Change from baseline or analysis of covariance?: Lord's Paradox and other matters. Stephen Senn (C) Stephen Senn 2004 1 Outline • Adjustment in Randomised Clinical Trials – The argument for ANCOVA • Lord’s Paradox – ANCOVA versus simple analysis of change scores (SACS) • Observational studies – The argument against ANCOVA • Resolution? – Why ANCOVA although not perfect may be best after all (C) Stephen Senn 2004 2 SACS and ANCOVA A simple randomised clinical trial in which there are two treatment groups and only two measurements per patient: a baseline measurement, X and an outcome measurement, Y. Popular choices of outcome measure are 1) raw outcomes Y 2) change score d = Y - X 3) covariance adjusted outcomes Y - X. (where is chosen appropriately) NB As Laird (Am Stat., 37, 329-330, 1983) has shown, covariate adjusted change scores are the same as 3) (C) Stephen Senn 2004 3 The Estimators Associated with the Measures If subscript t stands for treatment and c for control we have: 1) ˆraw Yt Yc 2) ˆchange Yt Yc X t X c 3) ˆ Yt Yc X t X c 1) and 2) are just special cases of 3). If is chosen to be the regression of Y on X, then 3) corresponds to analysis of covariance. (C) Stephen Senn 2004 4 Warning • These three measures, measure the same thing • No question of choosing between them on the basis of clinical relevance • Can only choose between them on the basis either of variance, or statistical philosophy • ANCOVA may generally be expected to have the lowest variance • Baseline is irrelevant to the definition of the treatment effect. (C) Stephen Senn 2004 5 V a r i a n c e s f o r T h r e e A p p r o a c h e s t o A n a l y s i s 0 . 5 2 . 0 1 . 6 c h a n g e _ s c o r e o u t c o m e s A N C O V A varince 1 . 2 0 . 8 0 . 4 0 . 0 0 . 0 0 . 2 0 . 4 0 . 6 0 . 8 1 . 0 c o r r e l a t i o n c o e f f i c i e n t (C) Stephen Senn 2004 6 ANCOVA and Baseline by Treatment Interaction • It is often stated that ANCOVA relies on the parallelism assumption. • This is not true. • If the effect of treatment varies with baseline it varies whether or not ANCOVA is used. • ANCOVA is a first approximation and better than either doing nothing or using change scores. (C) Stephen Senn 2004 7 Not to use ANCOVA, because you fear parallelism may not apply, is like saying crossing the channel in a rowing boat is dangerous I prefer to swim”. (C) Stephen Senn 2004 8 Dichotomania • Obsessive compulsive disorder – Cochrane Collaboration has a galloping case • Numbers Needed to Treat should have been strangled at birth • Division of patients into sheep and goats – Ignoring existence of geep and shoats • Use of difference from baseline – Sin number one • Destruction of information • Arbitrary division into responders non-responders – Sin number two • Further destruction of information • Unjustified causal interpretation (C) Stephen Senn 2004 9 A Red Herring • It is sometimes claimed that measurement error invalidates ANCOVA – The reason is that if baseline is measured with error the regression of outcome on baseline is attenuated • However this claim is incorrect • ANCOVA is still valid – The reason is that it is appropriate to correct for an observed imbalance using an observed regression (C) Stephen Senn 2004 10 Counter-Claims • There is a significant minority of papers arguing against ANCOVA as a means of dealing with bias – E.g. Liang and Zeger (2000), Sankyha, Samuelson (1986), American Statistician • The variance claims are accepted • Claims are made that ANCOVA is biased unless there is balance at baseline (C) Stephen Senn 2004 11 Justification of the Counter-Claim E X c c E X t t c E Yc c c E Yt t t c t c c Hence EX t X c E Yt YC E Yt YC X t X c ( ) E Yt YC X t X c ( ) 1 (C) Stephen Senn 2004 12 Lord’s Paradox • Lord, F.M. (1967) “ A paradox in the interpretation of group comparisons”, Psychological Bulletin, 68, 304-305. “A large university is interested in investigating the effects on the students of the diet provided in the university dining halls….Various types of data are gathered. In particular the weight of each student at the time of his arrival in September and his weight in the following June are recorded” (C) Stephen Senn 2004 13 Two Statisticians Statistician One • Calculates difference in weight for each hall • Finds non-significant difference in each case • (Also no difference between halls) Statistician Two • Adjust for initial weight • Finds significant hall effect • Concludes difference between halls (C) Stephen Senn 2004 14 A Simulated Example • Starting and final weights for two groups of students • Males and females • 300 In each group • Analysis illustrated with S Plus (C) Stephen Senn 2004 15 100 80 Final Weight 60 40 f f m m m m mm mm m m m m m m mm m m m m mm mm m m m m m m m m mmmmmmmmmmm m mm m m m mm m mm m m m m mmm mm m m m m m mm m mm m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m fmm m m mm m mm m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m mm m m m mm m m m m m m m mm m m mm m mm m m mm mm m mm m m m m m f m m m m m m f m m m m m mm fm mm mm m mf m f m m m f m m m m m m m m m m m m f mm m mm f mm mm m f m m m m m m fm ff mm m fm m m mm m f m m m m f m f m m m m f mf f mfm fm f f ffmm f f fm f f m m f f f m f m f mm f fff mf mf fm m fm ff f m fm f ffff ff fffm f f f f fff ffffffffffffffffm ff ffffffm f f f f f fmf ff fffffm f ff ffffffff ffffffffffffffff f ff fff f f f f f f fffff f f f f f f f ffffffffffffffffff fffffffffffff f f f ffffffffffffffffff f fffff fff f f f ff f f f fff fff ff ff f f ff f ff f f ff f f 40 60 80 100 Initial Weight (C) Stephen Senn 2004 16 Statistician One’s Analysis Paired t-Test data: Y.males and X.males t = 0.662, df = 299, p-value = 0.5085 Paired t-Test data: Y.females and X.females t = -0.0512, df = 299, p-value = 0.9592 Standard Two-Sample t-Test data: diff.males and diff.females t = 0.5017, df = 598, p-value = 0.6161 (C) Stephen Senn 2004 17 100 80 Final Weight 60 40 f f m m m m mm mm m m m m m m mm m m m mm mm mm m mm mmmmmm mm m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m mm mmm m mm m m mm mm m mm m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m mm m m m fmm m m mm m mm m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m mm m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m mmmm mm m mm m m m m m m f m m m m m f m m m m m fm mm mm m mf m m mfm m f mm m m m m m m m mm m fm m m f m m m fm m m m m m m f ff mm m m m m m f mfm m m mm fm m fmm fmm m f mm m ffm fm f m f m f f fm fm f ff m m fm f ffm fm f m f ffffm f m m ff f m fff fffm f m m f f f f f f f f f f fff fffffffffffffffm f f f m f f f f f f f f f f ff fm f f f ff f fmfffffffffff fff ffffffffffffff f ff ff f f f f f f f f f f f f f f f f f ffffffffffffffffff fffffffffffff f f f ffffffffffffffffff f fffff fff f f f ff f f f fff fff ff ff f ff f ff f f f ff f f 40 60 80 100 Initial Weight (C) Stephen Senn 2004 18 Statistician Two’s analysis Call: lm(formula = Y ~ X + sex) Coefficients: Value Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|) (Intercept) 12.4987 1.5135 8.2584 0.0000 X 0.8087 0.0232 34.9156 0.0000 sex 1.9816 0.2833 6.9945 0.0000 (C) Stephen Senn 2004 19 What People Usually Conclude • Where baseline values are not expected to be equal between groups ANCOVA can mislead • Therefore even though SACS will have a higher variance it should be preferred under such circumstances since it is obviously unbiased (C) Stephen Senn 2004 20 A Counter Counter-Example • Suppose we design a bizarre clinical trial • Only person with diastolic blood pressure at baseline equal to 95mmHg or 105mmHg may enter • In the first stratum they are randomised 3 to 1 and in the second 1 to 3 • Situation as follows (C) Stephen Senn 2004 21 A Stupid Trial Numbers of Patients by dbp and Treatment Treatment Baseline diastolic blood pressure Total A B Total 95mm Hg 300 100 400 105 mmHg 100 300 400 400 400 800 (C) Stephen Senn 2004 22 Approach to Analysis • Stratify by baseline dbp • Produce treatment estimate for each stratum • Overall estimate is average of the two • Stratification deals with the imbalance (C) Stephen Senn 2004 23 An Equivalent Approach • Create dummy variable stratum S = -1 if baseline dbp, X = 95mmHg S = 1 if baseline dbp, X =105 mmHg • Regress dbp at outcome, Y, on treatment indicator T and on stratum indicator S • Estimate will be same as by stratification • If you want variance estimate to be exactly the same you need to include interaction also (C) Stephen Senn 2004 24 An Equivalent Equivalent Approach • Regress Y on T and X rather than on T and S – This is called ANCOVA! • Note that S= (X-100)/5 • Hence this approach is equivalent to the previous one, which is equivalent to stratification, which is unbiased • On the other hand SACS is biased • Hence we have produced a counter-example (C) Stephen Senn 2004 25 Conclusion • Contrary to what is often claimed there are cases where ANCOVA is unbiased but SACS is biased. • No simple statement of the form “ANCOVA is more efficient but SACS is unbiased” is possible. • In fact it is very difficult to imagine cases where SACS is the preferred analysis (C) Stephen Senn 2004 26 Lord’s Paradox Revisited • Statistician one assumes that in the absence of any differential treatment effect the two groups despite different baselines would show equivalent changes • Statistician two assumes that in the absence of any differential treatment effect the change of the groups as a whole is the same as the change within groups • Both of these causal assumptions are untestable (C) Stephen Senn 2004 27 However • It is easy to design trials for which – ANCOVA is unbiased – SACS is biased – A causal interpretation can be given • It is rather difficult to design trials for which – SACS is unbiased – ANCOVA is biased – A causal interpretation can be given (C) Stephen Senn 2004 28 The Necessary Condition for ANCOVA to be Unbiased E Yt YC X t X c E Yt YC X t X c E Yt YC Or in everyday language that the bias in the raw comparison at outcome should be times the bias at baseline where is the individual regression effect (C) Stephen Senn 2004 29 Cut-off Designs Trochim and Capelleri have suggested that in many clinical trials randomisation will be unethical because some patients by the nature of their illness may be unwilling to assume the risks associated with an experimental treatment. They propose a class of designs called “cut-off” designs in which some patients are assigned to treatment in a deterministic manner on the basis of baseline values. The position, for example, might be as given in the diagram below. (C) Stephen Senn 2004 30 A Cut-off Design in Hypertension Standard treatment only Randomise to Experimental standard or treatment only experimental Mild hypertension Moderate hypertension (C) Stephen Senn 2004 Severe hypertension 31 Cut off Designs • Provided that the relationship between baseline and outcome is linear ANCOVA is valid • Cut off designs are thus a wide class of design for which ANCOVA is unbiased • SACS will be biased • Thus we have more counterexamples to the claims of Liang and Zeger (C) Stephen Senn 2004 32 A Challenge • Can you design a trial for which – SACS is unbiased – ANCOVA is biased – A causal interpretation can be given? (C) Stephen Senn 2004 33 Some Schemes That won’t Work • Select patient according to true baseline values – Not possible in practice since not known – Still won’t work since correlation of true values is not 1 • Select patients according to average of values at baseline and outcome – You need a crystal ball • Select according to some other value – ANCOVA will be biased but so will SACS • Select on binary covariate – Either this is permanent (e.g. sex), in which case causal inference doubtful – Or it varies over time in which case there will be a regression (C) Stephen Senn 2004 34 Conclusion • In RCTs ANCOVA is the appropriate way to use baseline information – SACS, responder analysis, NNTs all wasteful • A hallmark of second rate analysis • In observational studies things are more complex – ANCOVA may not be perfect but it may be the best you can do (C) Stephen Senn 2004 35 Here there be tygers! (C) Stephen Senn 2004 36