Development of Relative Clauses in African American English Gwynne Morrissey, Jill de Villiers, & Peter de Villiers Smith College, Northampton, MA Results Question 1: Use of Relative Clauses to Specify the Referent MAE Relative Clause Production: SS Prompts d) The child could passivize the verb: e.g. The cat sitting in the flowers is being fed by the man 100% 100% 90% 90% DIS 90% 80% 80% TYP 80% 70% 70% 10% 10% 20% 0% 0% OS Pronoun Use 80% 70% 100% 90% 40% 80% 30% 70% 20% 10% 0% 4 5 6 8.5 60% typaae 50% typmae 40% 30% Age Group KEY: typ = typically developing dis = disordered MAE = Mainstream American English AAE = African American English 10% 0% n 100% Prompt: Relative Clause Use © The Psychological Corporation Here are two cats. They are doing different things. 70% 60% DIS 50% TYP 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% 4 5 6 Age Group OS, F(3, 40) = 2.08, p = .12 An analysis of means of use of the zero relative versus that in SS responses showed that only that increased with age. 80% 70% 60% DIS 50% TYP 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% 4 Target: The man is feeding the cat that is sitting in the flowers. OS, F(1, 40) = .04, p = .8 100% Relative Clause Use I need to know which cat it is. Dialect was not significant for either: Targeted OS proved to be harder to produce than targeted SS in both dialects. SS, F(3, 56) = 2.77, p = .05 90% 5 6 Age Group 8.5 Clefts are more prevalent in MAE, likely because the it is… construction is existential in AAE, therefore blocking it from being used in a cleft. Passives are used earlier and more often in MAE, likely because the construction requires -ed, a morpheme often optional in AAE. Age was slightly significant for SS, but not for OS: Prompt: Age Group A repeated measures test was run for the typically developing children’s use of relative pronouns. Dialect and age were the independent variables. SS, F(1, 56) = 1.09, p = .3 or 8.5 AAE Relative Clause Production: OS Prompts Tell me what is happening in the red box on your picture. who 80% I have the same two cats. Only you can see these pictures. that Pronoun 90% Method Age Group Topicalization in SS situations declines in the oldest group in both dialects, but in OS situations, topicalization increases with age for AAE. 20% MAE Relative Clause Production: OS Prompts su TYP 4 5 6 8.5 un 50% 4 5 6 8.5 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% t 90% 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% cl ef Pronoun unsuc c essful Relative clause strategies - AAE OS Relative clause strategies - MAE OS who Percent usage what topic l that cc es sf u n DIS SS Age Group 0% AAE Relative Clause Production: SS Prompts 60% c left unsuccessful Age Group 100% Dialect and clinical status were determined by a combination of a clinician report and performances on the DELV screener and criterion-referenced test. topic ul 10% SS sf cleft cc es Age Group 30% un su 8.5 20% e 6 30% 20% si v 5 40% 30% pa s 4 typmae 40% e 0% 50% 40% 50% iv 10% typaae 50% pa ss 20% 60% 4 5 6 8.5 60% to pi c 30% 4 5 6 8.5 60% O S 70% S 40% SS 60% Percent usage 100% 70% 50% Relative clause strategies - AAE SS Relative clause strategies - MAE SS cl ef t Total 77 73 153 140 443 c) The child could use a cleft structure with a relative: e.g. It’s the cat sitting in the flowers that the man is feeding Percent usage 8.5 25 23 25 31 104 b) The child could use a topicalization structure with a relative: e.g. The cat that’s sitting in the flowers, the man is feeding it. SS Pronoun Use 80% Incidence 4 9 13 35 30 87 OS, F(1, 427) = 62.64, p < .000 Age was also highly significant for each: SS, F(3, 427) = 8.29, p < .000 OS, F(3, 427) = 18.319, p < .000 However, dialect was not significant; both groups performed equivalently on the task: SS, F(1, 427) = .325 n.s. OS, F(1, 427) = 1.59 n.s. 90% Relative Clause Use Group disAAE disMAE typAAE typMAE Total SS, F(1, 427) = 37.709, p < .000 Question 2: Relative Pronoun Use 100% Participants Age Group 5 6 18 25 18 19 36 57 41 38 113 139 a) Most primitively, the child could produce two independent sentences, or a conjunction. These were counted as unsuccessful attempts at relative clause production. e.g. The cat’s sitting in the flowers and the man’s feeding it. O There are two types of interest in this study: SS: subject-subject The relativized noun is the subject of the main clause and the relative clause, as in The mom who fed the baby is riding her bike. OS: object-subject The relativized noun is the object of the main clause and the subject of the relative clause, as in The man fed the cat who was sitting in the flowers. Previous research says that… in production, it is easier to relativize a subject than an object (Comrie, 1981), in comprehension, it is easier to parse an OS than an SS (Hamburger & Crain, 1982; Kidd & Bavin, 2002) Why might development of relative clauses in AAE-speaking children be different? AAE characteristics likely to affect relative clause acquisition. Past tense –ed deletion, some times She miss your call yesterday. because of consonan t cluster reduction Nonstandard relative pronoun what, zero the dog what we found in the street, marker (Ø) a whistle Ø sound like a bird Appositive pronoun (topicaliz ation) The girl that was dancing, her dad made her a sandw ich. Existential there it It’s so much stuff I hafta do. Several syntactic alternatives to OS and SS relatives were used to get the same meaning across. to pi c that Jack built. Specifies a referent, i.e. not the house that Joe built or the cheese in the fridge. Question 3: Strategies Used to Replace Basic Relatives Percent usage This is the cat that killed the rat that ate the cheese that lay in the house Results Two ANOVAs were run, one each for success on SS and OS relative clause types. Dialect (AAE versus MAE), clinical status (typical versus disordered) and age (4, 5, 6, 8.5) were the independent variables. Clinical status was highly significant for both types: Incidence A postnominal modifier (i.e. noun descriptor), as in: 1. What is the difference between typically developing and disordered children’s likelihood of producing a referent-specifying relative clause at all? 2. Of the intended responses to both SS and OS items (i.e. SS relative clauses and OS relative clauses), what relative pronouns were used? Green has suggested that relative pronouns are more optional and might be replaced by what in the adult AAE dialect (2002). Is there a tendency for AAE speakers to drop the pronoun or use a nonstandard one? 3. What strategies do children use in place of a basic relative clause? Relative Clause Use What is a relative clause? Results Research Questions SS Introduction Interestingly enough, the only child to use a nonstandard relative pronoun was an MAE speaker. All other zero uses were legitimate (e.g. in reduced relatives). Use of relative pronouns by disordered speakers was not analyzed due to small sample size (only 10 responses of disordered children included relative clauses requiring relative pronouns). Conclusions 1. The relative clause elicitation task proved to be a very useful diagnostic indicator for children with language disorders. At the same time, it proved useful for both AAE and MAE speakers, with no bias effects. However, it is highly demanding. 2. Relative pronoun use did not show up differently on this task between dialects, but this may reflect the limitation on types of relatives elicited. It was not revealing for language disorder because so few disordered children produced relatives at all. 3. Subtle dialect effects may be seen in the varieties of devices children use in the task. Poster presented at the ASHA Convention, Philadelphia, PA, 2004