‘What to do when?’ examples Example 1 - The moon illusion Why does the moon appear to be so much larger when it is near the horizon than when it is directly overhead? This question has produced a wide variety of theories from psychologists. An important early hypothesis was put forth by Holway and Boring (1940) who suggested that the illusion was due to the fact that when the moon was on the horizon, the observer looked straight at it with eyes level, whereas when it was at its zenith, the observer had to elevate his or her eyes as well as his or her head to see it. To test this hypothesis, Kaufman and Rock (1962) devised an apparatus that allowed them to present two artificial moons, one at the horizon and one at the zenith…. Subjects were asked to adjust the variable horizon moon to match the size of the zenith moon or vice versa. For each subject the ratio of the perceived size of the horizon moon to the perceived size of the zenith moon was recorded…. A ratio of 1.00 would represent no illusion. A ratio of 1.5 would mean the moon appears 1.5 times as large on the horizon as at its zenith. moon illusion (contd.) Question: Does the moon appear bigger at its zenith, i.e., does the ratio differ from 1.0? What kind of test would you use? Options: z-score test (C. 8) single sample t-test (C. 9) independent samples t-test (C. 10) related samples t-test (C. 11) Which analysis? One sample t-test Steps A ratio of 1.0 = no illusion 1. State hypotheses: H0: 1 = 1 H1: 1 <> 1 =.05 2. Determine df df = n - 1 = 10 - 1 = 9 Steps (contd.) 3. Obtain data 3a. Calc. SS SS =1.045 s = SS/n-1 =.341 M= 1.46 Subject ratio 1 1.73 2 1.06 3 2.03 4 1.4 5 0.95 6 1.13 7 1.41 8 1.73 9 1.63 10 1.56 sumX ratio^2 sum X^2 Calculations (contd.) M 1.463 1.00 t 4.29 sM 1.08 sM t.05(9) = 2.262 Confidence intervals 2 s .116 .108 n 10 M tsM 1.46 2.262 * .108 .244 1.462 upper 1.707 lower 1.219 Interpret your results the moon does appear larger at its horizontal position compared to its zenith the probability is .95 that an interval such as 1.219 - 1.707 includes the true mean ratio for the moon illusion. Note. the value of 1.00 is not included within this interval, which represents no illusion. SPSS output Analyze/Compare Means/One Sample T One-Sample Statistics N LEVEL 10 Mean 1.4630 Std. Deviation .3407 Std. Error Mean .1077 One-Sample Test Tes t Value = 1.0 LEVEL t 4.298 df 9 Sig. (2-tailed) .002 Mean Difference .4630 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference Lower Upper .2193 .7067 Example 2 - The moon illusion A different question Why does the moon appear to be so much larger when it is near the horizon than when it is directly overhead? This question has produced a wide variety of theories from psychologists. An important early hypothesis was put forth by Holway and Boring (1940) who suggested that the illusion was due to the fact that when the moon was on the horizon, the observer looked straight at it with eyes level, whereas when it was at its zenith, the observer had to elevate his or her eyes as well as his or her head to see it. To test this hypothesis, Kaufman and Rock (1962) devised an apparatus that allowed them to present two artificial moons, one at the horizon and one at the zenith, and to control whether the subjects elevated their eyes or kept them level to see the zenith moon. The horizon, or comparison, moon was always viewed with eyes level. Subjects were asked to adjust the variable horizon moon to match the size of the zenith moon or vice versa. For each subject the ratio of the perceived size of the horizon moon to the perceived size of the zenith moon was recorded with eyes elevated and with eyes level. A ratio of 1.00 would represent no illusion. If Holway and Boring were correct, there should be a greater illusion in the eyes-elevated condition than in the eyes-level condition. Is there a difference in the two conditions? Which analysis? Steps 1. State hypotheses: H0: 1 - 2 = 0 H1: 1 - 2 <> 0 =.05 2. Determine df df = df1 + df2 =(n1 - 1) + (n2-1) = 9+9 = 18 Steps (contd.) 3. Obtain data 3a. Calc. SSD =.169 sD2 = SSD/n-1 = .169/9= = .0189 MD=.019 Subject Elevated Level 1 1.65 1.73 2 1 1.06 3 2.03 2.03 4 1.25 1.4 5 1.05 0.95 6 1.02 1.13 7 1.67 1.41 8 1.86 1.73 9 1.56 1.63 10 1.73 1.56 Calculations (contd.) sM D s2 .0189 .0434 n 10 M D D .019 0 t .438 sM D .0434 Consult t-table =.05 2-tail SPSS output Paired Samples Statistics Pair 1 ELEVATED LEVEL Mean 1.48 1.46 N 10 10 Std. Deviation .37 .34 Std. Error Mean .12 .11 Paired Samples Correlations N Pair 1 ELEVATED & LEVEL 10 Correlation .931 s sM n Sig. .000 Paired Samples Test Paired Differences Pair 1 ELEVATED - LEVEL Mean 1.90E-02 Std. Deviation .14 Std. Error Mean 4.34E-02 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference Lower Upper -7.91E-02 .12 t df .438 9 Sig. (2-tailed) .672 Example 3 - one group receives Paxol and the other a placebo See text p.338, #20 Tx1 Tx2 3 12 5 10 7 8 1 14