ROUGH EDITED COPY EHDI Conference-SUPPORTING FAMILIES WITHOUT BIAS: A PANEL DISCUSSION Considerations in Assessment of Autism Spectrum Disorder with Children 3/9/15 CAPTIONING PROVIDED BY: ALTERNATIVE COMMUNICATION SERVICES, LLC P.O. BOX 278 LOMBARD, IL 60148 NOTE FROM CAPTIONER: Thanks! Everything good! Okay. Thanks! (Standing by). (Standing by). (Standing by). (Standing by). (Standing by). >> Okay. I think we are going to get started. I'm Katie, the moderator. Please just make sure you fill out the evaluation. We are in the considerations and assessment for autism spectrum disorder for children who are deaf and hard-of-hearing. >> Thank you. How lucky am I in that I have the privilege of presenting with Susan Wiley and Chris Yoshinaga-Itano, in fact. Making me do much of the presentation. So very lucky, indeed. The person who is also on our panel or contributed significantly to this is Deborah Mood, and she was not able to join us here today, but certainly gets kudos on this presentation. We are going to talk with you today about autism in children who are deaf and hard-of-hearing and some of the complications around that, a little bit of research about what we are learning with the population as well as what we know about diagnostic considerations and we will end with some intervention. So we hope it is of interest to you. We are from these places. Colorado and Cincinnati and Boston and we are collectively bringing together some of our minds and our experience clinically and wanting to help inform you. I will say that the work that we are presenting is based largely on a paper that was published recently. It is making too much noise. I think I'm too close. How is that? Better? Okay. We published a paper in seminars in speech and language and it came out in November of 2014. So a large part of this is sort of based on that and we do have that resource to be able to share with you. So access that if you are interested for more information. Disclosure, we are not getting paid for this, sadly. That's all. (Laughter). Christi is associated with the LINA (sp) project. But doesn't have a financial interest that we are presenting today. We are hoping to talk about rates of autism in deaf and hard-of-hearing population and as well as describing some of the developmental trajectories. We are going to talk a little bit about red flags, some tables to present to you that is sort of looking at when somebody presents with issues, how do you kind of figure out if it might be under ASD or just reduced hearing. And then we will talk also about assessments. Based on our experience and on the literature we are going to address these things. We will talk about that etiology, the red flags, assessment, implications for dual diagnosis and what that might mean in terms of intervention, resources for family and then also thoughts for professionals interested in this field and what might we need to know going forward. So why is this topic important? If you went to the plenary this morning then you are aware that within the deaf and hard-of-hearing population, the incidence of autism is increased relative to those who don't have reduced hearing. So studies are coming out, we are looking at that around 4 percent of children who are deaf and hard-of-hearing fall within the continuum of autism spectrum disorder. That's a rough number, though, because as we will show you the diagnostic process and the availability of how we figure that out, there is still some gaps in what we know. We will share what we do know and then we will point our where those gaps are and what we need to sort of think about going forward. Misdiagnosis can be significant. So if a child has autism spectrum disorder and they are deaf and the autism spectrum disorder is missed or misdiagnosed, there are big implications because they won't get the right kinds of interventions and supports and perhaps get less supports. We also know from the literature that historically the diagnosis hasn't happened efficiently or quickly and we will show you some of that in the slides, so that we are hoping that currently if we take kids who are newly diagnosed and they are young now and now we have better knowledge about how autism can present in this population, that going forward we can produce better outcomes and results for these kids and their families. All right. So rates of autism spectrum disorder for those who are deaf as well as those who are not. Taking research from the CDC, and this just shows some information about prevalence rates, we will say also going back to the plenary this morning rates of other conditions in the deaf and hard-of-hearing tend to be increasing -- more awareness, lots of those things. Regardless of what is contributing to that, we see more kids who appear to have autism and deaf and hard-of-hearing. If you are in the Early Intervention field, you are likely to encounter some of those kids and know how -need to know how to deal with and support them better. Prevalence of autism based on severity of hearing loss. So the tricky thing here, we want to make sure that we don't put out the idea that hearing, reduced hearing causes autism or -- you know, that's not the message. The idea that there is a correlation here. We also know that children who have mild or moderate hearing loss, that that might influence how the diagnosis comes to be assessed or determined. So there are different ways in which that can influence what this picture looks like. But you will see that in the profound range, the number of students who are diagnosed with autism is significantly higher than in the less severe hearing loss ranges. So this fancy slide, Susan and her group get credit for this, but what it shows with the circles, it shows us the age at the hearing loss diagnosis. And then the triangles that are in red show us when autism spectrum disorder was diagnosed. So we see that across these categories, that the hearing loss was diagnosed earlier, the autism spectrum disorder diagnosed later, and not very often at the same time. Sort of figuring that out. In this seminar, special edition, we also had another publication that looked at prevalence rates and found that on average, diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder is like 2 years later than in children who have typical hearing. So there is evidence building that suggests that this is the case. Part of the reason for that is people being uncomfortable with it. If a person specializes in autism and then they are faced with a child who has reduced hearing they don't know how to account for that. So part of our mission is to help educate people in both and say that it is important to know about both because I think that by doing that, it might be one good way to help bring those numbers down and make the diagnosis earlier. So in understanding this dual diagnosis, it is important to recognize the rates are higher within the deaf and hard-of-hearing population. And if we go back to that severity of results, it is not a clear link. It is not that if you have profound hearing loss you will have more profound autism. There is a mixed bag and there is different research that indicates different things in this, particularly with the mild hearing loss we see later delays. Importantly, the delayed diagnosis means delayed intervention which leads to delayed outcomes or reduced outcomes and we sort of see that. So part of why, probably the most significant reason that this is an issue is that it complicates language development, both being deaf and hard-of-hearing and/or having autism spectrum disorder has language implications. As you know, if you have worked in this field, reduced language is one of the criteria. So if we have reduced language for different multiple reasons, it could lead to a more significant issue in that area. All right. So what's the dilemma here? In studies that have worked at the dual diagnosis, in the process of that we are seeing that you can both underdiagnose, miss it, or overdiagnose, and there are implications listed for both of those. If we don't catch it we are missing the opportunity for interventions, or the interventions that are provided may be less. So a child may have reduced hearing, they may receive Early Intervention services only for speech-language therapy, for example, whereas they might need additional things. If we overdiagnose, the supports might not be appropriate. Sometimes certainly in parents, the more the better, bring it on, help us, anything we can get, and sometimes that's good and sometimes if it is not the right or appropriate intervention, then we are not likely to see necessarily good outcomes. So we want to make sure that we are trying to get as close as possible to finding what is an accurate diagnosis for that. Real-life implications also include the fact that there are places like schools for the deaf and other programs where if a child is deaf or hard-of-hearing and has other complications they may not be accepted or might not have the program support to manage what those children need. So there are real-life implications and, you know, certainly some people say it is not that many people. If we look at the overall numbers, we are talking about an increase in numbers and talking about really complicated kids who have particular needs that we want to try to support. So what are those diagnostic challenges? Why is it so difficult? Certainly in looking at our clinical population we could say who diagnoses. In the regular medical facility, perhaps they might have a developmental pediatrician, audiologist, psychologist. They make up the team that makes the diagnosis. But if you have a child who is deaf or hard-of-hearing they may not fit into the services, may be in deaf services and might be completely separate. We lack standardized assessments to make this diagnosis at this point in time. Providers really tend to be trained in autism or trained in deaf. And so finding the people who are interested in generating and growing their skills across those 2 areas is going to be an important thing. Further, some of the people who are working with autism, solely autism may not understand the implications of the language part in working with deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Lots of you folks know this really well, so you are in a good position to help inform that piece, but finding, again, providers who are fluent and proficient in providing both kinds of services can be challenging. We also know that kids act differently in different settings so getting the clear picture. With autism diagnosis, as I mentioned, there is often several providers that are involved in making that diagnostic decision. So when a child presents differently in one setting and then you might think, well, is that because of the reduced communication, if the deaf person who is the professional was working with them and says they seem to communicate in one way and then the person who is on the developmental side who may know less about the hearing loss sees them in a different way, how do they reconcile is it because of hearing loss, autism. And, you know, of course it is a very complicated intermix of those different factors and others. What's going on at that time for the child. If interpreters are involved in the process it can add an additional layer of complication. They may or may not recognize what is a typical language. Interpreters are amazing people who are sometimes really good at taking language that's provided to them in a less cohesive way and making it look really fluent. So it is a challenge because we really need to know what is coming from the child. And so if there isn't a direct path for communication, then that needs to be sort of -- needs to be considered. There is the possibility, if an interpreter is present that it can have (audio skipping), the parents and the providers and how all of that dynamic works, certainly, there is a lot of literature with interpreters and translators and that has an impact when somebody else is in the room. So if there are programs out there that are autism speciality programs and they are looking to grow and expand their services to deaf and hard-of-hearing, that consideration really needs to be about those language things, what role can the interpreter have and how might we support them in understanding how autism presents. So certainly conversations with interpreters in advance if at all possible around, you know, I really want to know exactly what the child is saying or if there are nuances with the sign language perhaps of the child, we need to know about those so that we are getting the full picture of the child. But given the importance of the language being produced by the child and how that relates to the diagnosis, when possible, to have direct communication, it seems like a really good idea. We need to talk about screening, and I'm going to hand it over to Christi. >>CHRISTINE YOSHINAGA-ITANO: So one of the biggest issues is that we are diagnosing these children from the newborn hearing screening with hearing loss, and then they are getting treatment. And you saw that there are more kids in the profound hearing loss category who are getting diagnosed with autism, and they are also getting cochlear implants anywhere between 12 to 15, 18 months of age, right? So this is before anybody would ever -- most people would not be suspecting autism in these newborn babies in the first couple of months of life when all of these decisions are being made about technology. So we have this group of kids now who have been implanted, we have kids who have been fit with hearing aids, and people are beginning to think there is something not quite right about their learning trajectory. They just aren't learning the way we would anticipate. Everything seems to be right. Now, part of the problem is that the screening devices that are out there, most of them are questionnaires, and they have not been done with deaf and hard-of-hearing children. This fact, I think there is only one and it is the -- it is not really deaf or hard-of-hearing, it is hard-of-hearing shall -- it is really kids with otitis media. So they use the baby and infant screen for babies with autism traits and they administer them to children with conductive hearing loss and PE tubes. And they found that if you have just a conductive hearing loss you don't overlap. Now, the problem with this is that we have a lot of kids with profound hearing loss, and they are completely different than our conductive hearing loss population. So it has been frustrating because we don't have an aid for Early Intervention specialists to say, okay, I have a feeling that something is not going right or the parents are saying, is there something else going on. And then we don't have something to help them with this. So we have been trying to figure this out in the state of Colorado, and we have been using a number of different instruments that are available to us. I'm going to talk about the LENA, which has an autism screen designed for hearing children. And it has amazing specificity and sensitivity with hearing children who are later diagnosed as autistic. It is somewhere around the 96, 97 percent right. And I'm going to explain to you what this is. But we have also been using the child development inventory, which we didn't actually know is as sensitive as it is, and I explained that it is a questionnaire. It has 7 developmental areas. It is the social subscale that actually seems to be the most powerful in conjunction with all of the other information that we have. So what we are looking at is who is at risk, right? So we have been doing, for any Early Intervention providers that requested or any parent who request it, we will do a LENA evaluation. And it stands for language environment analysis. It is this new technology, this little thing here. It is about a half of the weight of a cell phone. There are special clothing designed. I don't actually use the overalls, I like the little vests on kids. And it has got a little Velcro pocket so the parents turn it on, they stick it in, it runs for 10 to 16 hours, we tell them to run it all day long and don't stop it because if you stop it, it has to start all over again. So they kind of forget about it. The only time they might take it out is naptime. If they don't want it on the baby they can take it out, keep it running and just lay it by the side of the crib and then put it back when the child is awake. So after they do a day of recording, we either pick it up, they mail it in, we plug it into our computer with software, it dumps in about one to three minutes, and then it starts processing. It takes about, on average, about a 3-hour processing of this 16 hours of audio file. And then it starts spitting out reports. Now, the autism screen is not an automatic screen so you have to additionally request the autism screening. It is not in the basic software. This is basically what it does. It does an automatic processing of the vocal stream. It identifies different sounds within the vocal stream and matches it to the acoustic envelope of an adult production of individual phonemes, consonant vowels, VCs, so it is a fairly complicated paradigm. If the child's vocalization is in the acoustic envelope of one of these things that it is pairing with, it will accept it. Your ear might not accept it, but we are finding that if it has got a good enough use of the frequencies of that particular group, that eventually even if that child is not producing it exactly right, that they are going to go on to have a pretty good production of that phoneme. So it does -- it can separate out the key child, the child who is wearing it, other children, the adult conversational partner, and it separates it into male versus female. So if you have more than one male, it is going to have a hard time. It will think that it is the same conversational partner. If you have -- we have run into a little bit of difficulty with twins because twins sound so much alike and they are sitting -- they can to each other and they talk with the same volume, so twins are a little bit of a difficulty. Even listening to it, sometimes I can't tell which child is the one who is talking. So there are some times when it doesn't work perfectly. It measures the environmental sound, it measures, and this is a key thing for autism, it measures overlapping sounds of what we call colliding sounds. So that is when a child is using vocal production for conversational turn-taking, deaf children, language disordered children, typical developing children, they make a vocal utterance and they wait for their partner to make one, they make one. People with autism are using vocal production not for communication purposes so when they talk they actually collide all the time with their conversational partner. And they have a much higher incidence of this particular aspect of this. So this is kind of what it looks like. It can tell consonant like, vowel type sounds, separates out the nonspeech sounds, sounds that are not moving in the direction of any phoneme in the native language. And pauses and silence. So this is a dataset. The original dataset for the specificity and sensitivity of the LENA for autism of hearing children, they had 106 typically developing, 49 language delayed and 71 children with autism. And I'm just going to show you this -- there are -- I have like 26 -- I think right now there is over 26 variables that are run on the autism screen. This is one of them. And I like to show this because it clearly differentiates these 3 groups. The green is typically hearing, typically developing. And you can see that there is no overlap with the blue group, who are kids with language disorders. All right? So they have speech differences from the typically developing, but their speech with consonant production is significantly different than the red, and the red are the autistic children. So you can see what's really good about some of these features that we are doing is that it is separating out in a very nice way these 3 groups, and on many of the variables that we are testing like colliding speech there is no overlap. All right? I don't know if you can see this as well, but this is throwing in children who are hard-of-hearing. We have not yet thrown in children with cochlear implants. And the purple bars, let's see if I can just -- is this -- no. It is not going to do it. The purple bars and the green bars are almost completely overlapping, which is really good news for us because it says hard-of-hearing kids and their vocal production with their frequency uses, when they are paired to an adult production of phonemes of English language, they look really like hearing children when we are early identifying them and getting them amplification. They are completely different from language disordered children on this, consonants per their utterance. And this is not like speech-language pathologists, this is meant length of utterance, pause that the computer counts as an utterance. And if you see the blue for language disorders that is completely different, and the red for autism, they are completely different. Now, one of the things you should see is this is with autism, is the bar doesn't start until 2, and we think that it actually will be sensitive below 2, but the problem is that most autistic children are not diagnosed until 2. And so since they are not diagnosed until 2 we don't have -- we don't have enough of a control group to be able to say that. So we have now thrown in some kids with profound hearing loss and cochlear implants and I'm going to show you some of this. But this is the child development inventory, a parent questionnaire on social development, and these are 3 cases which we have now replicated about 16 times. It is exactly the same profile. And then that is that the kids are starting out looking not too bad at 14 months, and then 6 months later their quotient you think is dropping by developmental quotient point. And 6 months later, it is dropping by another 10 to 15 quotient points. Why is it happening? Children with autism have different patterns, and one of the patterns is sometimes they regress. They have skills and they lose those skills, right? But a lot of the children in this group didn't lose skills, they failed to gain the new skills which were parent reaction skills. So they were okay when they were just with themselves and with their parents or relatively okay on those things because it is not greatly sensitive for slight differences in parent-child interaction, but once it got to their interaction with peers, the kids were not developing those skills. So if you ever see approval like with a developmental quotient, so this is the social age over the chronological age or cognitive age, whatever you are using as the base, if it starts going down every 6 months, those kids are at extremely high risk for an autism diagnosis, and you should immediately get a referral. So our EI providers weren't comfortable with one indicator because autism and deafness at 2 to 3 years of age and referring, that's really kind of scary. So we also looked at the MacArthur Bates (sp), there are a lot of things, the gestures go down, but you have to -- have gotten them at 9 months, 16 months and 22 months, symbolic gestures, they can't keep up with the symbolic gesture growth of other children. So if you see a decrease in their quotient, that's another indicator. Symbolic play can go down. The problem is none of these instruments in and of themselves are 100 percent with every child no matter where they are in the spectrum. So you need a variety of different tools. Now, this probably is not -- I can say this better. What we are doing is we are combining the Early Intervention, speech pathologist, audiologist input, the professionals working with the kids say, hmm, something is not right. We are looking at the social quotient and whether that's going down, we are looking at symbolic play, symbolic gesture, and then we are doing the LENA. When all of those 3 things combined together are all saying the same thing, we are referring. And what's happened, is that it has been an incredibly effective screen and we are being able to identify our babies somewhere between 2 and a half and 3 and a half years of age and get them into the appropriate Early Intervention. And that's incredibly early for us, but I can tell you that when we have been able to do that, when we have the Early Intervention combined with our autism team and doing some really nice interventions, which we will talk about later, I'm going way over -- but that it is incredibly effective. So we have mainstreamed now the first 2 early diagnosed deaf-autistic kids into a regular kindergarten. And they are doing really amazingly well. If you had asked me if that would have ever happened, I don't know if we can ever do this, but there is all kinds of possibilities. This is written up in this article and you might want to see it in the seminars -- do we have 8 or 9 articles? Something like that. There is a lot. The whole edition is all on autism and deafness. So there is 2 different articles on what I just said. Seminars in speech and language. And then November of 2014 -- yeah. The whole edition is on autism and deafness. >> Christi was a special guest editor and all of the articles are related to this topic and of interest. So you should certainly check it out. And we can make our own available to you if you contact us. So you can certainly send emails and we can make sure you have access to those. We are going to talk about the red flags, which I think are really key and I think are -- that's a lot of information. So we are going to talk about them sort of cursory and encourage you to seek out further information from the article, if you can. So red flags. How do we know if there is an atypical behavior that we see, how do we know if that's pointing toward autism or pointing toward deaf or hard-of-hearing characteristics, perhaps. So we are going to look in these areas. Atypical preverbal communication, things like eye contact, poor orientation, poor (off mic). This is ASD. Delays in language that are beyond what might be expected by reduced access to language, ineffective or inappropriate supports for auditory access, those kinds of things. If you are in the field and you know this stuff, you know that there are some kids who present at 2 years of age and you sort of say, well, they got diagnosed a little late, here is what happened, it was unclear, so some of these delays might really be attributed to that process of their hearing status. And there are other times you say, even given all that we know about hearing loss, that can't be it. There is something else. The next category is atypical language features. Things like echolalia. Talks about palm rotation, individualized instead of saying bye. And then they self-effect. Kids who are on the spectrum and who have (off mic) don't correct. So there are things like that that we can look for as sort of an indication to see which path they might be on, or if it is both. So social difficulties. Things like having difficulty initiating with peers, even when we take into consideration communication challenges. So you have a child who is deaf or hard-of-hearing, hasn't been around a lot of hearing peers, you put them in a novel situation, we would expect that that might be a situation and we wouldn't attribute to it something else. But given our understanding of all those other things, if social interactions continue to be challenging and you continue to see difficulties or shying away, the prevalence rates that -- I don't mean shying away, avoiding those kinds of social interactional skills or when placed in them, a child is very in tune to what it is that he or she is doing, those could be indicators as well. Restrictive behaviors and interests that you see, which are typically associated with kids on the spectrum. So in terms of deficits in social and communication and social interactional skills, part of this is going to rely on your reading abilities. And because I can't go through all of them, but we have highlighted, if it is typical, typical autism spectrum, here is what we see. Difficulties with back-and-forth conversations, limited enjoyment or willingness to engage in social interactions. In typically developing deaf and hard-of-hearing children we see wanting to engage. There might be difficulties in engaging, might have been less role modeling and skills, facilitation in how to make that happen, but the desire is different. Eye contact, too, in kids who are deaf and hard-of-hearing tends to be good. They tend to be seeking out information visually, whether they are talking about modality, I'm not talking about signers, but any child with reduced hearing, their visual hearing, increased scanning, for example, is increased. That could be an indication that it is this way. And we should note that there is a whole bunch of these, so it is not like if you are thinking of a particular child and you say, they do that, they must be autistic or that must be just because of their it really is a combination of Christi alluded to, there are that go into determining this diagnosis. hearing status, things. And as many factors autistic But in the social realm, here is what we have highlighted. Here is a little bit more deficits, reciprocity. So the first 2 columns say autism spectrum, and then the final one says in kids who have both. In the kids who are deaf and hard-of-hearing and on the spectrum, this is what we think it looks like. This table is both on research as well as clinical experience. People who have worked in the population, here is what we see, this is what leads to that. This is a summary paper as opposed to the whole thing doesn't say this is what the research says, but it is also formed by clinical impressions and emerging and developing sort of gut feelings about how to sort these things out. Because before I set, you know, we are thinking is it autism or is it deaf or hard-of-hearing and the reason you are here is to talk about when kids have both, what does that look like, you will see that for each of the categories sort of going forward. Things like difficulties with direct attention, not responding to one's name, even making sure that we are getting everybody's attention and that they have access to that. This might look like that. Those kids who have both. Difficulties understanding other's needs and feelings as well as processing facial expressions and gestures and those motions. So that's an important piece, that's an overlay that people who are in either disciplines may be less familiar with. The next category of communicative behavior, this is what we see in typical autism, things like reduced eye contact, differences in body language, difficulties processing nonverbal information, whereas kids who are deaf or hard-of-hearing and only deaf or hard-of-hearing we see a lot more of that lighter use of facial expression and access to those things. So it is so much more engaged, more integrated, more use of eye contact and social exchange than in kids who have both. We see a little bit more limited gestures. Even those kids who rely on gestures and signed communication, we see limited use of them in exploration, expression of their thoughts. Reduced pointing for things, abnormal prosody. Poor understanding of the integrated ASL facial and grammatical features. That's one of the things we wrote about. So if you are relying on that as your means of communication, we would expect you to sort of pick up those things and so kids who are on the spectrum have more difficulty with that. So when we talk about prosody, those are the social rules that govern social exchanges. Many people have an inkling of what that looks like in kids who are on the -- kids who are deaf and hard-of-hearing, and on autism spectrum it might look a little bit different. We may have those responses to particular questions, but the way in which it is produced and understanding of how you are asking those questions using facial grammatical features would be different, too. All right. So some of the features of autism spectrum that we see in ASL. Palm reversals. So that's the bye-bye. Pronoun avoidance or pronoun reversal. Instead of I, some will refer to themselves as their own name. If you are engaging with them, what about you? Yes, you. Referring to themselves. And holding on to the "you," meaning me. And we have some -- there is some research that has been done by Erin Shield (sp), and we had some evidence of that. It is an interesting thing to watch if you have the opportunity. Continued use of one's own gestures despite being taught how to do something differently. One of the examples is continuing to sign red instead of ketchup. Even if this is how you do, here the context for red, it means something else, yes, ketchup is red, but -and you will see kids who are on the spectrum and deaf who use sign, continue to use those things over and over again. It is difficulties in correcting. The last one, the mixed results regarding facial expressions, there is a researcher who is doing some work on that, and it seems to be sort of inconsistent. There are ways in which kids who have both, autism and are deaf or hard-of-hearing, seem to follow social norms in the expression of their ASL, and yet not understand the social implications of that, if that makes sense. Does that make sense? Okay. All right. For the next category, difficulties in developing and maintaining appropriate relationships. So difficulties adjusting, being really rigid, difficulties making friends, all right? And kids who are typically developing deaf and hard-of-hearing, we see lots more flexibility, interest in making friends, desire to do that, interested in people who are of the same age. In the population that includes both, here is a little bit of what we see. Play continues to be really rigid, less imaginative. Unusual social overtones. Grunting at peers, hitting them, not engaging despite any natural consequences we might place on them. Lots of kids who are in the early preschool years really want to be liked by their teachers, and in situations like this they may care less about that or be less aware of that. How are we doing? Okay. With restrictive patterns, I think walking and (off mic). Those are often associated with ASD. In typically deaf hard-of-hearing kids we don't see that. If there are reduced cognitive ability, those kids who have reduced IQ you might see behaviors that can be more repetitive. Echolalia in typically developing, but that's like practicing it out, you know. Little kids do that, too, they copy you, and they are looking to see how you are responding. And if you start to say yes, that's right, baa, that means bottle and you make -- so typically developing deaf and hard-of-hearing might be copying what you are saying in order to try to understand. It can also be a sign of reduced comprehension. When I'm engaging with the deaf/hard-of-hearing, I may say is that what often gets them the head nod. That's all we are demanding, copying as opposed to clear understanding of what they know. Similar to what has been mentioned before with the palm rotation and echolalia and rocking and twisting and -- children (off mic) children have both. Strict routines. Typically developing kids generally are more flexible with that, although they may struggle with things like transitions. What we have seen in the school situation very often teachers will say these kids really have difficulties when we are trying to transition and that might be because of us not doing a good enough job telling us this is what's happening next. So that's an example of where you might see some rigidity. You might think, is that spectrumy behavior. But we have to think about the context, the level of understanding, the clarity we are providing to children who are deaf or hard of hearing that result in the behaviors that they are showing us. In the population that is actually diagnosed we might see the -- (off mic) done in the same way, at the same schedule, lots of resistance to change. Particularly if the communication -- there are communication gaps or communication is not smooth between child or providers, might see incidence of that increased or amplified. Typically developing deaf kids don't have necessarily -- they don't get stuck on being fixated or highly ritualistic. We can see sensitivities in autistic, highly sensitive to touch or certain textures or things that are too hot or too cold. Some kids who have reduced hearing have sensitivities, too, and might be related to other conditions they have. So they may be deaf and have a reason, an etiology for that that causes sensitivity. But in the combined population, we see more of that. Okay. Other diagnostic considerations. I'm going to hand it over at this point to Dr. Dr. Susan Wiley. >>SUE WILEY: Mine should be titled clear as mud, right? In hearing kids this isn't always as easy, and so there are a number of things that overlap. And when we think about it, it is a behavioral checklist, so where these behaviors are coming from or stemming from can vary from kid to kid. So I have seen a number of kids 2 to 3, I'm very worried about an autism spectrum and at 5 or 6 I go, wow, that was just language and OCD or something, you know. Or there was something driving that phenotype or what you are seeing in the environment, but it is really driven by something different qualitatively than autism spectrum. Because it is really pervasive, i.t is across settings and, you know, we see life-long impacts of that. So it can be challenging when kids have an intellectual disability or communication disorder because you have delayed communication, but it shouldn't be atypical or asymmetric. So I talk a lot about asymmetry in language and it kind of lines up with Christi's screening stuff of social communication or our play, is that markedly different than our language is, a lot more worried about that kid. But children with intellectual disabilities have a slower rate of progress, so if I have a child who is functioning across the board at 9 months I'm not going to see pretend play yet because that's just not developmentally there yet. So we have to take into that whole framework around where a child's functioning. There is a lot of behavioral things that can really mimic autism spectrum. ADHD might be impulsive, may not recognize personal space, they may act before they think, and/or they may not attend very well because my brain is going 20 places so I'm not attending to the language. And there are a number of kids who are hearing with ADD who have a little bit of language lag and then kind of catch up because they just weren't paying attention. But that ADHD in kids who are deaf and hard-of-hearing can be tricky, too, to sort that out. Anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorders are really challenging to tease out, and this is where if you have an anxiety issue or more obsessive-compulsive traits and language impairment, you can really look a lot like autism. Because I may be socially anxious, I may not want to engage because I'm nervous how that might look. There are kids with childhood OCD, it is not easy to figure out, but extremely rigid and I have have to have the blue -- juice in my blue cup and my milk in the orange cup. If it doesn't, it just bothers me. And you can talk to adults who say if I don't tap 3 times I feel really anxious inside. A little kid can't tell you that. So there is a lot of overlap at that point. There are some other medical conditions that can kind of mimic and imitate kids with autism spectrum. So a lot of people with Tourette's syndrome, with tic disorders, so that's a disorder where you have both a motor and develop tic, so motor tic, anything from a blinking, an eye raise, a little bit of shoulder shrug. And a vocal tic is usually you have got to have something that phonates, so it might be a throat clearing or a sniff, even a sniff can be a tic, so people often think, do you have allergies. But what comes along with Tourette's can be ADHD, obsessive-compulsive disorder. So again, these folks with Tourette's can look a little different in how they present. There is a specific EEG abnormality, it is very rare called Randall Clevinger. And they seem to be doing okay and have a period of regression. So you may have kids with regressive form, these kids will tend to be a little bit older, but they really have a more aphasic quality to them. So they want to engage and communicate but really struggle with formulating and processing our information. I bring up peripheral vision cues because sometimes kids with Usher syndrome are stand-offish to their peers because they are not seeing the cues of people trying to get their attention from the side. So I said hi to you and you blew me off, and how rude is that. If we are looking at joint attention, what is the kid's visual field, is vision actually an aspect of something we need to realize, that they may not be seeing what we are talking about and, therefore, you are not going to look toward that information. And then benign stereopodies. The flapping and the excitement. So one of my -- (off mic). One of my favorite pictures of my nephew is at like 3 years old going down the slide like this. All that flaps is not autism. And so he doesn't have autism. I mean, we might question other things, but not that. And then sensory integration difficulty, I bring that up because a lot of kids have sensory issues and my framework is a little bit different than an OT. I see that overlapping most frequently with 3 conditions, one being autism, so I'm tactically defensive, sensory seeking, whatever, you know, the numerous ways we can have sensory processing challenges. So autism is clearly one, but kids with ADHD can also have sensory issues in -- not knowing how their motor runs, kind of revved up, and then kids with anxieties and OCD, don't want you to touch them, these are the things where I see overlap more frequently and so just, you know, it is not always autism. So good luck. (Laughter). But we are going to talk, the next section we are going to talk more about interventions and hopefully that will give some guidance if you think you have somebody who has it. >> What we know about interventions is that we don't know that much. What we do know that has been published is based on case studies. If you refer back to the plenary this morning, we can take what we think works, try it with the deaf and hard of hearing population, and you can't just start from stratch, we do need to take what we know is out there now. All right. So we definitely need to consider multifaceted approaches to language. One of the issues in kids who are dually diagnosed is language and we need to make sure that we are making the information and the intervention appropriate for them and accessible to them. We need to understand that the way that they both express themselves and the way they understand receptively can be different and so we need to make sure we are making adjustments for those things and that we are targeting those core symptoms, sort of have been addressed in the red flags side, we are making sure we are addressing those and doing it in a way that is culturally and appropriately sensitive to children who have reduced hearing levels. So features of autism that might impact both language acquisition. Joint attention, reduced eye contact, also that motor imitation piece, we need to sort of be aware of those when we are thinking about how do we adjust communication for dually diagnosed kids. It might be important to try to see what is that a symptom of. Perhaps the child has cochlear implants and they are not working right now and part of the reason they are not responding on this particular day, it is not functioning like it should. So we might take a child who is deaf or hard-of-hearing, put them in an autism related program and unless they are sort of at least attune to those possibilities, they might not notice that. Sensitivities, as discussed by Dr. Wiley, might lead to a child not wanting to use their gear. They might want to -- they might refuse it, might be uncomfortable to them. So that's another way we might have to adjust communication for this population. >> We should have organized these a little bit better. I actually want to give credit to (off mic), who helped co-author one of the articles. This sounds like an infomercial. She participated in the focus group we did, and also helped co-author the article on how to support families. And I think, you know, I have been really struck by how families say thank you for helping me get in the right direction. I knew things weren't working, know, did I want to hear my kid might have autism spectrum, absolutely not, but got me on the right path. But we don't want to delay raising concerns, and it is easy to kind of want to hold off and try different things, but that really just slows us down. And a lot of the role of the deaf supports and EHDI programs are really to help families understand what is different or what, you know, what we usually see in kids and what we are seeing differently in this particular child, but I think also it is really important whenever we are working with families to start with strengths, you know. That's kind of a no-brainer. And then move into those areas of concern. But with a kindness, and I think when you work with families a while who trust you, they are more ready to hear that. And, you know, I think the other really important piece is there is lots and lots of investigation out there on the web, and you can screen things and you can get pretty scared. So helping families sort through all of that this was and what fits their child and what doesn't. So one of my very common conversations with families is you are going to read a lot and some of it is going to fit and some of it is not. Don't even accept, don't go where it doesn't fit. It is okay to not have to think in that direction. But they can use professionals to kind of be that thermometer of what's going to fit. And then try to find resources. And this is really challenging. There is just a lot of resources out there. But families seek information and we should help them in that regard and also try to get that family to family support through -- that we know is helpful for all families. I will put a little plug in for Hands & Voices, has a deaf plus area on their website, and one of the challenges is that even though you can connect families, they are pretty busy with children's needs, particularly deaf plus. So that can make it a little bit more challenging just to even have the mental energy and strength to, you know, connect and reach out. One other statement on that is sometimes we can give families information and say call them or reach out to them, but not every family is comfortable doing that. So, you know, just because we give information, that may not be the personal approach that one family may have. >> I'm going to actually -- we are running out of time, but let me just say, Christine covered this. One of the really great things about the interventions with children who are deaf and autistic is the interdisciplinary nature, and there are some things that have really, really good efficacy. And I want to actually point to this early start Denver model, which was designed by Sally Rogers, I don't know how many of you are in the -- have covered Early Interventions for children with autism, but I was fortunate enough to be starting in Denver when -- with the Early Intervention program back in the 1970s, and Sally Rogers was starting the early start Denver model, model techniques that she is now sadly no longer this Colorado, but she has just gone on to do wonderful things with. And one of the things that has been really helpful is that when we had a child who was autistic, she would consult with us and help us design Early Intervention therapies for these children, and we had this -- actually, the first child she consulted on was a hard-of-hearing child who was on the spectrum and we could not -- the child could hear everything, but we could not get spoken language going. No matter what kind of auditory skills we were doing. The mom decided that maybe she should try sign language and maybe she could get some communication going with sign language. We could not get him to imitate a sign. He would not look at our faces, he would not look at the imitation. We were just completely stuck. The auditory was going nowhere. We were trying visual to visual, it was going nowhere. I'm sure you have kids like this that you work with. Sally helped us develop a plan where we started out with the sign language, we started out actually behind, sitting him on our lap and we only chose signs for which he didn't have to look at our face. Right? And so they were from the neck down, basically. So more, ball, milk, those kinds of signs. And then we were behind him moving his hands with the things in front of him. So there was a ball in front and we would move his hands for ball, there was milk, and we would move his hands for milk, we would do want every time and then his parent would give him -- and we would basically be giving him tactile support for doing the signs. And he immediately started signing. So we were able to move him from going behind him to moving to the side so he would look to the side when we started doing the signs for the imitation to gradually being able to actually have a communication. He wasn't looking at our face, he was looking at the sign. And the exciting thing was we were able to move him up with every game that he made in communication so that he became a spoken language communicator. So we used the sign as a facilitator to get the spoken language going. And it turned out that he in addition also had oral apraxia, so part of the reason we were not getting the speech going is because -- but you know what? At 2 you don't have really good tests for oral apraxia and then a child who doesn't have any speech or language plan and won't imitate anything at all. Right? You can't get any spontaneous vocal production. So he was really a success story for us, but it took this kind of collaboration. Sally didn't know anything about deafness, we didn't know anything about autism, and there were a lot of videotapes and observations, but this is the kind of thing that you want to develop. You want an interdisciplinary team. We are now using her early start Denver model. Because if you are fortunate enough to diagnose kids very early, this model is really a wonderful model. It basically gets everybody who is doing intervention, no more than 3 well-defined goals. And those 3 well-defined goals are for at least a 6-week treatment with about 15 to 16 hours of treatment a day -- a week. And so what's nice about this, the early start model, it is based on home intervention with the parent as the driver. So the parent actually manages the program with the autistic specialist and in our case with our EI provider. And what they do is they come up with what their most pressing need is, and so then whatever the number one thing is, that's where the team sits and works out what do we need to do to get the child to not do whatever he is doing, biting, kicking or, I mean, it can be really at a very low level in the beginning. Every therapist working with the child, so the EI therapist, the audiologist who, if they are doing any follow-through with hearing, the physical therapists, a lot of times these kids have multiple people, but they all work on the same goal. And they all -- so the physical therapist does it with the physical therapy, speech therapist does with speech therapy. If they are fortunate to have other things, the mom then supplements whatever the professionals are doing for the extra hours. So she would be able to develop the lesson plan for how she is going to carry it on, and she does it intensively for 6 days a week, at least 3 hours a day. But it is doable. She can figure out, you know, I can't do 3 hours all day long, but I can do morning an hour, afternoon an hour, and I can do an evening an hour, if it is not the witching hour, right? And I don't have to worry about Tuesdays because Tuesdays, at least an hour and a half of it is with the Early Intervention provider and they are going to supplement whatever the goals are. We have actually had some really positive improvement with the kids using this. It is a highly structured ABA intervention, but at the beginning actually with a lot of these children because they have no communication at all, that's exactly what you need and things get -- as they progress, they learn faster. We have a group of autism specialists who are learning more about deafness. We have Debbie Mood, who unfortunately couldn't come, psychologist trained at Gallaudet, she does all the ADOS (sp) diagnoses. In the seminars article, if you have a psychologist that doesn't know very much about deafness or has some knowledge, I would have no knowledge. But ADOS, adapt it for deaf and hard-of-hearing children, and you need trained psychologists to work with you. Otherwise, we can't get the diagnosis, right? There are more people who are early start Denver model, there is one for parents, one for professionals, and there is a training program for professionals to get trained in the early start Denver model. There are also many other autism techniques, and I completely went over. So we are at time. Just wanted to leave you with at least some things on intervention that you could take home. Sorry. >>AMY SZARKOWSKI: I have some copies of the summary article. We can also go down to the -AUDIENCE MEMBER: (off mic). >>AMY SZARKOWSKI: The first one is Seminars in Speech and Language, special edition, and there is the website information. This is posted for -- on the EHDI website so you can access it there. And I have some paper copies. I have 20 paper copies of that article. First come, first serve on those, and these are other options for resources for you. We are happy to stay and answer a few questions. Thank you so much for coming. (Applause) (Session concluded)