1 Thymophor in dreams, poetry, art and memory: Emotion translated into imagery as a basic element of human creativity Ernest Hartmann MD Professor of Psychiatry, Tufts University School of Medicine Keywords: dream, dreaming, poetry, art, memory, thymophor Contact Ernest Hartmann MD 27 Clark Street, Newton, MA 02459 ehdream@aol.com 2 Abs: The word thymophor (from thymo: emotion and phor: carrying over) is introduced here. It refers to the carrying-over, or translation, of emotion into imagery, which we find is an important element in human creativity. This paper starts with dreaming: my collaborators and I have studied the Central Image (CI) of the dream over many years, and shown that the CI is driven by, and in the simplest case pictures, the underlying emotion of the dreamer. This is an instance of thymophor. The center of a poem, which TS Eliot calls the Objective Correlative, play the same role: it pictures the underlying emotion. We go on to find thymophor in painting, music, and other arts . Thymophor appears to be a basic element in our creative activity. This paper then discusses metaphor, a basic part of human thought, occurring everywhere, though especially prominently in dreams and poetry. We find that metaphor is understandable only when emotion is considered: emotion chooses or drives the metaphor to be used: thus, again, thymophor, a picturemetaphor driven by and carrying the emotion. A brief experiment in autobiographical memory is presented, suggesting that the moments that leap out in memory are recalled as imagery, but imagery that is driven by heightened emotion, once more: thymophor. Finally, an outline of the probable brain biology of thymophor is discussed. ……………… 3 Introduction This paper will discuss a number of approaches to a basic element of human creativity, the translation of emotion into imagery, which we call “thymophor.” Much of our experimental work has dealt with dreaming. Though some consider dreams to be confused nonsense, my collaborators and I have concluded that every dream is a creative product, and that the dream – especially the central imagery of the dream -- depends on turning emotion into imagery .+ We can arrive at thymophor from a number of different starting points. Starting with the dream Thus we will start with dreaming. A whole series of our studies on the nature and functions of dreaming began with the Tidal Wave Dream. I’m walking along a beach with a friend, I’m not sure who, when suddenly a huge wave, maybe forty feet high, sweeps us away. I struggle and struggle in the water. I’m not sure whether I’ll make it to the surface. Then I wake up. This dream, or something very like it, is common in people who have recently experienced a trauma of any kind (Barrett 1996, Hartmann 1998, Hartmann et al, 1998, Hartmann et al., 2001;). I have heard it from victims of rape or attempted rape, victims of attacks, from people whose close relatives or friends were killed or attacked, and from people who have barely escaped from a burning house or car. My associates and I consider this dream especially important, in fact paradigmatic, because it lets us see so clearly what is going on. The dream does not picture the actual traumatic experience – the burning house or the rape. It pictures the powerful emotion of the dreamer – “I am terrified. I am overwhelmed.” Similar tidal wave dreams have been reported after many kinds of trauma, for instance after a devastating fire in Oakland, CA by Siegel (1996). The dream image is not always literally a tidal wave; we have examples of images such as being swept away by a whirlwind, or being chased off a cliff by a gang. 4 Most dreams of course are not so straightforward. The simple picturing of an emotional state seems to occur most when there is a single powerful emotion present, as in someone who has just been traumatized. Terror is perhaps the most straightforward emotion in these situations, but there are others, which are also pictured in dreams, for instance vulnerability. Here are some dreams in which vulnerability appear to be pictured: I dreamt of a small animal lying in the road bleeding. Several of us were wandering around on a huge plain. There was no shelter. There was rain beating down on us. We had no place to go. We were all lost and helpless. There were shellfish creatures, like lobsters or crayfish, lying there with their shells torn off, all white and pink and very exposed. Guilt, especially survivor guilt, is a very powerful emotion, often pictured in dreams. For instance, a man who escaped from a burning house, in which a family member had died, dreamt: I dreamt of a fire somewhere, in a house very different from our actual house. In the dream my brother and everyone else escaped, but I was still in the house getting burned when I woke up. Sadness or mourning is also frequently portrayed very clearly. Here are dreams from two different dreamers in the week after their mothers’ deaths. There was an empty house, empty and barren, the furniture all gone. All the doors and windows were open and the wind was blowing through. A huge tree has fallen down right in front of our house. We’re all stunned. 5 In all these cases, the dream, and especially the central image of the dream seems to be picturing, very clearly, the emotions of the dreamer (Hartmann, 1998, 1999, 2011). Sometimes the dream involves a long story, with multiple scenes. However if there is one powerful image, or a few, these will usually be carrying the underlying emotion. All the above of course are only examples, or “anecdotal evidence,” illustrating rather than demonstrating a point. We then went on to see whether we could develop actual research evidence for this view of dreams. We then attempted to study quantitatively the tidal wave image and similar powerful central images. We first called the image a “Contextualizing Image” (CI) since it appeared to provide a “picture-context” for the emotion of the dreamer (Hartmann, 1996; Hartmann et al., 1997; Hartmann et al., 1998). However this term was found unwieldy and confusing by some, so the image is now called simply the Central Image, keeping the abbreviation CI. A scoring sheet for the CI has been developed (figure 1) (Hartmann et al, 1997, Hartmann et al, 1998) which can be used on any written or recorded dream report. It has now been used in over fifty different research studies. Fig 1 About here The scorer, who knows nothing about the dreamer or the circumstances surrounding the dream, looks at a dream report and first decides whether or not there is a scorable Central Image. If there is (this turns out to be the case in 50 to 60% of dreams scored) the scorer jots down a few words describing the image, and then scores the intensity of the image on a seven-point scale (0, 0.5, 1.0, 1.5, 2.0, 2.5, 3.0) based on how powerful, vivid, bizarre and detailed the image seems. (“0” means no CI and “3” means about as powerful an image as you have seen in dreams.) The scorer then tries to guess what emotion or emotions, from a list of emotions provided, might be pictured by this image. Central Image Intensity (CII) turns out to be an especially important measure. Although it is of course a subjective judgment by the scorer, there is good agreement between scorers -- inter-rater reliability of r = .70 to r = .90 (Hartmann, et al. 1998; Hartmann, et al. 2001, Hartmann, Zborovsky, et al. 2001). We will discuss a number of studies showing that Central Image Intensity (CII) is related to the power of the underlying emotion. 6 Since there are eighteen emotions to choose from, it has been more difficult to obtain good inter-rater reliability on the individual emotion pictured by the dream imagery. However there is quite good agreement between raters when emotions ( fig 1) are grouped into three categories: 1) fear/terror and helplessness/vulnerability; 2) other negative emotions (#s 310 on rating sheet); 3) all positive emotions (#s 11-18) (Hartmann, et al. 2001). First we showed that, on a blind basis, CII is rated higher in dreams that in daydreams ( Hartmann et al., 2001) as expected. We also found, as expected, that CII is higher in content from REM awakenings than from nonREM awakenings, which in turn score higher than material from waking periods (Hartmann and Stickgold, 2000). We then went on to look at whether CII is related to emotion and the power of the dreamer’s emotion. Several studies looked at “Big dreams” (Jung’s term for powerful dreams that remain with the dreamer for years). We operationalized “big dreams” in several ways. In one study we found that CII is rated higher in “dreams that stand out” than in “recent dreams” from the same persons. (Hartmann et al., 2001) Likewise CII is scored higher in dreams characterized as “ the earliest dream you can remember” than in “recent dreams” (Hartmann and Kunzendorf, 2005-2006). Thus CII appears to be high in dreams that are remembered and are presumably emotionally important. In one study, we examined dreams called “important” by the dreamer. A group of 57 persons each sent us a recent dream they considered “important” and a dream they considered “unimportant” or less important. CII was significantly higher in the “important” dreams (Mean for Important dreams 1.19, mean for Unimportant dreams 0.81. Difference = .38, S.D. 1.048; t = 2.78, p < .007) (Hartmann, 2008). We also studied one group of “especially significant” dreams. A group of 23 students very interested in their dreams each submitted one “especially significant” dream in a report by by Dr. Roger Knudson(2001). The mean CII in these 23 dreams was 2.62 (mean of two experienced raters). This is the highest mean CII score of any group we have seen, much higher that means of recent dreams in various groups. These students did not supply a “non-significant dream for a direct comparison. However, comparing these “highly significant” dreams with our largest 7 group of recent dreams, from 286 students, we found a highly significant difference: (Mean for Significant Dreams 2.62, S.D. 0.48; mean for recent dreams 0.75, S.D. 1.03. t = 16.0, p < .0001) (Hartmann, 2008). Dreams that were considered “big” in the sense of leading to a scientific or artistic discovery ( Barrett, 2001) likewise were scored as having unusually high Central Image Intensity ( Hartmann 2008). We went on to examine trauma and stress – situations involving strong and mainly negative emotions. We found that CII is higher in Ss who have suffered a recent trauma than in those who have not (Hartmann, et al. 1998; Hartmann, et al. 2001). We also found CII to be higher in the recent dreams of students who checked off on a questionnaire that they had suffered either physical or sexual abuse at any time, than in students who checked off no abuse (p < .02)(Hartmann, et al. 2001). Trauma and abuse are difficult to study systematically, since the trauma is different in each person, and the methods of dream collection differed as well. Therefore we conducted a more systematic study, using 9/11/01 as a day that we considered to have been traumatic or at least very stressful for everyone in the United States. We found a number of people who had been recording all their remembered dreams for years, and were willing to send us twenty dreams – the last ten they had recorded before 9/11/01 and the first ten dreams after 9/11. Our first study involved 320 dreams from 16 subjects before and after 9/11/01. When the code was broken, we found that the “after” dreams had significantly higher CII than the “before” dreams (p < .002). Somewhat to our surprise, the “before” and “after” dreams did not differ on length, “dream-likeness”, “ vividness”, or presence of towers, airplanes or attacks. CII was the only measure that clearly differentiated the after dreams vs. the before dreams (Hartmann and Basile, 2003). Subsequently we expanded the study to 880 dreams from 44 subjects (Hartmann and Brezler, 2008). Again, in the larger sample, CII was significantly higher in dreams after 9/11 (p < .001). This confirms our earlier studies finding higher CII at times of stress or emotional arousal. With the larger N there was now a slight but significant increase in content involving “attacks”, though there was still no before vs. after difference on the other measures. In addition to the finding of increased CI intensity after trauma, abuse, or after 9/11, there was also a shift in the ratings of “emotion pictured by the CI” towards the first two emotions on the list (Fear/terror and helplessness/ 8 vulnerability, which can be called the “nightmare emotions”). This shift was usually statistically significant, but was not as clear-cut and dramatic as the increase in CII – probably because of the difficulty in scoring and reaching agreement on the exact emotion pictured. This group of studies show that the power of the Central Image of the dream appears to be related to the strength of the underlying emotion – increasing in situations of increased emotion. And, after trauma or stress, when the emotions felt can be presumed to be negative, the negative emotions, especially fear/terror and helplessness/vulnerability, were rated as being pictured by the Central Image. We also examined this view of dreams as imagery picturing powerful emotion in a different way. We suggested that perhaps sleep and REM-sleep, though the usual place for dreams, were not absolutely necessary. If a dream was made of imagery driven by powerful emotion, could we introduce these conditions in waking persons and produce a dream or something like a dream? In fact this is exactly what we found. Forty-four students in a classroom setting were each asked to write down four things, in a balanced order, after appropriate instructions: 1) a recent dream; 2) a recent daydream; 3) a daydream or reverie allowed to develop right there in class, in a relaxed state, with no other instruction than to let imagery develop; and 4) a daydream or reverie allowed to develop in a relaxed state, after they had chosen an emotion that felt close to them and had been instructed to intensify the emotion, allow it to envelop them and become as strong as possible. All the written material was examined on a blind basis, and rated on standardized scales of “dream-likeness” and “bizarreness.” Results showed that material written under condition 4 (imagery while experiencing emotion) was rated significantly more dreamlike and more bizarre than material from conditions 2 and 3, and was rated almost exactly as dreamlike and bizarre as condition 1, the recent dream (Hartmann and Kunzendorf, 2000; Hartmann, et al. 2002-2003). Thus the CI paradigm – imagery under intense emotion – can produce a dream, or very dream-like material, even in the waking state. Overall, these quantitative studies convinced us that the CI is driven or guided by the dreamer’s underlying emotion, and in the clearest cases the CI straightforwardly pictures the emotion, as in the tidal wave dream. Recently, Kunzendorf and Veatch (2013) have developed a new way of approaching the central imagery of the dream. They asked their study participants, who were digital art students, to picture their dreams digitally. This is an intriguing way to examine the CI of the dream directly, bypassing the verbal dream report and 9 the scorer. They came up with striking images in 100 dreamers, which I believe were the Central Images of their dreams, though that term was not used. The authors also studied the importance of underlying emotion in these dreams: they found that the students could clarify the meaning of their dreams and images by focusing on the underlying emotion. In addition to the quantitative studies of CIs, summarized above, we find that in a rough way we can explain a number of the most common dreams as an emotional state translated into a clear central dream image ( thymophor). Here are some emotional states and dreams which may be familiar to the reader Emotional state: I’m just three (or two, or four) years old. I’ve been pretty well treated. So far I assumed that I was the center of the world, but now I’m beginning to notice that the world is actually run by large creatures who are much more powerful than I am. These creatures act friendly most of the time, but they’re unpredictable. Once in a while they yell or fight or hurt me or make demands of me that I can’t understand. It’s scary at times. Dream: I’m being chased by a big monster (this is the most common dream at that age). What is a monster, basically? It‘s a large, powerful, unpredictable creature. Emotional state: I’m a bit anxious. People think I’m competent, but I’m not sure. I feel a bit lost. I’m not sure I can manage all this stuff. Dream: I can’t find the classroom / I haven’t studied / I’ve studied the wrong subject / I can’t read the exam, etc. (These “typical exam dreams” are among the most common dreams in adults -- at least in adults who’ve been to school in developed nations.) Emotional state: Things are going quite well, in fact: I’m getting married soon/ starting a new job/ moving to a nice new area. But I’m worried. Others seem so much more confident. They know what they’re doing but I don’t. Dream: I’m naked/ half naked/wearing a torn dress/ dressed in rags/ while other people are more fully and appropriately clothed. (This is another very common dream in adults.) 10 There are some situations that produce a very expectable group of emotions. For instance, we have gathered many dreams from pregnant women expecting their first child, though we have not studied the Central Images in a quantitative way. The most complete series of such dreams, including the examples below, are in an excellent work by Patricia Maybruck (1989) . Such dreams have also been collected by Garfield (1988), Van de Castle ( 1994). Many of the dreams picture clear emotions and emotional concerns, which differ with the stage of pregnancy. The dominant concerns early in pregnancy generally revolve around “what is happening to my body?” which includes “will I still be attractive?” and the dreams reflect this through changes of shape and increases in size of the self and other objects or animals; the dreams also involve large disfigured creatures. There were many reports of dreams such as this: “I was attending some kind of show or concert. I was real fat; I looked like a blimp. The guys were all laughing and admiring these really cute girls on stage. No one paid attention to me.” Later concerns arise about what will this thing, this baby, actually look like which then leads to innumerable dreams of small and then large creatures sometimes monsters or strange ill-formed things - portraying these concerns of the dreamer. Here are two such examples: “I go out the back door of my house and I see, in the neighbor’s yard, their pet dog with many small black animals. They are a triangular shape, a kind I never saw before. They are playing with some rats on a little hill of dust.” “There, sitting between my legs, was this naked little boy... his face was like an old man’s and there were fangs coming out of his lips." And towards the end of the pregnancy, the woman’s primary concern becomes whether or not she’ll be able to handle motherhood, and child-rearing. Here are some of these dreams: “Our apartment was invaded with mice, lizards, rabbits, kittens, puppies. They were coming in the windows and front door. I couldn’t stop them and they were messing up everything.” “Someone left a box full of baby chicks on our doorstep. I was busy and they died before I had time to take care of them.” These dreams clearly picture the woman’s concerns that she'll be overwhelmed and be unable to care properly for her child. 11 My impression from reading many such series of dreams during pregnancy is that these very clear emotion-picturing dreams are found especially in first pregnancies, in stable young women who are basically happy to be pregnant, and in whom the pregnancy is the most important thing in her life. However, in later pregnancies, unwanted or not-definitely-wanted pregnancies, or pregnancies which are only one of many ongoing issues for the woman, the emotional state is more complicated, and likewise the dreams. In these many examples and situations, we have again seen that the CI of the dream pictures the dreamer’s underlying emotion or emotional concern. Again thymophor -- translating emotion into imagery. I have discussed in detail elsewhere ( Hartmann 2010a, 2011) the fact that the dream does not simply replay waking events; it always combines things, adds something new. This is true even in so-called recurrent or repetitive dreams, and even in PTSD dreams ( Hartmann 2010a). Thus the dream is always a creation, not simple replay. This is exactly the encyclopedia definition of creating a work of art. “The creation of a work of art is the bringing about of a new combination of elements in the medium. The elements existed beforehand but not in the same combination; creation of is the re-formation of these pre-existing materials.” (Nash, 1986). And, at least for the last two hundred years, we have usually added a bit: “ …. Combining old material in new ways, guided by the artist’s emotion. Thus the dream can be considered a work of art, or at least the beginning of one. Let us go on to see whether we can find the thymophor in the more recognized forms of art as well. Starting with Poetry Starting with film might be easiest, since we often feel that dreams are like movies, and film has been called the most oneiric ( dreamlike) of the arts. But I’d like to start with poetry, where the similarities may not be so obvious. Ordinarily poems and dreams live in quite separate rooms of our mental mansions. However, I will argue that a poem, at least a short memorable poem, is actually very like a dream. I am not referring to poems that supposedly arrived fully formed from a dream, such as Coleridge’s Kubla Khan. I am referring to a basic similarity in the creation of dreams and poems. The similarity I want to emphasize involves the Central Image (CI), discussed above. A poem too, surprisingly often, contains a powerful Central Image that pictures an emotional state. I suggest that what T.S. Eliot calls the “objective 12 correlative” of the poem, or other work, is more or less identical to what we have called the Central Image of the dream. (1917) He introduces the term: “The only way of expressing emotion in the form of art is by finding an ‘objective correlative’; in other words, a set of objects, a situation, a chain of events which shall be the formula of that particular emotion.” The Oxford Dictionary of Literary Terms (2008) defines objective correlative as “an external equivalent for an internal state of mind; thus any object, scene, event, or situation that may be said to stand for or evoke a given mood or emotion.” Eliot cites his own lines in “The Love Song of J. Alfred Prufrock”: I should have been a pair of ragged claws, Scuttling across the floors of silent seas. as the objective correlative picturing Prufrock’s emotional state of social shyness. Of course, the first lines of Eliot’s poem: Let us go then you and I, When the evening is spread out against the sky Like a patient etherized upon a table. are also an objective correlative, though they picture a harder-to-define emotional state, that might be called a state of unease, or disturbance. Another of the greatest poems of the 20th century – Yeats’ “ The Second Coming” -- is built on two dramatic objective correlatives, or Central Images. It begins: Turning and turning in the widening gyre The falcon cannot hear the falconer; Things fall apart; the centre cannot hold; and ends: And what rough beast, its hour come round at last, Slouches toward Bethlehem to be born? These CIs pictures an emotional state that could be called something like concern, turning to powerful apprehension, approaching dread. Clearly not all poems contain a clear-cut objective correlative or CI. But not all dreams do either. So let me narrow the field a bit: first of all, I believe we are 13 speaking of short poems, and in fact the dreams we have discussed are short dreams too. Both the dreams and the poems can usually fit on one or two pages in written form – occasionally three or four. I cannot deal with book-length poems, and luckily there are few or no book-length dreams. (I’m speaking of literal dreams, not literary dreams in which a long narrative is framed as a dream or vision). Ezra Pound (1934) defined three major aspects of poetry: Melopoieia refers to the sound, the music of poetry; phanopoieia to the imagery; and logopoieia to the sense or meaning. The correlation we are discussing, linking dreams and poems, works best for short poems, and obviously it emphasizes the imagery: the phanopoieia. Let’s examine some actual poems. Since the boundaries of “poetry,” “good poetry,” “real poetry” are notoriously difficult to define precisely, I shall consider only well-known poems, poems recognized over time as “good” or “great” poems. We might call these “big poems” -- analogous to “big dreams,” following Jung. And I’ll try to stick with relatively short “big” poems. Short image-dependent poems, then. My impression is that memorable, impactful, “big” short poems, almost always contain one or more CIs, as we found in big dreams. There’s always more than that, to be sure. The image is built of words, and the words have both a sound and a sense, which ideally blends so well with the imagery that a single entity is formed. Everything comes together. First of all I want to look at some great and memorable poems that clearly do contain one or two powerful central images. Leafing through anthologies, and through the anthology in my head of poems I have read and remembered convinces me that quite a number of the great short poems of the English language do appear to work in this way. I’ll start with the 20th century, and briefly mention some poems of previous centuries We’ve already mentioned two of the great poems of the past 100 years: The love song of J. Alfred Prufrock by Eliot and The Second Coming by Yeats, each of which have powerful central images. Here is Robert Frost’s: Stopping by woods on a snowy evening. 14 Whose woods these are I think I know. His house is in the village though; He will not see me stopping here To watch his woods fill up with snow My little horse must think it queer To stop without a farmhouse near Between the woods and frozen lake The darkest evening of the year. He gives his harness bells a shake To ask if there is some mistake. The only other sound's the sweep Of easy wind and downy flake. The woods are lovely, dark and deep. But I have promises to keep, And miles to go before I sleep, And miles to go before I sleep. The image is supreme here. But of course it is integrated with the music and the meaning. The words and the rhythm are so gentle, so song-like, that the narrator (and we the reader) are pulled into the image of the snowy woods and are tempted to stay (death? suicide?) until our harness is softly pulled, by the all-important word “but,” and we return slowly to life, to our lives. Here are some others, among the greatest short poems in English, going back from the 20th century to Shakespeare. I hope they are familiar enough to the reader so that I can cite the titles and need not include the complete texts here. William Carlos Williams: The Red Wheelbarrow Wallace Stevens: Of Mere Being Thomas Hardy: In the Time of the Breaking of Nation Wilfred Owen: Strange Meeting Sylvia Plath: Tulips Matthew Arnold: Dover Beach Emily Dickinson: There’s a certain slant of light 15 Walt Whitman: O Captain! My Captain! Percy Bysshe Shelley: Ozymandias John Keats: Ode to a Grecian Urn William Blake: The Tyger William Shakespeare: Sonnet 73 (That time of year thou mayst in me behold) I cannot talk of each poem in detail. My point is that these famous poems are written in a variety of very different poetic styles and traditions, yet each of them depends on one Central Image, or a few , which carry the emotional power. Can all poetry be characterized in this way? No, certainly not. First of all, as mentioned, really long poems can hardly be viewed as depending on one central image. Some of the best known classic long poems such as Lucretius’s De rerum Naturae, or Horace’s long odes on agriculture, are basically logopoieia, and they are didactic, meant to teach the reader. Modern readers hardly consider these to be poems, but rather essays or treatises, written in verse. Other long poems, such as Dante’s 100-canto Divina Commedia are quite different. Dante’s work is unquestionably poetry, often considered the greatest or one of the greatest poems of the Western world. The work is bursting with imagery, but it is impossible to pick out a single powerful image, because there are so many. If each canto is considered as a separate short poem, then indeed one, or sometimes two or three, central images can easily be found. Similarly in Fitzgerald’s famous long poem ( or loose translation) “The Rubaiyat of Omar Khayyam,” a central image is prominent in almost every verse or group of verses. For instance here is a marvelous image of fate: “The Moving Finger writes: and having writ, Moves on: nor all thy Piety nor Wit Shall lure it back to cancel half a Line, Nor all thy Tears wash out a Word of it.” 16 So I am definitely restricting the discussion to short poems, or short chunks of long poems. This is not so different from the situation with dreams. It is very difficult to deal with the occasional very long dream that occasionally show up in research or clinical material. The researcher or student gives up on these, or if necessary divides them into short chunks – as though the long dream were a series of short dreams. So we are dealing with short poems, and short dreams. Admittedly, not all short poems involve a powerful central image. Many popular short poems depend heavily on a solid rhyme scheme and ease of recall. And often a simple lesson. This is usually the case in poems for children. For instance here’s a short poem, recited by coaches to millions of American children and teenagers, and often remembered by many who hardly read poetry at all. When that Great Scorer comes To put a mark against your name, He will score not whether you won or lost, But how you played the game. ( Rice, 19--) (Grantland Rice from Alumnus Football, 1932) Such poems, though very well known, are certainly not considered among the world’s great poems. I have gone through lengthy anthologies of “light verse” and “humorous verse” as well as poetry for children and found innumerable such examples. These poems contain a lesson, an aphorism, or a joke ( logopoeia) , and they rely for their effect on the sound– an easy and regular rhyme scheme (melopoieia) They almost never very seldom contain striking Central Images ( phanopoieia). Thus in my perhaps prejudiced view the great short poems -– the ones I call the “big short poems” -- almost always contain a central image, and in this way they are closely related to “big “ short dreams.” In both cases thymophor, the translation of emotion into imagery, is prominent. Starting with Metaphor We have discussed dreaming, which depends heavily on metaphor. The Central Image of the dream is a picture-metaphor, a metaphor for the dreamer’s 17 emotional state ( Hartmann, 2011 ). And as we have seen, poetry often depends on a powerful metaphoric Central Image. Now let us briefly discuss metaphor itself. For many years metaphor was taught only as one of many “figures of speech,” along with simile, synecdoche etc., but it is now generally recognized that metaphor is much more than that. Metaphor is everywhere, not only in our language, but in our thought, and in our entire mental life. Lakoff and his associate have convincingly demonstrated the ubiquity of metaphor in our thinking. (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980; Lakoff & Turner, 1989) Lakoff’s group generally uses the term “Conceptual Metaphor,” to make it clear that the discussion is not restricted to poetic or decorative metaphor, but rather involves our basic concepts, our entire framework of thought. For instance, it is almost impossible to describe a relationship between two people, without using a metaphoric phrase: (we’re really moving, we’re on the fast track, it’s all downhill from here, we’re hitting a rough patch, it’s an uphill battle, it’s time to bail out, etc., etc.). These are all subsumed under the conceptual metaphor “ A relationship is a journey.” We think in these metaphoric terms when thinking of a relationship, or for that mater, of any important large concept, such as life, love, mankind, etc. Metaphor appears to be an essential part of our thinking. A major portion of the field known as cognitive linguistics has recently grown up around this insight. Lakoff and his associates are right in insisting that metaphor is essential to our thought as well as our language. However, the discussions of Conceptual Metaphor usually omit an obviously important element --the emotion – which I believe is essential to understanding the metaphor. It is the underlying emotion of the user ( speaker or writer) that determines which metaphor will be used. “It’s time to bail out” implies a very different underlying emotional state from “ it’s all smooth sailing,” within “Love is a journey.” A Metaphor generally explains the “tenor” of the metaphor (the first term, the subject being discussed) by providing a “vehicle” (the second term), which is simpler or more easily pictured. ( This second term is also called the “source” or “source domain”). Thus “man is a wolf” or “love is a bumpy journey” work as useful metaphors. The metaphor does not simply find a similarity between any two terms, as some have claimed. The reversed phrasing: “a wolf is man” or “ a bumpy journey is love” do not work, and sound confusing. Often, especially in a poem or in a dream, only the vehicle of the metaphor is stated. The tenor ( love, life, death ) has to be understood by the reader of the poem, or whoever is trying to understand the dream. The vehicle of the metaphor in some sense explains the tenor, but it tells only a part of the truth. Of course love – in the sense of a continuing relationship -is a journey, but love is also a growing plant, or two intertwined plants, or it may be a competition, a fight, a war. Everything depends on the underlying emotion. The 18 choice of vehicle is guided by our underlying emotion. And as we saw above, the emotion also determines which specific metaphor will be used, when we think or talk using the broad conceptual metaphor: “love is a journey.” When we say, “ Man is a wolf” it goes without saying that we are portraying a certain aspect of man – rapaciousness, violence, greed. And we’ re usually picturing our (negative) feeling for that part of man. Similarly, speaking of a particular man, we can say, “That man is a monster,” which pictures our hatred and/or fear of that man. When we say, “That woman (or that man) is an angel, we are calling up another portion of that person, guided by our feelings -– this time a more positive feeling. It is worth noting that although these metaphoric descriptions may sometimes feel accurate, the pictures they provide are not complete, and certainly not completely true. They give only a very partial description. They do not provide an overall description or definition of man, or of that man. What we are saying is I like, or I fear, or I dislike, that man. They are accurate pictures of our emotional state – our feelings (at the time) about man, or about that particular man. Simply stating that the vehicle of the metaphor is chosen according to our emotion is true, and often obvious, but it gives emotion only a subsidiary role. I believe we can more accurately state that what is being pictured –- the tenor of the metaphor -– is not the large concept (life, love, man) but the emotional state itself: our emotional attitude towards life, love, man. It is actually the underlying emotional state that is the tenor of the metaphor. So we can note that metaphor is often Thymophor – an emotion-picturing metaphor, that turns an emotion, or emotional attitude, into an image -- similarly to what we have found in the Central Image of the dream, and of the poem. In the dream too, I have argued that the underlying emotion is essential in understanding the dream imagery. Some, including Lakoff himself have examined the dream in terms of Lakoff’s conceptual metaphors (1993). In this sense one considers the dream image as “metaphorically picturing” or “symbolizing” a concept or an event. Indeed a dream journey often seems to picture the dreamer’s life, or relationships. However, I consider this a very incomplete view, since again it omits the emotion.. About the Tidal Wave Dream, for instance, I have been asked: “ Aren’t you saying that the tidal wave symbolizes the fire or the attack?” Well, no. That way of thinking avoids the main issue. Why should a tidal wave symbolize a fire or an attack? There is no resemblance between these items except for the emotion. The emotion drives the imagery, and in the simplest case the picture is an excellent description of the emotional state: “How do you feel (having just escaped from a fire in which your brother died) ?” “Tidal wave!” 19 Similarly, a poem’s Central Image does not simply describe a scene, or a concept. Reading Frost’s poem ( as mentioned above) it is possible to listen to the melody and love the beautiful snowy woods, and not think of a metaphor at all. Children, and readers first coming upon the poem may read it that way. Or one can think of the snowy woods as a metaphoric picture of a gentle life, lived in a rural setting. But then, on more careful reading, we run into the word “but” in the last verse, which tells us to look again. It gives us an emotional tone. The woods are not just pretty. They pull us/him in. He considers remaining there. The woods are peaceful sleep; the woods are death, suicide perhaps. He pulls himself away: BUT I have promises to keep. The woods are not a generic, conceptual metaphor: it is not the case that deep woods obviously or always stand for death. It is this particular speaker’s emotion that we begin to feel and understand. The emotion makes the metaphor, and makes the poem. Again, we have met the Thymophor. We see that a conceptual metaphor provides only a partial, generic description of its subject, the tenor of the metaphor, ( life, love, man) if we leave out emotion. However, the metaphoric phrase often pictures clearly the underlying emotion. So the Thymophor is a more common beast than we at first thought. It shows up as an essential element in our everyday inescapable use of metaphor (“It looks like a bumpy road ahead”) as well as in our dreams and our poetry. Starting with Other Arts: emotion translated into imagery in painting, sculpture and music Our examination of metaphor can leads us directly to one form or art or almost-art: the cartoon or the caricature. This usually takes the form of drawing, or painting, but occasionally sculpture or other forms as well. Caricature A cartoon or political caricature may involve a direct picture of a metaphor. A cartoon may show a man -- a politician, say -- as a wolf, or a pig, or a snake. Here a caricature is nothing but a painted or drawn metaphor, and usually the emotion 20 attached is quite clear. This can be considered a very simple example of emotioncarrying metaphor, or thymophor. In the caricatures or cartoons that make an impression (“big caricatures?”), the caricaturist’s (artist’s, cartoonist’s) emotional attitude is very obviously being pictured or emphasized “ (In my opinion) that man is a pig, feeding himself at the expense of the poor. “ Or in some cases a negative emotional attitude to a whole field such as medicine or law. Daumier’s well-known caricatures are wonderful examples, and he created these caricatures as sculptures,, as well as paintings and drawings. Sometimes the caricature simply exaggerates one aspect of the subject’s physical appearance. Whatever feature is slightly odd, or too big, or ugly in the subject is made twice as odd, twice as big or ugly in the caricature. This may seem to be only a draughtsman’s trick, but usually an emotional statement is being made too. The caricature “puts people in their place.” It allows us to laugh at a big important man: “ doesn’t he look ugly!” or a class of persons (greedy lawyers; fat plutocrats) or occasionally to laugh at all mankind: “aren’t we a silly-looking bunch of creatures!” Here in cartoons and caricatures we have a thymophor in relatively pure form: the image very directly pictures either an emotional metaphor, or an emotional attitude. The details of the craft – just how the picture is drawn or painted – are of less importance here than in other arts. And indeed some will argue that caricature is not truly an art at all. There is another sort of art that specializes in depicting emotion, often in exaggerated form. I don’t think the genre has an overall name. It includes items such as the heads sculpted by Franz Xavier Messerschmidt in the 18th century; each showing a face in the grip (throes?) of an emotion, in highly exaggerated form. (Ilg, 1885). A great deal of photographic art falls in this category, trying to depict a strong emotion or emotional tone, sometimes clearly showing extremes of emotion. Such art could be considered a form that is often very basic and very concrete thymophor. A kind of naked thymophor perhaps. The craft plays a minor role, though this is not always the case: for instance, Munch’s “The Scream” could be included as a depiction of emotion, though there’s much more involved. Painting, sculpture Let’s move on to the more traditional fine arts and consider broadly the classic visual arts such as painting and sculpture. Finding the thymophor will be easiest if we start with non-representational art. Consider a painter who works by stretching a canvas , and painting whatever occurs to her or him. The result may be abstract, -- lines, spaces, splotches of color, drippings of paint-- or there may be suggestions of people or objects, or places. What is this painter painting? I’d say in a broad sense that she’s painting her emotional state. What else is there? Of course, 21 she may well say that the picture is a depiction of a landscape she knows or imagines, or an impression of a person in her memory, or many other things. But clearly she is not producing a camera image of that scene. She is at least in part painting her emotional state. Sometimes she will state directly: “I’m painting the way I feel today.” How about the more traditional painter , outdoors, painting an actual landscape in front of him, or an interior, or a bowl of fruit. What is he painting? He may say, “I’m painting it just as it is,” or “I paint what I see.” But of course it’s not “ just as it is.” It’s a painting, not an actual landscape. And different painters will come out with totally different paintings of the same landscape. 1 “I paint what I see” may be the artist’s honest view, but the “I” is usually more important than the “see.” And the “I” is of course not just the “eye.” The “I” is the entire person of the painter, which comprises past history, personality, and emotional state. (Past history and personality are important in forming the painter’s style, and are also important in determining the present emotional state.) Two painters do not “see” the same thing. I suggest that often “ I paint what I feel” is more accurate than “ I paint what I see,” or at least that the two work in conjunction. Or a painter may state some form of “ I’m looking for the truth, or the essence, of the landscape/ interior /fruit.” That’s fine, but the question arises as to whether the truth turns out to be a universal truth or essence, or rather an emotional truth within the artist. Nothing new here. It is a truism that different painters painting the same scene will come out with different results. Painters, and critics, will tend to describe the differences in terms of craft or technique. Indeed craft is hugely important, and sometimes the details of the craft overwhelm or hide any attempt to find a thymophor. In a sense the craft is the art. The paints and pigments chosen, the material painted on (stucco, canvas, paper, wood, etc.), the brushes or other instruments to apply the paint, the style of application, the thickness of the paint, the length of the brushstrokes, the force applied, etc. These elements make all the difference, and can explain the different results, the different styles of the painters. Craft is surely important, but is it the total picture? I’d say not. I believe that all the elements of the craft are important, but they are not enough to explain the picture. There is an emotional state being expressed, This is reminiscent of what we saw in dreams. The dreamer may for various reasons want to emphasize that the dream repeats the actual scene, for instance the traumatic scene. “ Just the way it was.” However, on examination, we find there’s always something added. And the addition is guided by the dreamer’s emotion(Hartmann 2010a). 1 22 even in a depiction of a landscape, though this is far clearer in some painters, than in others. To some extent “I paint what I feel” always plays a part. As a very simplistic experiment, let’s put two pictures up in our mind’s gallery – two landscape paintings, both showing a field in a countryside in Southern France. One is a fairly realistic depiction, by an unknown painter, that looks somewhat like a color photo. The other is one of Van Gogh’s paintings of the same scene. How are the two different? Well, we can describe many differences: In the Van Gogh, the paint is thicker, the brush strokes are shorter, the application seems stronger and more abrupt, the pigments emphasize yellow and gold. An expert could go on for several more pages. But are we satisfied with this description? Isn’t there more? Isn’t there some form of thymophor? Some emotional state – presumably Van Gogh’s emotional state – is being pictured, and we feel some of this as we look at it. He may well have been painting what he saw, but we do not look at his painting to get a precise depiction of the grasses and flowers of Southern France. Nor do we (most of us amateurs at least) look at the painting to admire the specific details of the unusual brush strokes. What we see and feel is a powerful image of an emotional state: the thymophor. I believe that to a certain extent the painter is always painting what s/he feels, not simply what s/he sees. Van Gogh is unusually dramatic. His emotion is right out on the surface, screaming at us. For some painters, perhaps for the painter of the other picture in our imagined gallery, the emotion may be something less dramatic -- something like a mild, calm love of nature. We see this in much of the painting covering the walls of a waiting room or office. Critics and other painters may consider the brushstrokes and pigments and other essential elements for their own sakes. We, as amateur appreciators, can see these as clothing for the thymophor – much as we could see the words and lines of a “big short” poem as similarly clothing the thymophor, the emotion-carrying central image. Considering poetry we noted that this view makes sense especially for “big short poems,” as it does for “big short dreams.” Perhaps the same is true of painting: perhaps we find the thymophor especially in certain impactful “big” paintings. Clearly, in painting and other arts, the elements of the craft are essential, but I believe that the picturing or carrying over of emotion -- the thymophor – is important too, in the creation of the work., We should keep in mind that “emotion” covers a lot of ground. We do not necessarily have to think of personal anguish, or anger, or sadness. In many of the religion-themed paintings that fill our grand museums and Christian churches, the dominant emotion is “awe, wonder, mystery.” This is an emotion that does not show up on typical lists of the four or six most basic human emotions, but it does appear as one of the eighteen basic emotions in our studies of emotions pictured by the CI of the dream ( fig 1). And we find that this emotion is scored quite frequently, especially in “big” dreams. 23 Film and longer visual works So far, we have considered a single painting, or drawing, caricature, sculpture, rather than with something longer, such as a film, a series of paintings, or tapestries, perhaps. A single work appears to be closer to the moment of inspiration, to the thymophor as a unit of artistic creation, and closer to the “big dream” or “big poem”. With film, or longer visual works, we are faced with the same issues as with long poems, long literary works, or even long dreams. Life becomes more complicated. We may find a series of dramatic emotional images, within a lengthy story-context. We can sometimes find a CI, a thymophor, in a certain portion of the work. For instance, there are certain dramatic images that we remember in classic films: the cut eyeball in Bunuel/Cocteau, Un Chien Andalou , the knight playing chess with death in Bergman’s The Seventh Seal, T.E. Lawrence emerging gloriously from the shimmering desert in Lawrence of Arabia. These moments are definitely emotional, and have the basic characteristics of thymophor. So, again, a longer work is problematic, but we can find thymophor if we are willing to look at bits or chunks of it. Music What about music? It has often been stated that music is especially close to the emotions, and indeed a short piece of music, such as a typical love song pictures an emotional state very directly. Similarly, the music of an operatic aria usually portrays the emotional state of the character singing it. Sometimes a short theme in instrumental music perfectly pictures a mood : anyone listening to the first bars of Beethoven’s Pathetique sonata feels a sense of sadness . This is also an instance of thymophor: if we can think in terms of an auditory image, rather than a visual image, the first bars of Pathetique are a Central Image carrying the emotion of grief or sadness. An instrumental piece may involve a spontaneous outpouring, picturing the composer’s underlying emotional state, but not necessarily. The composer may also be picturing an emotional state s/he is imagining or inventing. In fact the composer of an opera or musical comedy tries hard to compose different music for each character, picturing that character’s emotional state. The same is true of a playwright or dramatic poet, who frequently chooses words to picture the separate emotional states of each character. For some, the idea of an auditory image is obvious and clear, but for many of us who are very visual, it takes time to accept the idea. Our dream images are overwhelmingly visual, even in those who love music and play music. However, some composers report that they definitely dream in music. Mozart and Wagner 24 both claimed that they heard their music in dreams. Others see a visual image of the score -- actually picturing the notes on paper, or picturing a modified image of the notes on paper: in one recent instance, a composer dreamt of pine trees scrabbling their way up a snowy hillside, which suggested to him in musical notation the ascending arpeggios he needed to complete his composition. (Knudson, 2001). If we can accept the idea of an auditory image, then thymophor -- translating emotion into imagery – definitely occurs in music. Thymophor and Memory: An Experiment in Autobiographical Memory Memory is an extremely complex subject and I will not try to discuss it in detail here. There are at least four separate, identifiable types of human memory (Miyushita, 2004). One of these is “episodic declarative memory,” often called “autobiographical memory.” There is general agreement that our autobiographical memory system is strongly affected by our emotions, and that memory is a constructive process; we are constantly making small alterations in our memories. For instance, we have a tendency to forget, repress, or alter memories that are not in accord with our selfimage. Nietzsche already knew or intuited this in the 19th century: “ ‘I did it’ says my memory. ‘But I could not have done it’ says my pride. ‘ But I did it…’ And so, back-and-forth, until finally memory gives in.” (Nietzsche, 1885). Overall, we usually remember best what is striking or unusual and what is emotionally important to us. For instance, I have windsurfed on a beach in Truro, on Cape Cod, many times over the years, but I remember hardly any details of these experiences. However, on one occasion, twenty dolphins found me and circled around me on my sailboard for several minutes. This was not only unusual (it is rare to see any dolphins in Truro), but it was emotionally striking. I felt a sense of awe, first mixed with a little fear, and then I felt somehow blessed. I believe I will always remember that particular episode. I can still see it, clearly and visually, in my mind. Following up on this, I decided to jot down, as quickly as I could, whatever memorable moments leapt out at me, going back year by year to my childhood. I was not interested in long series of memories about a particular person or place— the way we usually search our memories. I was simply looking for moments of memory that stood out strongly from the background. In fifteen minutes, I had written down 28 specific memories. Later, looking them over one by one, I found that what I had written down in each case was a visual scene, sometimes in considerable detail. And each scene involved a strong emotion. However what stayed in my mind, what I remembered and wrote down, was the visual scene, not so much the emotion. The emotion was obviously there; it had “tagged” the scene or event that was to be remembered. 25 Based on many interviews, I realize that not everyone remembers things in as vivid a visual form as I do; there is quite a range of visual memory. But the emotional element is found in everyone, as far as I can tell, whether the visual imagery is clear and detailed or not. Autobiographies, biographies, and even novels are usually organized around emotional moments in a similar way. They contain mostly what is emotionally important to the author or narrator. In an autobiography or memoir such as Proust’s incredibly detailed “À la recherche du temps perdu” we may initially feel that his memory is simply gigantic, and that he includes everything, but obviously, he does not. He remembers what is emotionally important to him -- in his case the appearance, personality, and quirks of his numerous family members, friends and acquaintances, and the details of their interactions. He describes the details in vivid visual form, which makes it easy for the reader to “see” the scene, and makes me suppose that when he recalled a scene, he “saw” it visually as well. Emotion-guided autobiographical memory has also been studied by psychoanalysis over the past 100 years. The course of treatment, in psychoanalysis and related dynamic psychotherapies, involves constant work with the patient’s memory –- the telling, retelling, and reexamining of the personal past. There is generally a slow, gradual learning about one’s life and behavior patterns, and an attempt to understand current relationships and relate them to past ones. But, occasionally there are moments involving a sudden insight or an “aha” experience. These emotional moments often involve visual or other perceptual modalities: I suddenly saw it. It came to me in a flash. I saw my mother criticizing me, and turning away from me, stalking out of the room before I could say anything. And I realize that’s just what I have been doing to my children . . . I feel like screaming. Such instances involve old and new memory images, usually in visual form, and connections between them. They may occur when a current emotion, say in the relationship to the therapist, is very similar to an old emotion related to parents or other figures from the past. These flashes of memory can be considered instances of creativity. They make new connections in old material, guided by emotion, and thus, they are instances of thymophor. Overall, our memories definitely include emotion-guided imagery. Emotion guides or drives the process, but what appears in memory is the image –- usually a visual image –- that captures or pictures the emotion –- similarly to what happens in the dream. The importance of this emotion-guided memory goes far beyond these sudden flashes of insight. Emotion not only determines what memories are most easily remembered. Emotion is also essential in determining what gets into the memory stores in the first place and how it is organized. I have suggested that dreaming plays an important role in this process (Hartmann, 1998, 2011). The CI of the dream carries the underlying emotion, as we have discussed. We have called the 26 CI of the dream the beginning of a work of art. But this is definitely not the beginning of art for art’s sake. There is an adaptive function related to memory. I have suggested that the dream (especially the CI) connects new memories with old memory stores, based on emotion, and plays a crucial role in organizing our memory systems in accordance with what is emotionally important. This is indeed the way our memory systems appear to be organized, and this is by no means a small detail about memory storage. This is who we are; this is our unique self. We are our emotionally guided memory systems. In this sense, thymophor is involved in creation, at a larger level. Our lifelong creation of ourselves is based on our emotions and our emotion-guided imagery, which determines what we remember and in what patterns. Thymophor as a Basic Element of our Creative Process in the Mind and in the Brain We have discussed thymophor as it appears in the central image of the dream and in the “big short poem”, and in other arts as well. We have seen how thymophor supplies the emotional sense of metaphor, and we have suggested at a role for thymophor in organizing autobiographical memory. Thymophor can be approached in a number of other ways as well, which I explore elsewhere (Hartmann, to be published), and will only mention briefly here. For instance, thymophor may be important in understanding the process of translation: it is accepted that, although literal or logical meaning can be readily translated, poetry is extremely hard to translate successfully from one language to another. Indeed the prosody of poetry, the sound of the language (melopoieia), is very difficult to translate. However the thymophor -- the emotionally driven central imagery--lies behind the language and usually survives translation into another language quite well. Thymophor also plays a role in our myths and the archetypal figures that inhabit the myths. Almost always, these figures involve a powerful emotional state clothed ( imaged) in flesh. We have discussed thymophor especially as a part of artistic creation, but I believe it plays a role in other types of creativity as well. For instance, scientific creativity is often described as a coming together or merging of different lines of thought—referred to, for instance, as “bisociation” by Koestler (1964). Imagery is most often part of the creative process (Einstein, 1945), But the emotion is usually left out in these descriptions. I believe that the emotional element is crucial, though it is difficult to give this emotion a simple name. I suggest a combination of several 27 emotions: first, the “awe-wonder-mystery” (discussed above) that the scientist feels while looking at a problem in nature. Then, there is the “powerful push” or “desperate need” to find a solution or connection. And finally, a “wow” feeling-perhaps another instance of “awe-wonder-mystery”--when a solution is found. Emotion does play an important role, though the imagery does not picture or translate the emotion here as straightforwardly as in artistic creation. Finally, the thymophor, which we have found so prominently in many types of human functioning, clearly must have an underlying brain biology. The details are not certain at present, but we can obtain some rough hints from studies of REM sleep, the brain state most associated with dreaming, and also from studies of the brain’s “default network”. (fig 2 about here) First of all, thymophor generally occurs during the kind of mental activity which we situate towards the right end of the continuum of mental functioning (Figure 2) which we have discussed in detail elsewhere (Hartmann, 2007, 2010b, 2011). This continuum of mental functioning is also a continuum of brain (chiefly cerebral cortical) functioning. Thymophor seems to occur chiefly in states of artistic reverie, daydreaming and dreaming. And there is a great deal of overlap between these states. Dreaming is not “totally different” from the rest, as we sometimes seem to believe. Studies of activation patterns during REM sleep can give us a hint, since we have seen thymophor so prominently in dreaming(Braun et al, 1997;Maquet et al, 1996; Maquet et al, 2004; Nofzinger et al, 1997; Sutton et al, 1996; Jakobson et al, 2012). I believe that the decreased activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and increased activation of ventromedial portions of the frontal cortex certainly play a role in the biology of thymophor. The very definition of thymophor—emotion—translated into imagery— suggests roughly what may be going on in the brain. Imagery implies widespread cortical activation. The powerful role of emotion implies subcortical input from the amygdala and related regions. More specifically, since thymophor occurs not only in dreaming, but also in “artistic reverie”, or daydreaming, the underlying biology must be related to the brain biology underlying these states, which has been called the brain’s default network (Buckner, 2008). We note that there’s considerable overlap between the 28 brain biology of this default network and the brain biology of REM sleep. Thus, thymophor occurs at times of activation of ventromedial PFC and activation of the various medial temporal and parietal regions involved in the default network. But all this is only the background within which thymophor occurs. The PET and fMRI techniques, establishing regions of activation, as above, have excellent spatial resolution, but they have poor temporal resolution. They cannot show spread of activation. The temporal element in thymophor is very important, since thymophor can occur in very short time intervals within the dream and in creative reverie. A rapid spread of activation and simultaneous or near-simultaneous activation of cortical and subcortical regions is most likely involved, However, exploring these temporal aspects will have to wait for studies using magnetoencephalography (MEG) and similar techniques. Conclusion Much is still unknown. But I hope I have convinced the reader that this strange beast we have called thymophor –-the translation of emotion into imagery – actually does exist, and that thymophor is an important element in human creativity including dreams and art, and that it may play a role in memory as well. It is at least worth exploring further. ……………………………………… 29 Figure 1. Scoring dreams for the Central Image SCORING DREAMS FOR THE CENTRAL IMAGE Definition: A Central Image (contextualizing image) is a striking, arresting, or compelling image — not simply a story — but an image, which stands out by virtue of being especially powerful, vivid, bizarre, or detailed. List of Emotions 1. fear, terror 11. supremacy 2. helplessness, vulnerability, being trapped, being immobilized 12. 3. anxiety, vigilance 13. excitement 4. despair, hopelessness (giving up) 5. anger, frustration 6. disturbing — cognitive dissonance, disorientation, weirdness 7. guilt 14. 8. grief, loss, sadness, abandonment, disappointment 15. 16. 9. shame, inadequacy 17. 10. disgust, repulsion 18. power, mastery awe, wonder, mystery happiness, joy, hope peace, restfulness longing relief, safety love (relationship) If there is a second contextualizing image in a dream, score on a separate line. Dream 1. CI? ID# (Y/N) 3. Intensity 2. What is it? (rate 0 – 3) 4. What emotion? 5. Second emotion? 30 Figure 2 A Continuum of Mental Functioning Although we often consider dreaming to be “totally different” from the other states, research shows that there is actually considerable overlap. 31 References Barrett, D. (2001) The Committee of Sleep. New York: Crown Publishers. Barrett, D. (Ed.) (1996) Trauma and Dreams. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Braun, A., Balkin, T., Wesenten, N. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. Brain 120: 7. Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. In (Eds.) Kingstone, A. & Miller, M. B., Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. pp. 1-38. Einstein, A. (1945) Letter to Jacques Hadamard. Eliot, TS (1917) Prufrock and Other Observations. Egoist Press: London. Garfield, P. L. (1988) Women’s Bodies, Women’s Dreams. New York : Ballantine Books. Hartmann E (1996): Outline for a Theory on the Nature and Functions of Dreaming. Dreaming, 6: 147-170. Hartmann, E. (1998) Dreams and Nightmares: The New Theory on the Origin and Meaning of Dreams. New York: Plenum Press. Hartmann, E. (1999) Dreams Contextualize Emotion: A new Way of Understanding Dreams and Dream Symbolism. Psychoanalytic Dialogues. 9: 779-788. Hartmann, E. (2007) The Nature and Functions of Dreaming. In Barrett,D., and McNamara P., eds: The New Science of Dreaming. Praeger, Westport. Vol III, pp 171-192. Hartmann, E. (2008) The Central Image (CI) makes “Big” dreams Big: The Central Image is the emotional heart of the dream. Dreaming, 18, 44-57. Hartmann, E. (2010a). The dream always makes new connections: The dream is a creation, not a replay. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 5, 241-248. Hartmann, E. (2010b). Meteorite or gemstone? Dreaming as one end of a continuum of functioning: Implications for research and for the use of dreams in therapy and self-knowledge. Dreaming, 20, 149-168 Hartmann, E. (2011). The nature and function of dreaming. New York: Oxford University Press. 32 Hartmann, E., Basile, R. (2003). Dream Imagery Becomes More Intense After 9/11/01. Dreaming, 13:61- 66 Hartmann, E., Brezler, T. (2008). A systematic change in dreams after 9/11/01. Sleep. 31: 213-218 Hartmann, E., Kunzendorf, R. (2000). Constructing a dream in the laboratory. Sleep, 23S, p. A102. Hartmann,E. and Kunzendorf, R. (2005-2006) The Central Image (CI) in Recent Dreams, Dreams that Stand Out, and Earliest Dreams: Relationship to Boundaries, Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 25, 383-392. Hartmann, E., Kunzendorf, R., Baddour, A., Chapwick, M., Eddins, M., Krueger, C., Latraverse, T., Shannon, R. (2002-2003). Emotion Makes Daydreams More Dreamlike, More Symbolic. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality. 22: 255-274 Hartmann, E., Kunzendorf, R., Rosen, R., Grace, N. (2001). Contextualizing Images in Dreams and Daydreams. Dreaming, 11:97-104 Hartmann E, Rosen R, Gazells N, Moulton H, (1997): Contextualizing Images in Dreams — Images That Picture of Provide a Context for an Emotion. Sleep Research, 26, 274 Hartmann E, Rosen R, Grace N, (1998). Contextualizing Images in Dreams: More Frequent and More Intense After Trauma. Sleep, 21S, p. 284. Hartmann, E., Stickgold, R. (2000) Contextualizing images in content obtained from different sleep and waking states. Sleep, 23S, p A172 Hartmann E., Zborowski, M., Rosen, R., Grace, N., (2001). Contextualizing Images in Dreams: More Intense After Abuse and Trauma. Dreaming, 11: 97-104 Ilg, A. (1885) Messerchmidt, Franz Xaver. Allgemeine Deutsche Biographie. Liepzig: Dunker and Humblot. Jakobson, A. J., Laird, A. R., Maller, J. J., Conduit, R. D., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2012). Brain Activity in Sleep Compared to Wakefulness: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 2(2), 249-257. Knudson, R. (2001). Significant Dreams: Bizarre or Beautiful. Dreaming 11: 167-177 Koestler, A. (1964) The Act of Creation. New York, 13. Kunzendorf R., Veatch J. (2013) Envisioning the Dream through Art and Science. Amityville, NY: Baywood Publishing Company Lakoff, G. (1993) How metaphor structures dreams: the theory of conceptual metaphor applied to dream analysis. Dreaming 3: 77-98. 33 Lakoff, G, Turner, M. (1989). More than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to Poetic Metaphor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, G., Johnson, M. (1980) Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Maquet, P., Peters, J., Aerts, J., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Franck, G., (1996). Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature, 383, 163-166. Maquet, P., Ruby, P., Schwartz, S., Laureys, S., Albouy, G., Dang-Vu, T., Desseilles, M., Boly, M., Melchior, G., & Peigneux, P. (2004). Regional organization of brain activity during paradoxical sleep (PS). Arch Ital Biology, 142, 413-419. Maybruck, P. (1989). Pregnancy and Dreams: How to Have a Peaceful Pregnancy by Understanding Your Dreams, Fantasies Daydreams, and Nightmares. Los Angeles: JP Tarcher. Miyushita, Y. (2004). Cognitive Memory: Cellular and Network Machineries and Their Top-Down Control. Science Magazine, 306: 435-440. Nash, Paul. (1986) Philosophies of Knowledge. In Encyclopedia Britannica Macropedia. Vol 25 710 Nietzsche, F. (1885) Thus Spoke Zarathustra: A Book for All and None Nishida, M., Pearsalll, J. , Buckner, R., Walker, M. (2009) REM Sleep, prefrontal theta, and the Nofzinger, E., Mintun, M., Wiseman, M., Kupfer, J. (1997). Forebrain activation during REM sleep: An FDG PET study. Brain Research. 770: 192-201 Oxford Dictionary of Literary Terms. (2008) Oxford University Press: Oxford. Pound, E., (1934). ABC of Reading. New Haven: Yale University Press Proust, M. (1913) A la recherché du temps perdu. Paris: Grasset and Gallimard. Rice, G., Powel, H. (Eds.) (1932) Alumnus Football. New York: Harper & Brothers. Siegel, A. (1996) “Dreams of Firestorm Survivors” In D. Barrett (Ed.) Trauma and Dreams Cambridge, MA. Harvard University Press. Sutton, J., Breiter, H., Caplan, J., Huang-Hellinger, K., Kwong, J., Hobson, A., & Rosen, B. (1996) Human brain activation during REM sleep detected by fMRI. Neuroscience Abstracts. 22: 690. Van de Castle, R. L. (1994). Our dreaming mind (p. 250). New York: Ballantine Books. 34