Compliance THE MODIFICATION OF BEHAVIOUR IN RESPONSE TO A DIRECT REQUEST, EVEN THOUGH THE PERSON MAKING THE REQUEST HAS NO POWER TO ENFORCE COMPLIANCE Compliance vs Conformity Literature often uses the two terms interchangeably. COMPLIANCE: the surface behavioural response to a request by another individual. CONFORMITY: the influence of a group upon an individual that usually produces more enduring internalized changes in one’s attitudes and beliefs The Rule of Reciprocation • Cialdini (1993): Argues that people are socialized into returning favours so powerful rule underpinning compliance • Anthropologists Tiger & Fox (1971) argue that reciprocation (mutual indebtedness) is perhaps the result of evolution. • Lynn & McCall (1988) found that restaurants who offered a mint or sweet with the bill received larger tips. The Rule of Reciprocation Regan (1971) • Carried out a lab experiment to investigate if people who had received a free coke would return the favour when asked to buy raffle tickets from the same person compared to a control who had received no favour. • Questionnaires were given to rate the “liking” of the confederate to investigate the effect of liking on doing a favour for a person. • Participants who had received a favour in the experimental condition bought a significantly higher number of raffle tickets compared to participants in the control condition. • “Liking” did not influence the number of raffle tickets bought in the experimental condition. Those who did not like the confederate bought as many tickets as those who said they liked the confederate; in the control condition they bought more tickets if they liked the confederate. • Conclusion: the rule of reciprocity is very powerful – a strong sense of social obligation. Cultural norms and reciprocity Ting-Toomey (1986) anthropologist • Carried out field research; compared the forms of reciprocity in 3 individualist cultures (Australia, USA, France) with reciprocity in two collectivist (Japan and China). Results • The rule of reciprocity appears to be universal – which supports an evolutionary explanation) but • Reciprocity displayed differently: • • Individualist countries: voluntary reciprocity – free will in returning favour Collectivist countries: obligatory reciprocity the norm - moral failure if reciprocity is not honoured Compliance Techniques Cialdini Authority Liking Social Proof Scarcity Reciprocity Consistency & Commitment Authority People are more willing to follow the directions or recommendations of a communicator to whom they attribute relevant authority or expertise. One study showed that 3 times as many pedestrians were willing to follow a man into traffic against the red light when he was merely dressed as an authority in a business suit and tie. Liking Individuals are more likely to buy a product from someone they like (e.g., a friend or relative having a Tupperware party, or people being more likely to buy a product from people who are similar to themselves). Social Proof One means used by individuals to determine what is correct is to find out what others believe is correct. People often view a behavior as more correct in a given situation--to the degree that we see others performing it. One researcher went door to door collecting for charity and carrying a list of others in the area who had already contributed. The longer the list, the more contributions it produced. Social proof is most influential under two conditions: Uncertainty--when people are unsure and the situation is ambiguous they are more likely to observe the behavior of others and to accept that behavior as correct Similarity--people are more inclined to follow the lead of others who are similar. Scarcity People find objects and opportunities more attractive to the degree that they are scarce, rare, or dwindling in availability. Even information that is scarce is more effective. A beef importer in the US informed his customers (honestly) that, because of weather conditions in Australia, there was likely to be a shortage of Australian beef. His orders more than doubled. However, when he added (also honestly) that this information came from his company's exclusive contacts in the Australian National Weather Service, orders increased by 600%! Reciprocity Individuals tend to reciprocate; if someone does something for you (such as, lowering the price of their product), then you feel more obligated to do something for them (like buy the product). One of the most prevalent social norms directs us to return to others favours, goods and services that they offer to us. According to the American Disabled Veterans organization, mailing out a simple appeal for donations produces an 18% success rate; but, enclosing a small gift-personalized address labels--boosts the success rate to 35% Consistency & Commitment If you make a small commitment, then you will be more likely to commit to something larger in the future (e.g., just buy a single book, then you'll be more likely to buy all the other books that the Time/Life company will regularly send you). Consistency & Commitment Good personal consistency is highly valued by society. Consistent conduct provides a beneficial approach to daily life. A consistent orientation affords a valuable shortcut through the complexity of modern existence. That is-- by being consistent with earlier decisions we can reduce the need to process all the relevant information in future similar situations. Instead, one merely needs to recall the earlier decision and respond consistently. Consistency & Commitment Foot in the Door Door in the Face Lowballing Consistency & Commitment Foot in the Door: A compliance technique whereby a small request is made first and is then followed up with a larger one If asked to sign a petition first then more likely to comply when next asked to make a donation Consistency & Commitment: Foot in the Door Technique Freedman & Fraser (1966) Researchers arranged for a researcher posing as a volunteer worker to ask a number of householders in California to allow a big, ugly public service sign reading ‘Drive Carefully’ to be placed in their front garden. Only 17% of householders complied with the request. A different set of householders was asked whether they would display a small ‘Be a Safe Driver’ sign. Nearly all of those asked agreed to this request. Two weeks later, these same homeowners were asked by a ‘volunteer worker’ whether they would display the much bigger and ugly ‘Drive Carefully sign in their front gardens. 76% complied with the second request – far higher than the 17% that complied with the first request. Consistency & Commitment: Foot in the Door Technique Freedman & Fraser (1966) In a SECOND study, Freedman and Fraser (1966) first asked a number of householders to sign a petition in favour of keeping California beautiful, something nearly everybody agreed to do. After two weeks, they sent a new ‘volunteer worker’ who asked these homeowners if they would display the large, ugly ‘Drive Carefully’ sign of the previous study to be displayed in their front gardens. Note: the two requests relate to completely different topics but nearly half of the homeowners complied with the second request. Again – this is significantly higher than the 17% of homeowners who agreed without prior contact. Consistency & Commitment: Foot in the Door Technique Factors affecting FITD compliance Initial requests can’t be so large that people refuse to do them (Burger, 1999). FITD requires delay between initial request and larger one (Burger, 1999). When the same person makes the requests, it is difficult to get compliance (Chartrand et al., 1999) People with a high degree of consistency show bigger FITD effects (Cialdini et al., 1999) Consistency & Commitment: Foot in the Door Technique WHY? Self Perception Theory, Bem, (1972) We perceive from the first request that we are the type of person who gives help in this type of situation and our future behaviour is guided by this. FITD only works if the initial request is big enough to gain some sense of commitment to the cause which is attributed by the individual to dispositional (internal) factors Consistency & Commitment: Foot in the Door Technique WHY? Perceptual contrast hypothesis: Cantrill and Seibold (1986) The first request acts as an anchor (baseline) against which subsequent requests are compared Second request not seen as so burdensome as first request has already ‘prepared the ground’; it doesn’t seem so great Not supported as well as Bem’s theory Consistency & Commitment: Door-in-the-Face Technique A compliance technique in which a large request is made first and is then followed up by a small one Someone calls asking for a large donation to a charity which is likely to be refused, they then ask for a smaller donations; this is has proved to be far more effective than asking straight out for the same small donation. Consistency & Commitment: Door-in-the-Face Technique Cialdini et al (1975) Control Group 1: Participants were approached and asked to escort a group of juvenile delinquents to the zoo; most refused. Control Group 2: Participants were approached and asked to spend 2 hours per week as a peer counsellor to juvenile delinquent children for around 2 years; again most said no Experimental Group (the DITF): asked to be peer counsellors and then asked to escort children to the zoo. Consistency & Commitment: Door-in-the-Face Technique Cialdini et al (1975) - Results Large request only: 0% Small request only: 25% Told about but not asked big request then small request made: 16.7% DITF group: 50% compliance Also tested whether the two requests needed to be done by the same requester in order to achieve compliance. With two different requesters only 10.5%, Also found that two equivalent requests did not improve compliance (33%) Consistency & Commitment: Door-in-the-Face Technique Guéguen & Meineri (2011 ) tested whether the amount of time between the first request and the second request made a difference in the level of compliance. In their study, waitresses were told to ask randomly selected restaurant customers whether they wanted dessert at the end of their meal. There were then two conditions if the naive participant refused. In the first condition, the waitress immediately asked if the participant wanted tea or coffee. In the second condition, they left and returned three minutes later to ask if they wanted tea or coffee Consistency & Commitment: Door-in-the-Face Technique Guéguen & Meineri (2011 ) The results showed that people were more likely to order tea or coffee in the immediate condition, rather than in the delayed condition. Consistency & Commitment: Door-in-the-Face Technique DITF Evaluation Many studies support its effectiveness Evidence suggest it is more effective then FITD Why does it work? The norm of reciprocity The lion with the thorn in its foot Help those who help you; cultural conditioning: salesman makes concession, you feel compelled to do the same The more concessions made, the more likely the compliance, (Goldman and Creason, 1981) Consistency & Commitment: Door-in-the-Face Technique DITF Explanations Perceptual contrast hypothesis; evidence is inconclusive Worthy person hypothesis, (Foehl and Goldman, 1983): guilt is induced by refusing a worthy cause (most studies have made requests to give to charity etc) Consistency & Commitment: Lowballing A compliance technique in which an unreasonably low offer is made, and when commitment is elicited, replaced with a higher offer on the pretence that the lower one could not be honoured Used by salespeople, who say they have to check the offer made with their manager and then get back to you saying it they have to offer a slightly higher price Consistency & Commitment: Lowballing Burger & Cornelius (2003) In this study, students were contacted by phone by a female caller and asked whether they would be prepared to donate $5 to a scholarship fund for underprivileged students. There were THREE experimental conditions: 1. Lowball Condition: Students were told that those who contributed would receive a coupon for a free smoothie at a local juice bar. Students who agreed were then informed that the investigator realised she had run out of coupons. The students were asked if they would still be willing to contribute. 76% agreed to make a donation in this condition. Consistency & Commitment: Lowballing Burger & Cornelius (2003) 2. Interrupt Condition: The caller made the same initial request as in the Lowball Condition. However, before the participants had a chance to give their answer, the caller interrupted them to let them know there were no more coupons left. 16% of the participants made a donation in this condition. 3. Control Condition: Participants were simply asked to donate the $5 without any mention of coupons. 42% made a contribution in the Control condition. Consistency & Commitment: Lowballing Burger & Cornelius (2003) cont. The results support the view that the Lowball technique is effective only when individuals have made an initial public commitment. Once they have made this commitment, individuals feel obliged to act in accordance with it even when the conditions that led to them making the commitment have changed (Cialdini, 2009) Consistency & Commitment: Lowballing Caldini (1974) Researchers asked a class of first year psychology students to volunteer to be a part of a study on Cognition that would meet at 7 am. 24% of the College students were willing to get up at that time of day and attend. In a second group, they were asked the same favour – to be a part of the study on Cognition – but were not told the time. 56% agreed to participate without knowing the time. Consistency & Commitment: Lowballing Caldini (1974) cont When subsequently told that it would mean being in attendance at 7 am, nobody backed out of their commitment. 95% (of the 56%) showed up for the 7 am commitment. That represents DOUBLE the 24% that would have attended from the first condition. Consistency & Commitment: Lowballing Once a commitment has been made you are likely to follow through with it even if the conditions change somewhat Commitment to an individual seems more important than committing to the behaviour; if the ‘sales-manager’ takes over the negotiating , the customer is more likely to pull out than if the original salesperson continues with the deal (Burger and Petty 1981) Consistency & Commitment: Lowballing WHY? Cognitive Dissonance having made a decision to purchase something (following the low offer), we justify the decision to ourselves; we are not just being rash because it seems like a bargain, we actually do need this item! If the item is then re-offered at a higher price, we will experience a uncomfortable state called cognitive dissonance if we then decide to pull out (suggesting that we did only want the item because it was a bargain) We are more likely to continue with the deal, making our behaviour consistent with our attitude (we really do need this item) Consistency & Commitment