in their books for revision purposes. The rigid pattern of attention also robs the ABI pupil of the ability to follow when a teacher moves the next activity, and poor monitoring skills mean they do not complete tasks. Later they miss the transition to a different task or to homework being set. If this is not understood, or not written down, they are then unable to reinforce the learning while studying on their own. Thus much of the lesson has been lost to the pupil. This is a significant barrier to understanding what the teacher is trying to communicate to the class. This paper has drawn on the observations of a few pupils, but they are in line with the others in the study. As pupils develop after the point of injury, it seems that some, like Simon and Nasser, are able to learn to focus their attention, but earlier on in recovery, like William, they need assistance. Frequently lack of concentration in the classroom is seen by teachers as the pupil engaging in ‘attention-seeking behaviour’ (Hawley 2005). From this study it may be seen that the issue is more complex than this may suggest. 13 Bibliography Baldwin, T., Seddon, H., Demellweek, C., Hughes, B., & Fishwick, S. (1998) Cognitive Deficits in Appleton & Baldwin Management of brain-injured children Oxford: OUP Begali, V. (1992) Head injury in children and adolescents: A resource and review for school and allied professionals Brandon VT: Clinical psychology Publishing Company Dennis, M., Wilkinson, M., Koski, L. & Humphreys, R.P.(1995) Attention deficits in the long term after childhood head injury. In Broman, S. & Michel, M.E.(eds) Traumatic head injury in children and adolescents NY:OUP (pp165-187) Dennis, M., Barnes, M.A., Donelly, R.E., Wilkinson, M. & Humphreys, R.P. (1996) Appraising and managing knowledge: Metacognitive skills after childhood head injury Developmental Neuropsychology 12(1) 77-103 Dennis, M., Barnes, M.A., Wilkinson, M. & Humphreys, R. (1998) How children with head injury represent real and deceptive emotion in short narratives Brain and Language 61, 450-483 Ewing-Cobbs, L., Levin, H. S., and Fletcher, J.M., (1998) Neuropsychological sequelae after paediatric traumatic brain injury: advances since 1985 in Ylvisaker, M. (ed) Traumatic Brain Injury Rehabilitation: Children and Adolescents Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann (pp11-26) Hawley, C.A. (2005) Saint or sinner? Teacher perception of a child with TBI Pediatric Rehabilitation 8(2) 117-129 Hawley, C.A., Ward, A.B., Magnay, A. R. & Long, J. (2002) Children’s brain injury: a postal follow up of 525 children from one health region in the UK Brain Injury 161 (1) 969-985 Hawley, C. A., Ward, A. B., Long, J., Owen, D. W. & Magnay, A. R. (2003) ‘Prevalence of traumatic brain injury amongst children admitted to hospital in one health district: a population based survey.’ Injury, International Journal of Care for the Injured , 34, pp. 256–60. Hawley, C.A., Ward, A.B., Magnay, A.R. & Long, J., (2004) Outcomes following childhood head injury: a population study Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 75, 737-742 Johnson, D.A. (1992) Head injured Children and education: a need for greater delineation and understanding British Journal of Educational Psychology 62, 404-409 Kehle, T.J., Clark, E., & Jenson, W.R. (1996) Interventions for students with Traumatic Brain Injury: Managing behavioural disturbances Journal of Learning disabilities 29 (6) 633-642 Lord-Maes, J. & Obrzut J.E. (1996) Neuropsychological consequences of traumatic brain injury in children and adolescents Journal of Learning Disabilities 29 (6) 609-617 Mateer, C.A., Kerns, K.A. & Eso, K.L. (1996) Management of attention and memory disorders following Traumatic Brain Injury Journal of Learning Disabilities 29 (6) 618-632 Petit-Zeman, S. (2001) Missing out: The lifelong impact of children’s acquired brain injury CABIIG QSR International Nvivo 10 http://www.qsrinternational.com/products.aspx accessed 19/2/15 Rees, S.A. (2007) Understanding instruction: how severely brain injured pupils ,make meaning in the mainstream secondary classroom Unpublished PhD thesis. Bath: University of Bath Rees, S.A. (in preparation) Pay attention please: issues of classroom attention after Acquired Brain Injury (ABI) Royal College of Surgeons of England (1999) Report of the Working Party in Management of patients with head injuries London: Royal College of Surgeons of England Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2001) Traumatic Brain Injury in Children and Adolescents: Assessment and intervention Guildford Press 14 Sharples, P.M., Storey, A., Aynsley-Green, A., & Eyre, J.A. (1990) Avoidable factors contributing to the death of children with head injury British Medical Journal 300, 87-9 Telzrow, C.F. (1987) Management of academic and educational problems in head injury Journal of learning disabilities 20 (9) (pp563-545) Walker, S. & Wicks, B. (2005) Educating children with acquired brain injury London: David Fulton Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89-100. Ylvisaker, M. & Szekeres, S.F. (1998) A framework for cognitive rehabilitation, in Ylvisaker, M. (ed) Traumatic Brain Injury Rehabilitation: Children and Adolescents Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann (pp125-158) 15