1 Coaching in the Wild: Identifying Factors that Lead to Success Abstract: While executive coaching has been shown to be effective, few research initiatives have attempted to understand the importance of the emergent relationship between a coach and coachee. This paper explores the factors that influence coaching outcomes from both the coach and coachees’ perspective and presents the results of the mediating effect that working alliance and information sharing have on coachee goal attainment and coachee insight outcomes. The authors explore these factors in both an academic coachee sample as well as a field sample. Results show that coachee motivation is significantly positively related with coachee goal attainment and coachee insight in the academic sample, but not a field sample. Moreover, working alliance and information sharing partially mediate the relationship between a coach’s psychological mindedness and coachee insight in an academic but not field sample. Other notable results are that the difficulty of the coaching goal does not impact how successful the coaching engagement is in terms of goal attainment. Implications of these findings for both research and practice are discussed. 2 Coaching in the Wild: Identifying Factors that Lead to Success In lieu of utilizing conventional training methods, organizations have been increasingly implementing coaching as a means of improving different employee outcomes (Feldman & Lankau, 2005; Quick & Macik-Frey, 2004; Diedrich, 2001) to cope with the increased demands of the modern workplace (MacKie, 2014). Coaching can be defined as a relationship between a consultant and an individual with the ultimate goal of facilitating professional development and achieving a host of other targeted outcomes (Feldman & Lankau, 2005). Coaching, and especially executive coaching, is one of the fastest growing industries in recent years and is being used more frequently for the professional development of workers. Although coaching is more prevalent, there still remains a lack of evidence for its effectiveness (MacKie, 2014). It is unknown, at a high level, precisely what factors within the coaching process facilitate certain outcomes (Bacon & Spear, 2003). This stems from a number of problems within the extant literature. To begin, case studies were one of the most common techniques utilized in early coaching research. Such studies vary widely in consistency, making it difficult to generalize results (Passmore & Gibbes, 2007). Recent research has been more quantitative in nature, but it is often conducted within organizations, which complicates the interpretation and generalizability of findings (Grant, Passmore, Cavanagh, & Parker, 2010) as a number of organizational factors may contaminate results (Luthans, Luthans, Hodgetts, & Luthans, 2001). Moreover, the coaching process remains largely unstandardized and, consequently, interventions are generally inconsistent across organizations; a host of different outcomes may serve as the focus of a coaching program (De Haan & Duckworth, 2013). There is also a general lack of 3 theoretical grounding for a large number of studies conducted (Grant, 2013). Coaching studies have also been noted to lack rigorous methodology. For instance, a recent meta-analysis ultimately included eighteen studies, but only four were identified as truly rigorous (i.e., utilizing a between-subjects design, collecting data other than self-report, and conducted within field settings; Theeboom, Beersma, & van Vianen, 2014). The meta-analysis indicated that coaching was positively and significantly related to five critical identified outcomes, including performance, coping, work attitudes, goal-directed self-regulation, and well-being (Theeboom et al., 2014), but, given the limitations of the primary studies and coaching literature in general, it is difficult to determine precisely what fostered these specific outcomes. In sum, a dearth of theory and inconsistent as well as diverse methodologies and outcomes have limited the ability of researchers to conclusively identify which aspects of coaching contribute to its effectiveness. While many have made the business case for executive coaching (McGovern et al., 2001; Parker-Wilkins, 2009; Wasylyshyn, 2003), key issues exist within the industry and the field. Sherman and Freas (2004) likened executive coaching to the “wild west.” Even casual exposure to this industry validates their claim. “Effectiveness” in executive coaching may range from improved interpersonal skills, to decreased stress, to better turnaround time in responding to emails; as a result of this lack of consistent outcomes within coaching interventions, outcomes are nearly impossible to systematically assess across interventions (Mackie, 2007). This is a major issue for executive coaching, because it is the systematic assessment of interventions that drives scientific and practical progress. It will be difficult, if not impossible, to maximize the effectiveness of coaching on the whole, until more stringent standards of practice are developed which are based in theory, tested, agreed upon, and continually developed (Lowman, 2007; Mackie, 2007). It has been stated elsewhere that coaching is still in its nascent stages and that 4 much works remain to be done (Kilburg, 2004; Mackie, 2007). For example, research conducted on coaching often fails to account for individual differences, even though constructs like ability, personality, and motivation have consistently been established as influencing the effectiveness of outcomes typically associated with coaching, such as transfer of learning and job performance (Colquitt, LePine, & Noe, 2000). Consequently, it is largely unknown what factors may cause an individual to differ in their responses to coaching. In the aim of addressing this gap, Stewart and colleagues (2008) offered some preliminary evidence that individual factors, including conscientiousness, openness, emotional stability, and general self-efficacy, contribute to the attainment of desired coaching outcomes. Self-awareness, core coaching competencies, and an understanding of the management of coaching relationships have also been identified as significant predictors (Jarvis, lane, & FilleryTravis, 2006). There has also been some work conducted on which coaching behaviors may be most beneficial to facilitating targeted results. Challenge, listening, reflecting back and checking back on understanding have been identified as integral to overall effectiveness (Hall, Otazo, & Hollenbeck, 1999). Yet, despite this initial work, the question of how and what works in contributing to coaching outcomes still remains largely unknown (MacKie, 2014). There is still a need to quantify the impact of coaching behaviors and processes on coachees. We seek to advance the state of research in the field of executive coaching by addressing this gap and taking an empirical approach to understanding which attitudinal and behavioral factors contribute to effective coaching outcomes. Specifically, we will examine inputs, processes, and outcomes in the coaching relationship. We will examine the contributors and barriers to an effective coaching engagement. In conducting this empirical investigation, we will utilize available theoretical rationale derived from the extant coaching literature to inform 5 the development of our hypotheses as well as the identification of constructs which should be examined. The Input-Process-Output Coaching Model We leverage current research on the critical components of coaching and coaching relationships (e.g., coach-coachee working alliance, information sharing, coachee motivation) (de Haan et al., 2013; Bozer, Sarros, & Santora, 2013; McKenna & Davis, 2009), to propose a framework of coaching that addresses the mediating and explanatory mechanisms behind achieving coaching goals. To accomplish this, we first provide a generalizable “input-processoutput” (IPO; McGrath, 1964, 1984) framework for modeling linkages between key coaching variables. In assessing the state of coaching science, Mackie (2007) highlighted the need for a testable, theory-driven model of coaching, but acknowledged the difficulty associated with designing and implementing feasible randomized controlled trials. Rather, he proposed that the best way to validate such a model would be to continue to validate the model against the objective outcomes (e.g., return on investment) as well as by examining the mediating components involved in the coaching process (e.g., motivation). Accordingly, we developed a testable model of contributors to coaching outcomes (see Figure 1). We then proceeded to test its validity in multiple environments. In study 1 we will detail the results of a pilot study comprised of academic coaching engagements within two executive MBA program. In study 2 we will test the model using a field sample of executive coaches and coachees in multiple organizational contexts. 6 Coach and Coachee Inputs As Baldwin and Ford (1988) argue, developmental interventions are driven by three inputs: trainee characteristics, aspects of training design, and external elements of the work environment. Following their logic, coachee characteristics (e.g., coachee personality, motivation), coaching design (e.g., techniques, delivery), and the coachee’s work context should all influence proximal and distal coaching outcomes. Indeed, research frequently points to the importance of these broad categories of variables in driving coaching effectiveness. Bono and colleagues (2009) conducted a wide-scale survey, examining the frequencies and connections between a broad array of coaching inputs, including demographic (e.g., age, race) and professional (e.g., psychologist vs. non-psychologist) information, logistical inputs (e.g., average number of sessions), and coaching techniques, behaviors, and strategies (e.g. challenging questions, reflection exercises). Sue-Chan, Wood, and Latham (2012) examined the effect that coachees’ implicit person theories had on the effectiveness of coaching techniques in eliciting behavioral change. Other inputs that the coaching literature has considered include coachcoachee match (Boyce et al., 2010), coachee personality (Berglas, 2013), specific questioning and feedback techniques (Hicks & McCracken, 2010; 2011), interpersonal communication strategies (Quick & Macick-Frey, 2004), and even the use of literary techniques such as metaphor and storytelling (Robinson, 2010). These are all proposed to influence coaching outcomes in some way, directly or indirectly. An input particularly salient to the coaching domain is coachee motivation. Coachee Inputs 7 Research and theory alike have consistently identified motivation as a key input influencing the obtainment of goals and other salient outcomes across a broad array of work behaviors (e.g., Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1994). Motivation can be conceptualized as “a set of energetic forces that originates both within as well as beyond an individual’s being, to initiate work-related behavior, and to determine its form, direction, intensity, and duration” (Pinder, 1998, p. 11). Highly relevant to motivation is goal-setting theory, as motivation is consistently linked to the achievement of targeted goals both theoretically and empirically (Locke & Latham, 2002); as a primary objective of the coaching relationship is fostering the achievement of set goals, goal obtainment is an outcome that is highly salient to the coaching domain (Witherspoon, 2000). Building on motivation theories explaining behavior in the workplace (e.g., Boyacigiller & Adler, 1991; Shamir, 1991) Meyer and colleagues (2004) described an integrative model of commitment and motivation to explain the specific process by which motivation impacts goal obtainment. They expanded upon Locke and Latham’s (1990) theory of goal setting, which posits that goals high in specificity and difficulty are more likely to facilitate high task performance. Specifically, Meyer et al. (2004) argued that goal regulation plays an integral role in the motivation process, which they defined as “a motivational mindset reflecting the reasons for, and purpose of, a course of action being contemplated or in progress” (p. 998). In other words, motivation is the force influencing goal directed behavior and stems from multiple sources. Relevant to this argument is the concept of intrinsic motivation, which refers to the propensity to engage in activities solely to experience a sense of accomplishment and promote personal growth (White, 1959). It can also be conceptualized as undertaking a task due to associated enjoyment and interest (Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Deci, 2006). Conversely, extrinsic 8 motivation can be defined as “engaging in an activity to obtain an outcome that is separable from the activity itself” (Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Deci, 2006, p. 20; deCharms, 1968; Lepper & Greene, 1978). Individuals who are guided by intrinsic motivation, as opposed to extrinsic motivation, are found to exhibit high task performance (Cerasoli, Nicklin, & Ford, 2014). As individuals high in intrinsic motivation are primarily motivated by working on the task itself, engaging in task completion serves as reinforcement for subsequent behavior aimed at achieving goals associated with the task (Lavigne et al., 2009).This effect has been confirmed within the coaching literature, with motivation being identified as a key component of coachee willingness to change (McKenna & Davis, 2009) and accept coach direction (Peterson, 1996). Therefore, we hypothesize: Hypothesis 1a: Coachee Motivation will be positively related to coachee goal attainment One of the underlying goals of the coaching relationship is for the coach to guide the coachee through the process of implementing behavioral changes that will foster goal attainment (Prochaska et al., 2008; Weinstein et al., 2008). However, for this process to occur, the coachee must first understand the relevance and importance of making such alterations. This requirement can be realized through insight. Insight can be conceptualized as a coachee’s understanding regarding the precise behavioral changes that need to occur to facilitate goal obtainment (Hicks & Peterson, 1999). In other words, insight allows a coachee to comprehensively assess and understand information gleaned from the coaching relationship and subsequently apply that information to further goal attainment and the achievement of other integral outcomes. One factor that can further this process is coachee motivation. 9 Research indicates that intrinsic motivation is associated with deeper, less superficial information processing (Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Sheldon, & Deci, 2004). Coachees must first engage in information processing to make sense of information acquired from the coaching relationship and achieve insight (Sowa, 1984). Information processing can be defined as the underlying mechanisms that explain the manner in which individuals collect, perceive, encode, store, retrieve, and apply information (Jarvinen & Poikela, 2001). As intrinsic motivation “involve[s] learning in the service of inherent psychological needs and growth tendencies” (Vansteenkiste et al., 2004, p. 247), intrinsically motivated individuals are predisposed to more fully engage in the learning process and this promotes deeper information processing. This, in turn, allows coachees to more comprehensively assess information pertinent to goal achievement and this more focused examination furthers insight. Thus, we hypothesize: Hypothesis 1b: Coachee Motivation will be positively related to coachee insight. Coach-Coachee Relationship Coaching places a great emphasis on the coach-coachee relationship (Boyce et al., 2010; McKenna & Davis, 2009), sharing some construct space with mentoring and therapy/counseling (Feldman & Lankau, 2005). Counseling research highlights the therapeutic relationship in eliciting changes in the client (Gassman & Grawe, 2006; McKenna & Davis, 2009). This refers to the relationship between therapist and patient (or coach and coachee), and is evaluated along a number of dimensions such as respect, openness, and affect (DiGiuseppe, Leaf, & Linscott, 1993; Horvath & Symonds, 1991; Saltzman, Luetgert, Roth, Creaser, & Howard, 1976). While there are several theoretical frameworks used to describe the coaching process (Feldman & Lankau, 2005), above all the coaching relationship is one in which the coach and coachee form a collaborative working alliance, set mutually defined goals and devise specific action steps to 10 ultimately achieve goal attainment (Kemp, 2008; Grant et al., 2009). The relationship between the coach and coachee is identified as an integral factor which ultimately determines the success of the coaching intervention (Passmore & Fillery-Travis, 2011). This relationship is characterized by numerous facets, but researchers have identified working alliance and information sharing as two particularly significant attributes comprising this construct (Baron & Morin, 2009; Naficy & Isabella, 2008). We limit the coaching relationship constructs we discuss to information sharing and working alliance, as these two constructs represent both the emotional and informational components of relationship development. While the coaching literature discusses many others (e.g. trust, rapport), we take a more focused examination of these particular relationship variables. Relationship forms the medium and context by which specific coach inputs (e.g., feedback, challenging questions) are delivered (Baron & Morin, 2009; Horvath & Symonds, 1991). For example, de Haan and colleagues (2011; 2013) have looked at how common relationship factors important to coaching, such as empathy, trust, listening, and working alliance influence coaching outcomes such as behavior change and generic perceptions of helpfulness. Moreover, van Woerkom (2010) found that coaches attribute the effectiveness of their coaching to a large part to the relationship they had with their coach. In fact, it was found that when coaches provide unconditional acceptance and respect it was perceived to be directly responsible for change. These findings suggest that a mutual relationship between the coach and coachee will be more effective in facilitating the attainment of coachee goals than a unilateral helping relationship. Working Alliance and Information Sharing 11 Working alliance refers to the quality and strength of the collaborative relationship between a coach and coachee (Bordin, 1979). More specifically, it refers to the trust that emerges from a common relational bond, sense of credibility, and/or a mutual commitment to shared goals (Horvath, Del Re, Fluckiger, & Symonds, 2011), and is often highlighted as a major component of the coaching relationship (de Haan, Culpin, & Curd, 2011; Wasylyshyn, 2003). It has been argued that working alliance plays an important role in predicting coaching outcomes (Baron & Morin, 2009), and that the coaching relationship has even been said to be the ‘vehicle for change’ (Gyllensten & Palmer, 2007, p.163). It is asserted that when working alliance is strong, it enables deeper discussions of thoughts, feelings, and values and overall greater information sharing (Haeussler, 2011) which translates to and facilitates both coachee goal attainment outcomes and coachee insight outcomes. In the context of the coaching relationship, working alliance can be conceptualized as comprised of three aspects: (1) the bond that develops between the coach and coachee, (2) the goals associated with the coaching relationship, and (3) the tasks required to achieve those goals (Baron & Morin, 2001). Moreover, aspects such as mutual respect/trust and shared goals are also frequently identified as composing working alliance (Horvath, 2005; Bluckert, 2005). Coach Inputs The proposed framework, as previously mentioned, draws from Baldwin and Ford’s (1988) model of training effectiveness. Baldwin and Ford’s (1988) model posits that elements such as individual characteristics and behaviors drive the training process. Similarly, we argue that individual characteristics and behaviors serve as integral inputs within the coaching process. Specifically, we posit that couch inputs are necessary to facilitate the coaching process. These inputs influence the coach-coachee relationship which ultimately influences immediate coaching 12 outcomes. Research and theory identify psychological mindedness and coach behaviors as two influential coach inputs. A salient coach input explaining why some coachees may benefit more from the coaching relationship than others is psychological mindedness. Psychological mindedness can be defined as “a person’s interest and ability to be in touch with and reflect on his or her psychological states and processes” (Nyklicek & Denollet, 2009, p. 32). This input can help facilitate the coach-coachee relationship by fostering different processes. Specifically, it can facilitate working alliance. In the context of clinical therapy, there is evidence that psychological mindedness facilitates a high-quality working alliance among therapists and clients (Castonguay, Constantino, & Grosse Holtforth, 2006) as those with high psychological mindedness have a better awareness of the motives and experiences of others (Gough, 1957; 1975). We extend this argument to the coaching literature, arguing that coaches who have a better sense of their emotions and psychological states will be better able to understand the motives, experiences, and psychological states of their coachees and consequently tailor their guidance in a manner conducive to helping that particular individual. Providing this customized guidance is likely to enhance the coachee’s perception of the relationship as helpful and promote a high-quality relationship. We therefore hypothesize: Hypothesis 2a: Coach psychological mindedness will be positively related to working alliance We further argue that coaches high in psychological mindeness are also more likely to engage in and foster information sharing. Information gathered from multiple sources results in a larger amount of informational resources (Henningsen & Henningsen, 2006); it is thus critical that both coaches and coachees engage in information sharing. Doing so allows the coachee to 13 obtain novel information pertaining to goal attainment. However, certain characteristics of the coach are likely to influence the degree to which information sharing occurs. Psychological mindedness is particularly influential in this regard because coaches with high psychological mindedness have an acuter grasp of their thoughts and feelings (Beitel, Ferrer, & Cecero, 2004). This deeper understanding enables these coaches to more easily draw information from their perspective and clearly articulate and provide information to the coachee which, in turn, can motivate the coachee to reciprocate this behavior by sharing information gleaned from their own perspective (Haeussler, 2011). We argue that this consequently results in a high degree of information sharing between the coach and coachee. We thus posit: Hypothesis 2b: Coach psychological mindedness will be positively related to information sharing between the coach and coachee Coach Behaviors In addition to individual differences, coach behaviors are an integral aspect of facilitating pertinent outcomes. The coaching literature has identified a myriad of coaching behaviors that can foster positive outcomes as a result of coaching interventions. However, we argue that several behaviors are especially salient to the coaching relationship including rapport building, providing content, coaching authentically, regulating motivation, and boundary setting. Rapport building refers to “mutual understanding, agreement, and liking between the client and coach” (Boyce & Neal, 2010, p. 917). Boyce and Neal (2010) further noted that rapport building includes reducing differences between the coach and coachee in addition to building similarities. Engaging in such behavior lends itself to the development of a high-quality working alliance by increasing the bond between coach and coachee. 14 We define providing content as a coach behavior aimed at structuring the coaching relationship around goal achievement, encompassing several relevant constructs such as assessment (Gettman, 2008) to yield a more parsimonious behavior. This includes behavior such as verbally providing goal relevant feedback and guidance. We argue that this will promote working alliance because it entails the coach taking a more active part in the relationship and promoting a collaborative relationship. Boundary setting is another coach input that serves a similar purpose; this behavior can be defined as the coach providing clear guidelines for goal achievement (e.g., Albrecht, 2006). We argue that the coach demonstrating such behavior will assure the coachee of the coach’s investment in the relationship and, in doing so, further working alliance. Coaching authentically is another coach behavior that functions similarly; this refers to the coach promoting an authentic sense of mutual respect and commitment between the coach and coachee (e.g., Noer, 2004). This can include engaging in behaviors geared towards goal achievement. A sense of commitment is argued to be necessary for the establishment of a working alliance (Baron & Morin, 2009) and this behavior fosters commitment. Yet another coach behavior integral to the development of working alliance is regulating motivation, which refers to engaging in various strategies to enhance motivation to further the attainment of relevant outcomes (Wolters, 1999; 2003). In the context of the coaching relationship, this can be conceptualized as the strategies and behaviors the coach utilizes to motivate the coachee. As previously argued, coachee motivation can foster a high-quality working alliance, as motivated individuals are more likely to invest energy in activities that will further goal attainment, such as building a working alliance with the coach (Baron & Morin, 2009). Thus, we argue that each of 15 these behaviors work in tandem to foster a high-quality relationship and promote a strong working alliance between the coach and coachee. Consequently, we hypothesize: Hypothesis 3a: Coach behaviors a) rapport building, b) providing content, c) coaching authentically, d) regulating motivation, and e) boundary setting will be positively related to coach-coachee working alliance. In addition to promoting working alliance, we posit that coach behaviors, consisting of rapport building, providing content, coaching authentically, regulating motivation, and boundary setting, will also foster information sharing. Rapport building and coaching enable the coachee to develop a high-quality relationship with the coach (Boyce & Neal, 2010). This, in turn, can develop an associated sense of trust which generally promotes greater information sharing among entities (Abrams, Cross, Lesser, & Levin, 2000). Providing structure and clarification through the behaviors of boundary setting and structuring content requires communicating information pertinent to goals (Gettman, 2008); as coach information sharing can influence the coachee to reciprocate by sharing information from their own perspective (Haeussler, 2011), these behaviors promote overall, collective information sharing between the coach and coachee. Moreover, by engaging in the behavior of regulating motivation, the coach can further foster information sharing by encouraging a sense of motivation in the coachee; highly motivated individuals are more likely to invest the necessary energy into undertaking actions that will further goal achievement (Baron & Morin, 2009). This may entail information sharing. Consistent with these arguments, we hypothesize: 16 Hypothesis 3b: Coach behaviors a) rapport building, b) providing content, c) coaching authentically, d) regulating motivation, and e) boundary setting will be positively related to coach-coachee information sharing. Working alliance is a robust predictor of positive outcomes within the therapy domain (Horvath & Symonds, 1991; Martin, Gaske, & Davis, 2000).Within the context of the coaching relationship, working alliance has been argued to function in a similar manner by lending credibility to the coach and promoting trust (Murphy, 2005). Enhancing source credibility increases the likelihood of behavioral change targeted at goal achievement and more focused processing of information provided by the coach (Hovlan & Weiss, 1951; Missirlian, Toukmanian, Warwar, & Greenberg, 2005). In other words, working alliance fosters goal attainment and insight. When the coachee engages in mutual information sharing with the coach, more information is generated than when the coachee is assessing information from their perspective alone (Naficy & Isabella, 2008). Consequently, more information is available to be leveraged to obtain insight and applied to goal attainment. Drawing from the training literature, Baron and Morin (2009) posited that coachee inputs can enhance working alliance. They argued that the participant’s underlying motivation to acquire and transfer skills associated with the coaching relationship “constitutes a prerequisite for establishing a working alliance with the coach” (p. 92). In other words, motivated individuals are more likely to invest energy in building a working alliance with the coach because of an innate propensity to engage in activities promoting growth and learning (Vansteenkiste et al., 2004). Similarly, coaches who engage in the aforementioned coaching behaviors and who are high on psychological mindedness are more likely to achieve goal attainment and coachee insight through the strong positive relationships that are elicited by their behaviors. These aspects of the coaching relationship serve as a partial 17 explanatory mechanism explicating the relationship between individual motivation and the achievement of pertinent outputs. Consequently, we hypothesize: Hypothesis 4: Working alliance between the coach and coachee will partially mediate the relationship between coach psychological mindedness and (a) coachee insight and (b) coachee goal attainment. Hypothesis 5: Working alliance between the coach and coachee will partially mediate the relationship between coach behaviors and (a) coachee insight (b) coachee goal attainment. Hypothesis 6: Working alliance between the coach and coachee will partially mediate the relationship between coachee motivation and (a) coachee insight and (b) coachee goal attainment. Hypothesis 7: Information sharing between the coach and coachee will partially mediate the relationship between coach psychological mindedness and (a) coachee insight (b) coachee goal attainment. Hypothesis 8: Information sharing between the coach and coachee will partially mediate the relationship between coach behaviors and (a) coachee insight and (b) coachee goal attainment. Hypothesis 9: Information sharing between the coach and coachee will partially mediate the relationship between coachee motivation and (a) coachee insight and (b) coachee goal attainment. Goal Difficulty Goal difficulty refers to the complexity and relative effort required to attain a goal. While executive coaching has been shown to facilitate the achievement of goals, it is important to take 18 into account whether the difficulty of the agreed upon goals impacts the relationship between coaching and goal attainment (Grant, Curtayne & Burton, 2009). While we argue that information sharing and working alliance may facilitate goal attainment, we argue that the relationship between coach-coachee relationship and goal attainment will be weaker for more difficult goals than less difficult goals. In other words, we posit that as goal difficulty increases, the relationship between the coach and coachee will be less effective in facilitating goal attainment. Hypothesis 10: Goal difficulty will moderate the relationship between the coach and coachee (a) working alliance and (b) information sharing and goal attainment, such that the more difficult the goal the weaker the relationship between information sharing and goal attainment. [INSERT FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE] Methods Sample The participants include an executive MBA sample where each student was assigned a coach, as well as an executive MBA student sample in a large northwestern university. 55 coaches and 44 coachees participated in the study. The coachee sample was 47.2 percent male, and participant age ranged from 18 to 41. The coach sample was 71.7 percent male, and participant age ranged from 18-70+. Surveys In developing the survey, we consulted the existing literature in an effort to represent a comprehensive set of elements important to coaching. The final version of our longitudinal 19 survey contains repeated time collection points and four major sections (1) Coach and Coachee inputs (e.g. coachee motivation, coach psychological mindedness), (2) general inputs (e.g. coaching behaviors), (3) interpersonal coaching variables (e.g. information sharing, working alliance), and (4) coaching outcomes (e.g. goal attainment). We employed a survey based approach to collect dyadic data to obtain the perspective of both the coach and coachee. Measures Coaching Behavior. Coaching behavior was a rationally derived measure based on structured interviews with executive coaches (Sonesh, Coultas & Salas, 2014). We extracted behaviors and grouped them into five types of behaviors: rapport building, boundary setting, providing content, regulating motivation, and coaching authentically. To assess the degree to which coaches engaged in each of these behaviors we developed a list of items which allowed the coach to report how often they engaged in each class of behaviors on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). Sample items include “communicating encouragement and care for the coachee” for rapport building, “establishing common ground with the coachee” for boundary setting, “giving feedback that includes performance related incidents” for providing content, “giving coachee homework” for regulating motivation, and “collecting data through interviews” for coaching authentically. For both studies, the reliability for rapport building, boundary setting, providing content, regulating motivation, and coaching authentically were α=0.598-0.606, α=0.614-0.684, α=0.612-0.735, α=0.691-0.868, and α=0.638-0.778. Coach Psychological Mindedness. Psychological mindedness was measured using the 14 item Balanced Index of Psychological Mindedness (BIPM) (Nyklíček, I., & Denollet, J., 2009). 20 Items were on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not true) to 5 (very much true). A sample item includes “my feelings show me what I need”. Reliability for the scale ranged from 0.713 to 0.760. Coachee Motivation. Motivation was measured using an adapted version of the Pelletier, Tuson, and Haddad (1997) Client Motivation for Therapy Scale. This scale includes 15 items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A sample item includes “I am being required to go to coaching”. Reliability for the scale ranged from 0.647 to 0.786. Information Sharing. Information sharing was measured using a 7 item measure on a 7point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very inaccurate) to 7 (very accurate). A sample item includes “we kept in touch effectively”. Reliability for the scale ranged from 0.805 to 0.941. Working Alliance. Working alliance was measured using the 12 item short form of the working alliance inventory scale (WAIS) (Corbiere et al., 2006; Horvath et al., 1989). Either ‘coach’ or ‘coachee’ was substituted as the referent depending on who the survey taker was. A 5point Likert scale was used ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A sample item includes “My coach and I both feel confident about the usefulness of our current activity.” Reliability for the scale ranged from 0.918 to 0.948. 21 Insight. Insight was measured using a 5 item measure adapted from a previously developed scale (McCay-Peet & Toms, 2011). A 5-point Likert scale was used ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A sample item includes “I obtained unexpected insights”. Reliability for the scale was 0.861. Goal Difficulty: To measure goal difficulty we first asked coaches to select from several pre-populated options what the goals they were working towards with their coach were. We provided multiple "other" options that could be manually filled in, in the event that the three biggest goals were not listed. Sample goals that were presented include time management, public speaking, and listening. Once identified, coachees reported the difficulty level of each of the goals they selected on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very difficult) to 7 (very easy). The average difficulty for all of the goals mentioned was calculated for each participant to arrive at an overall goal difficulty score. Goal Attainment: To measure goal attainment coachees were asked to assess how much they believed that the coaching goals they had previously identified were achieved. A 5-point Likert scale was used ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (completely). Results The descriptive statistics and correlations of all measured variables are shown in Table 3. Hypothesis 1a and Hypothesis1b, which asserted that coachee motivation is positively related to coachee goal attainment and coachee insight, was supported (see Table 3). The relationship between motivation and coachee goal attainment was significant with a correlation of r = .30, p < .05. Moreover, there was a significant positive correlation between coachee motivation and coachee insight (r = .56, p = .01). 22 However, Hypothesis 2a, which predicted that coach psychological mindedness would have a positive relationship with working alliance, was not supported (r = .16, p = .32). Hypothesis 2b, which predicted that coach psychological mindedness would have a positive relationship with information sharing, was also not supported (r = -.01, p = .95). Hypothesis 3a and 3b, which asserted that coach inputs (behaviors) are positively related to working alliance and information sharing between the coach and coachee, was partially supported. Out of the five different coaching behaviors explored, it was found that rapport building and coaching authentically behaviors were significantly correlated with information sharing (r = .42, p < .01; r = .40, p < .05, respectively), thereby lending partial support to Hypothesis 3b. Only regulating motivation coach behaviors were significantly correlated with working alliance (r = .38, p < .05), lending partial support to Hypothesis 3a. Mediation analyses To examine whether the coach-coachee relationship mediated the relationship between coach and coachee inputs and coachee outcomes, mediation analyses were performed using the procedures outlined by Preacher and Hayes (2004); the simple mediation macro SPSS was utilized with the Sobel test via bootstrapping. Multiple regression analyses were conducted to assess each component of the proposed mediation model. When examining working alliance as a mediator for coachee insight and coachee goal attainment, we found it did not serve as a significant mediator for relationships between any of the inputs and coachee goal attainment. Thus, there is no support for Hypothesis 4b, Hypothesis 5b, or Hypothesis 6b (see Table 4). There was also no support for Hypothesis 4a, which asserted that working alliance would partially mediate the relationship between coach behaviors and insight. However, of all the coach 23 behaviors, it was found that regulating motivation was positively associated with coachee insight (B = .33, t (38) = 2.03, p < .05). It was also found that regulating motivation behaviors were positively related to working alliance (B = .41, t (38) = 2.58, p < .01). Lastly results indicated that working alliance was positively associated with insight (B = .72, t (38) = 6.14, p < .001). Because both the A path and B path were significant, mediation analyses were tested using the bootstrapping method with bias corrected confidence estimates (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). In the present study, the 95% confidence interval of the indirect effect was obtained with 5000 bootstrap samples (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). Results of the mediation analysis confirmed the mediation role of working alliance in the relationship between regulating motivation behaviors and coachee insight (B = .30, 95% CI [.05, .55]). In addition results indicated that the direct effect of regulating motivation behaviors on coachee insight became non-significant (B = .03, t(38)=.25, n.s.) when controlling for working alliance, thus suggesting partial mediation (see Figure 2). This lends partial support to Hypothesis 5a. It was also found that insight was positively associated with coachee motivation (B = .89, t (47) = 4.63, p < .001) and working alliance (B = .57, t (47) = 5.59, p < .001). Finally, results indicated that working alliance was positively associated with motivation (B = .75, t (47) = 3.50, p < .001). As both the A path and B path were significant, mediation analyses were tested using the bootstrapping method with bias corrected confidence estimates (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). Results of the mediation analysis confirmed the mediating role of working alliance in the relationship between insight and motivation (B = .43, 95% CI [.14, .71]) (see Figure 3). This supports Hypothesis 6a. There was partial support for information sharing as a mediator of coach and coachee inputs and coach insight. There was no support for information sharing as a mediator of the 24 relationship between coach and coachee inputs and goal attainment. Thus there is not support for Hypothesis 7b, Hypothesis 8b, or Hypothesis 9b. Information sharing did not serve as a mediator for the relationship between psychological mindedness and coachee insight or the relationship between coachee motivation and coachee insight, thus there is no support for Hypothesis 7a nor Hypothesis 9a. However, our mediation analyses revealed that while the direct effect of coaching authentically on insight was not significant (B = .03, t (38) = .17, n.s.), coaching authentically was positively related to information sharing (B = .96, t (38) = 3.06, p < .01). Finally, results indicated that the predicted mediator, information sharing, was positively associated with insight (B = .27, t (38) = 3.40, p < .001). Results of the mediation analysis confirmed the mediating role of information sharing in the relationship between coaching authentically and coachee insight (B = .26, 95% CI [.03, .49]), thus suggesting full mediation (see Figure 4). This evidence lends support for Hypothesis 8a. Moderation analyses Goal difficulty was examined as a moderator of the relationship between the coach and coachee’s (a) working alliance, (b) information sharing, and (c) goal attainment. First, we centralized the predictor variables in order to reduce error in the analysis of interactions that could be due to differences in measurement (McClelland & Judd, 1993). In two separate analyses, (a) working alliance, (b) goal difficulty, and (c) information sharing were entered in the first step of the regression analysis. In the second step of the regression analysis, the interaction term between goal difficulty and working alliance was entered and results were not significant; additionally, the regression examining the interaction between goal difficulty and information sharing was not significant. Thus, Hypothesis 10 was not supported. 25 [INSERT TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE] [INSERT TABLE 4 ABOUT HERE] [INSERT FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE] [INSERT FIGURE 3ABOUT HERE] [INSERT FIGURE 4 ABOUT HERE] Study 1 Discussion Study 1 provides initial, partial support for the overall hypothesized model. Results suggest that both coachee and coach individual differences (i.e., inputs) play a role in the development of the coaching relationship. Moreover, these inputs also contribute to coachee insight and goal attainment outcomes. However, contrary to our hypotheses, not all coach inputs were associated with relationship development among the coaches and coachees. Specifically, the coach’s psychological mindedness did not seem to have a direct relationship with information sharing or working alliance. Additionally, the specific coaching behaviors that are related to relationship outcomes differ depending on the outcome assessed. Rapport building and coaching authentically were positively related with working alliance, while regulating motivation behaviors were positively related with information sharing. This suggests that coaches should first identify what outcomes they are striving for in order to accurately assess what behaviors would be most effective. For example, if a coach is concerned with creating a strong working alliance, they should ensure their coaching efforts are authentic. This finding aligns with the person-centered coaching approach identified by Joseph (2006), which highlights the importance 26 of coaching authentically in order to facilitate a social relationship between the coach and coachee. In contrast to our hypotheses, the relationship variables did not mediate all relationships between coachee and coach inputs and outcomes of the coaching engagement. Working alliance was found to partially mediate the relationship between the coach’s engagement in regulating motivation behaviors and coachee insight and the coachee’s level of motivation and insight. The relationship between coaching authentically and coachee insight was partially mediated by information sharing. Interestingly, a significant relationship between coachee insight and coachee goal attainment was identified. This suggests that insight is related to goal attainment, thereby exhibiting the potential to be a predictor of coachee’s goal attainment. As such, coaches should strive for insight as a proximal goal in order to obtain more distal outcomes (e.g., goal attainment) of the coaching relationship. These findings highlight the importance of fostering a strong, high-quality relationship between coaches and their coachee, thereby supporting previous work on this topic (e.g., de Haan, Duckworth, Birch, & Jones, 2013). Finally, while we had hypothesized that goal difficulty would moderate the relationship between each coach-coachee relationship variable and goal attainment, there was no evidence to support this. This indicates that despite the difficulty level of the goals set within a coaching engagement, the working alliance that is built between the coach and coachee and the information exchange that occurs between them is enough to help facilitate and achieve those goals. This speaks to the value and importance of coaching as a tool to develop leaders and managers to accomplish even seemingly unattainable goals. On the other hand, this finding may have been a result of subjective determinations of difficult, as opposed to easy, goals. Future 27 work should consider determining whether there are objectively harder and easier coaching goals that influence the effectiveness of coaching engagements. Although the current findings provide significant contributions to the science and practice of coaching, it is necessary to test the hypothesized model utilizing field data. The MBA sample utilized provides a good representation of the individuals that will ultimately be the consumers of coaching; however, we cannot be certain that the same results will generalize to a field sample of executives. The coaching behaviors that engender results for MBA students may not be the same as what is needed for executives working within the field. For example, the coach’s ability to regulate motivation may not be as necessary when working with an executive because this coachee is already highly motivated to achieve results because their job is at stake. As such, study 2 investigates the hypothesized model utilizing a sample of executive coachees and coaches in the field. STUDY 2 Method In study two we sought to determine whether similar effects would generalize to a field sample of executive coaches and executive coachees. We identified a diverse sample of coaches from both field and academic settings. We identified coaches from large organizations with which executive coaches are usually affiliated: International coaching federation (ICF), Society of Consulting Psychology, and the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology (SIOP). We distributed the survey via Qualtrics to approximately 36 contact points (including practicing executive coaches and coaches working in various academic settings). Coaches were asked to inform their coachees about the survey, allowing the coachee to have the final say as to whether 28 or not the dyad will complete the survey. When coachees completed their surveys, a notification email was automatically sent to their coach counterpart. Approximately 90 executive coachees and 17 field coaches participated in the study. Of these respondents, there were 89 coachcoachee dyads represented. The survey each completed was identical to that of study 1. The coachee sample was 45.1 percent male, and age ranged from 22-69. The coach sample was 43.8 percent male, and age ranged from 34-70+. Study 2 Results The descriptive statistics and correlations of all measured variables are summarized in Table 5. Hypothesis 1a and Hypothesis 1b, which asserted that coachee motivation is positively related to coachee goal attainment and coachee insight, were not supported (see Table 4). The relationship between motivation and coachee goal attainment was insignificant (r = .37, n.s.). Moreover, the correlation between coachee motivation and coachee insight was also insignificant (r = .19, n.s.). Hypothesis 2a, which predicted that coach psychological mindedness would have a positive relationship with working alliance, was not supported (r = .27, n.s.). Hypothesis 2b, which predicted that coach psychological mindedness would have a positive relationship with information sharing, was also not supported (r = -.15, n.s.). Hypothesis 3a and 3b, which asserted that coach inputs (behaviors) are positively related to working alliance and information sharing between the coach and coachee, was not supported. None of the five different coaching behaviors explored were found to be significantly correlated with information sharing or working alliance (see Table 4). 29 However, it was found that coachee insight was positively correlated to both the coachee perception of information sharing and of working alliance (r = .42, p < .01; r = .64, p < .01, respectively). Also, goal attainment was found to be positively correlated with both the coachee perception of information sharing and of insight (r = .38, p < .01; r = .30, p < .01). Mediation analyses To examine the role of the coach-coachee relationship as a mediator between coach and coachee inputs and coachee outcomes, mediation analyses were performed using the procedures outlined by Preacher and Hayes (2004); the simple mediation macro built for SPSS with the Sobel test via bootstrapping was utilized. Multiple regression analyses were conducted to assess each component of the proposed mediation model. We found that working alliance did not significantly mediate the relationships between any of the inputs (coach psychological mindedness, coach behaviors, and coachee motivation) and coachee insight or coachee goal attainment. Thus, there is no support for Hypothesis 4a, Hypothesis 4b, Hypothesis 5a, Hypothesis 5b, Hypothesis 6a, or Hypothesis 6b (see Table 6). Our analyses revealed no significant results for information sharing as a mediator of coach and coachee inputs and coach insight. Thus, there was not support for Hypothesis 7a, Hypothesis 8a, or Hypothesis 9a. The analyses also revealed information sharing was not a significant mediator of the relationship between coach and coachee inputs and goal attainment; therefore, Hypothesis 7b, Hypothesis 8b, or Hypothesis 9b (see Table 6) were not supported. Moderation analyses The same method as in study 1 (i.e., McClelland & Judd, 1993) was leveraged to examine the role of goal difficulty as a moderator of the relationship between the coach and 30 coachee’s (a) working alliance and (b) information sharing, and (c) goal attainment . The interaction term between goal difficulty and working alliance was entered and it was not significant. Moreover, the interaction term between goal difficulty and information sharing was also not significant. Thus, goal difficulty was not a significant moderator of the relationship between (a) working alliance and goal attainment nor (b) information sharing and goal attainment. Therefore there was no support found for Hypothesis 10. [INSERT TABLE 5 ABOUT HERE] [INSERT Table 6 ABOUT HERE] Study 2 Discussion Contrary to the hypotheses, Study 2 results did not mirror Study 1 results. Specifically, both coachee inputs (i.e., motivation) and coach inputs (i.e., psychological mindedness and coaching behaviors) were not significantly related to goal attainment or insight. The nonsignificant findings are unexpected, and may be due to the small amount of variance within the independent variables. Specifically, the standard deviation for coachee motivation was low, suggesting that most coachees display high levels of motivation. The same pattern was found for coach psychological mindedness and the coaching behaviors. Because the current methodology was limited to self-report measures, both coaches and coachees may have inflated scores due to individuals’ tendency to respond in socially desirable ways . This is consistent with research identifying this bias in self-reports of behaviors (Goode & Hatt, 1952), attitudes (Arkin & Lake, 1983), and personality (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960). Nevertheless, results do suggest that relationship variables, information sharing and working alliance, are significantly related to goal attainment and coachee insight. This suggests 31 that relationship outcomes are more related to, and matter more for desired outcomes, in comparison to coach and coachee inputs. Additionally, this provides initial support for the hypothesis that relationship outcomes partially mediate the relationship between inputs and outcomes. However, when we further tested this hypothesis utilizing mediational techniques, no support was found. Specifically, working alliance and information sharing were not significant, partial mediators of the relationships between coach/coachee inputs and coaching engagement outcomes. This finding runs contrary to the results of Study 1 and previous research (e.g., de Haan, Duckworth, Birch, & Jones, 2013). Because the current data is cross-sectional, this procedure may have hindered the ability to identify causal relationships. Maxwell and Cole (2007) found that cross-sectional mediation analyses lend themselves to biased estimates of longitudinal statistics. Moreover, the interpretability of mediational relationships utilizing crosssectional data is argued to be questionable (Maxwell, Cole, & Mitchell, 2011). Collecting longitudinal data with executive coaches, however, proves to be a lofty challenge. Although executive coaches may work with executives over several months, having to report on their effectiveness throughout the coaching relationship is not something that is desired or practical. If possible, future research should investigate the proposed mediational hypothesis in a longitudinal study utilizing executive coachee and coach dyads. Contrary to the hypotheses, but in line with the results of Study 1, goal difficulty did not moderate the relationship between relationship variables and goal attainment. This provides further support for the promising finding that despite goal difficulty the quality of the relationship between executives and their coach serves a compensatory function to achieve coachee goals. General Discussion 32 Findings from study 1 and study 2 suggest that the study sample employed influences coaching effectiveness. It is evident that coachee motivation matters more in an academic sample than in a field sample. One reason for this may be that field sample data suffers from ceiling effects, such that coachee motivation is high across the board. At the executive level, this is to be expected. However, the purpose of coaching may play a role in how motivated the coachee is. For example, coahes hired for remediation purposes may still want to engage in regulating motivation behaviors, as it was shown to be effective in the academic sample. Another reason for this discrepancy may be that academic coachees set more attainable goals that can be completed in shorter periods of time than executive coachees. These issues should be more fully explored in future coaching research. It was also found that coachees in an academic sample achieved coachee insight when their coaches reported regulated coachees’ motivation. However, coach behaviors are not significantly related to coachee outcomes of interest in the field sample. This finding suggests that coaching in general might not need to follow any particular strategies but rather what is important is the emergent relationship that develops between an executive and a coach. Both study 1 and study 2 illustrated the importance of information sharing and working alliance as it relates to goal attainment and the achievement of coachee insight. The sense of openness, trust, alliance, and rapport is critical for translating coaching into measurable coachee achievements. The findings suggested that coaches must foster a strong working alliance, rapport, and mutual information sharing, through regulating coachee motivation, by coaching authentically and creating a feeling of mutual respect to facilitate the achievement of coachee insight. Moreover, it was found that coachee insight is a proximal goal that coaches should strive to achieve since it is highly correlated with more distal goal attainment outcomes. For novice coachees or for coachees not 33 fully entrenched in the executive world it would seem that motivation is a critical ingredient to success. It is necessary for the coach to activate the coachees’ strengths, resources, and sense of personal agency. However, It is not all about the coach and the techniques or behaviors they employ bur rather it is important for the coach to understand the readiness and ‘coachability’ of the coachee to best coach them and achieve a strong relational bond with the coachee. Finally, our findings suggest that coaches and coachees should not shy away from difficult and challenging goals. In both studies it was found that regardless of self-reported goal difficulty the relationship between working alliance and information sharing remained significant. This is an encouraging finding considering the lofty and challenging objectives coaches and coachees jointly set for their coaching engagements. Limitations There are several limitations in our study that must be noted. First, coachee participants who voluntarily chose to participate in the study may be different from a general set of coachees who may not be as engaged in the coaching process and motivated to advance the science of coaching. Moreover, we had a relatively low dyadic sample size, as such shared coach and coachee perceptions of their emergent relationship could not be captured. Rather, we explored each perspective separately using single source data. Future research should attempt to collect more data where true dyadic analyses can be conducted. Moreover, future research should collect third party data to determine whether intended coaching goals were attained from the perspective of stakeholders. For example, if the coaching goal was to become a better leader, gathering data from the leader’s subordinates would be a more accurate source than solely relying on self-report data collected from the coach or coachee. In the same way, the use of self-report measures also complicates the interpretation of findings and leads to limited evidence pertaining to the impact 34 of the coaching process on organizational outcomes (Spence, 2007). While we collected multisource data, which helps to quell this issue, more objective data would largely strengthen understanding of whether coachees ultimately achieve their goals. Another limitation is that our data is cross-sectional, which limits our ability to make causal inferences. Longitudinal data is the next step to better understanding how the coachcoachee relationship evolves over time and whether it is predictive of proximal and distal coaching outcomes. Finally, because coaches who participated in this research were asked to respond with specific coachees in mind, they may have chosen to recall coaching engagements that were particularly successful and coachees with whom they had relatively strong relationships with. As such, we may have suffered from range restriction, and ultimately lost variance in responses. Future work should aim to randomly select which coaching engagements coaches recall, in order to attain a more representative and accurate representation of the dynamics that emerge within coach-coachee interactions. Conclusion Executive coaching has emerged as an important tool for the development of managers, executives, and leaders. It aims to contribute to career development and to ultimately improve organizational outcomes. While coaching research has been described as a ‘black box’ we have attempted to delineate the why and the how of coaching by exploring the factors of coaches and coachees that influence the coaching relationship and how that, in turn, ultimately influences important coaching outcomes. It is our hope that the results presented here will serve as a stepping stone for both scholars and practitioners of executive coaching and developmental interventions in the workplace. 35 References Abrams, L.C., Cross, R., Lesser, E., & Levin, D.Z. (2003). Nurturing interpersonal trust in knowledge-sharing networks. The Academy of Management Executive, 17(4), 64-77. Arkin, R., & Lake, E. (1983). Plumbing the depths of the bogus pipeline: A reprise. Journal of Research in Personality, 17, 81-88. Bacon, T.R., & Spear, K.I. (2003). Adaptive coaching: The art and practice of a client-centered approach to performance improvement. Palo Alto, CA: Davis-Black. Baldwin, T.T., & Ford, J.K. (1988). Transfer of training: A review and directions for future research. Personnel Psychology, 41, 63-105. Baron, L., Morin, L. (2009). The coach-coachee relationship in executive coaching: A Field study. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 20(1), 85-106. Beitel, M., Ferrer, E., & Cecero, J.J. (2004). Psychological mindedness and cognitive style. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60(6), 567-582. Berglas, S. (2013). Truth-resistant people. Leadership Excellence, 30(3), 11. Bluckert, P. (2055). Critical factors in executive coaching- the coaching relationship. Industrial and Commercial Training, 37(7), 336-340. Bono, J.E., Purvanova, R.K., Towler, A.J., & Peterson, D.B. (2009). A survey of executive coaching practices. Personnel Psychology, 62(2), 361-404. Bordin, E.S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, 16(3), 252-260. 36 Boyacigiller, N.A., & Adler, N.J. (1991). The parochial dinosaur: Organizational science in global context. Academy of Management Review, 16(2), 262-290. Boyce, L.A., Jackson, R.J., & Neal, L.J. (2010). Building successful leadership coaching relationships: Examining impact of matching criteria in a leadership coaching program. Journal of Management Development, 29(10), 914-931. Bozer, G., Sarros, J.C., & Santora, J.C. (2013). The role of coachee characteristics in executive coaching for effective sustainability. Journal of Management Development, 32(3), 277294. Castonguay, L.G., Constantino, M.J., & Holtforth, M.G. (2006). The working alliance: Wehre are we and where should we go? Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 43(3), 271-279. Cerasoli, C.P., Nicklin, J.M., & Ford, M.T. (2014). Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic incentives jointly predict performance: A 40-year meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140(4), 980-1008. Colquitt, J.A., LePine, J.A., & Noe, R.A. (2000). Toward and integrative theory of training motivation: A meta-analytic path of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(5), 678-707. Corbiere,M., Bisson, J., S., & Ricard, N. (2006). Factorial validation of French short-form of the working alliance inventory. International journal of methods in Psychiatric Research, 15(1), 36-45. 37 Crowne, D. P., &Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349-354. DeCharms, R., & Carpenter, V. (1968). Measuring motivation in culturally disadvantaged school children. Journal of Experimental Education, 37, 31-41. De Haan, E., Culpin, V., Curd, J. (2011). Executive coaching in practice: What determines helpfulness for clients of coaching? Personnel Review, 40(1), 24-44. De Haan, E., & Duckworth, A. (2013). Signalling a new trend in executive coaching outcomes research. International Coaching Psychology Review, 8(1), 6-20. De Haan, E., Duckwoth, A., Birch, D., & Jones, C.(2013). Executive coaching outcomes research: The contribution of common factors such as relationship, personality match, and self-efficacy. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice & Research, 65(1), 40-57. Diedrich, R.C. (2001). Lessons learned in – and guidelines for- coaching executive teams. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice & Research, 53(4), 238-239. Digiuseppe, R., Leaf, R., & Linscott, J. (1993). The therapeutic relationship in rational-emotive therapy: Some preliminary data. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 11(4), 223-233. Elliot, A.J., & Harackiewicz, J.M. (1994). Goal setting, achievement orientation, and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 66, 968980. Feldman, D.C., & Lankau, M.J. (2005). Executive coaching: A review and agenda for future research. Journal of Management, 31(6), 829-848. 38 Gettman, H.J. (2008). Executive coaching as a developmental experience: A framework and measure of coaching dimensions (Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation). University of Maryland, College Park, United states. Goode, W. J., & Hatt, P. K. (1952). Methods in social science. New York: McGraw-Hill. Gough, H.G. (1957). Manual for California personality inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Grant, A.M. (2013). The efficacy of coaching. In J. Passmore, D.B. Peterson, & T. Freire (Eds.), Handbook of psychology of coaching and mentoring (pp. 15-39). West Sussex: WileyBlackwell. Grant, A.M., Curtayne, L., & Burton, G. (2009). Executive coaching enhances goal attainment, resilience and workplace well-being: A randomized controlled study. The Journal of Positive Psychology: Dedicated to furthering Research and Promoting Good Practice, 4(5), 396-407. Grant, A.M., Passmore, J., Cavanagh, M.J., & Parker, H.M. (2010). The state of play in coaching today: A comprehensive review of the field. International Review of Industrial & Organizational Psychology, 25, 125-168. Gyllensten, K., Palmer, S., (2007). The coaching relationship: An interpretive phenomenological analysis. International Coaching Psychology Review, 2, 168-177. Haeussler, C. (2011). The determinants of commercialization strategy: Idiosyncrasies in British and German biotechnology. Entrepreneurship: Theory & Practice, 35(4), 653-681. 39 Hall, D.T., Otazo, K.L., & Hollenbeck, G.P. (1999). Behind closed doors: What really happens in executive coaching. Organizational Dynamics, 27(3), 39-53. Hicks, R., & McCracken, J. (2010). Solution-focused coaching. Physician Executive, 36(1), 6264. Hicks, R., & McCracken, J. (2011). How to give difficult feedback. Physician Executive, 37(3), 84-87. Hicks, M.D., & Peterson, D.B. (1999). Leaders coaching across borders. In W.H. Mobley (Eds.), Advances in global leadership (Vol. 1, pp. 297-315). Horvath, A. (2005). The therapeutic relationship: Research and theory. Psychotherapy Research, 15(1/2), 3-7. Horvath, A.O., Del Re, A.C., Fluckiger, C., & Symonds, D. (2011). Alliance in individual psychotherapy. In J.C. Norcross (Eds.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: evidencebased responsiveness (2nd ed., pp. 25-69), New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Horvath, A.O., & Symonds, B.D. (1991). Relation between working alliance and outcome in psychotherapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38(2), 139-149. Hovland, C.I., & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 15(4), 635-650. Jarvinen, A., & Poikela, E. (2001). Modelling reflective and contextual learning at work. Journal of Workplace learning, 13(7-8), 282-289. 40 Jarvis, J., Lane, D.A., & Fikkery-Travis, A. (2006). The case for coaching: Making evidencebased decisions on coaching. London: Chartered Institute of Personal and Development. Kemp, T. (2008). Self-management and the coaching relationship: exploring coaching impact beyond models and methods. International coaching Psychology Review, 3(1), 32-42. Kilburg, R.R., (2004). Trudging Towards Dodoville: Conceptual approaches and case studies in executive coaching. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice & Research, 56(4), 203213. Lavigne, G.L., Hauw, N., Vallerand, R.J., Brunnel, P., Blanchard, C., Cardoretta, I., & Angot, C. (2009). On the dynamic relationships between contextual (or general) and situational (or state) motivation toward exercise and physi9cal activity: A longitudinal test of the topdown and bottom-up hypotheses. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 7, 147-168. Lepper, M.R., & Greene, D. (1978). The hidden costs of reward: New perspective on the psychology of human motivation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Locke, E.A., & Latham, G.P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Locke, E.A., & Latham, G.P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. The American Psychologist, 57(9), 705-717. Lowman, R.L. (2007). Coaching and consulting in multicultural contexts: Integrating themes and issues. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice & Review, 59(4), 296-303. 41 Luthans, F., Luthans, K.W., Hodgetts, R.M., & Luthans, B.C.(2001). Positive approach to leadership (PAL) implications for today’s organizations. Journal of Leadership Studies, 8(2), 3-20. MacKie, D. (2007). Evaluating the effectiveness of executive coaching: Where are we now and where do we need to be? Australian Psychologist, 42(4), 310-318. MacKie, D. (2014). The effectiveness of strength-based executive coaching in enhancing fullrange leadership development: A controlled study. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice & Research, 66(2), 118-137. Martin, D.J., Garske, J.P. & Davis, M.K. (2000). Relation of the therapeutic alliance with outcome and other variables: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 68(3), 438-450. Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychological Methods, 12, 23–44. Maxwell, S. E., Cole, D. A., & Mitchell, M. A. (2011). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation: Partial and complete mediation under an autoregressive model. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 46(5), 816-841. McCay-Peet, L., & Toms, E. (2011). The serendipity quotient. Proceedings of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 48(1), 1-4. McClelland, G.H., & Judd, C.M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 376-390. 42 McGovern, J., Linderman, M., Vergara, M., Murphy, S., Barker, L., & Warrenfeltz, R. (2001). Maximizing the impact of executive coaching: Behavioral change, organizational outcomes, and return on investment. The Manchester Review, 6, 1-9. McGrath, R.J., & Rideout, V.C. (1964). A simulator study of a two-parameter adaptive system. Simulation, 3(1), 45-54. McGrath, J.E. (1984). Groups: Interaction and performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: PrenticeHall. McKenna, D.D., & Davis, S.L. (2009). Hidden in plain sight: The active ingredients of executive coaching. Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 2(3), 244-260. Meyer, J.P., Becker, T.E., & Vandenberghie, C. (2004). Employee commitment and motivation: A conceptual analysis and integrative model: Theoretical models and conceptual analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(6), 991-1007. Missirlian, T.M., Toukmanian, S.G., Warwar, S.H., & Greenberg, L.S. (2005). Emotional arousal, client perceptual processing, and the working alliance in experimental psychotherapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and clinical psychology, 73(5), 861871. Murphy, S.A. (2005). Recource to executive coaching: The mediating role of human resources. International Journal of Police Science & Management, 7(3), 175-186. Naficy, K., & Isabella, I. (2008). How executive coaching can fuel professional and personal growth. OD Practitioner, 40(1), 40-46. 43 Noer, D. (2005). Behaviorally based coaching: A cross-cultural case study. International Journal of Coaching in Organizations, 3, 14-23. Nyklicek, I., & Denollet, J. (2009). Development and evaluation of the balanced index of psychological mindedness (BIPM). Psychological Assessment, 21(1), 32-44. Parker-Wilkins, V. (2006). Business impact of executive coaching: Demonstrating monetary value. Industrial and Commercial Training, 38(3), 122-127. Passmore, J., & Fillery-Travis, A. (2011). A critical review of executive coaching research: A decade of progress and what’s to come. Coaching: An International Journal of Theory, 4(2), 70-88. Passmore, J., & Gibbes, C. (2007). The state of executive coaching research: What does the current literature tell us and what’s next for coaching research? International Coaching Psychology Review, 2(2), 116-128. Pelletier, L.G., Tuson, K.M., & Haddad, N.K. (1997). Client motivation for therapy scale: A measurement of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and activation for therapy. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68(2), 414-435. Peterson, D.B. (1996). Executive coaching at work: The art of one-on-one change. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 48(2), 78-86. Pinder, C.C. (1998). Work motivation in organizational behavior. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. 44 Preacher, K.J., & Hayes, A.F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedure for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 36(4), 717-731. Prochaska, J.O., Butterworth, S., Redding, C.A., Burden, V., Perrin, N., Flaherty-Robb, M., & Prochaska, J.M. (2008). Initial efficacy of I, TTM tailoring and HRI’s with multiple behaviors for employee health promotion. Preventive Medicine, 46(3), 226-231. Quick, J.C., & Macik-Frey, M. (2004). Behind the mask coaching through deep interpersonal communication. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice & Research, 56(2), 67-74. Robinson, E. (2010). The use of literary techniques in coaching. Journal of Management Development, 29(10), 902-913. Saltzman, C., Luetgert M., Roth, C., Creaser, J., & Howard, L. (1976). Formation of a therapeutic relationship: experiences during the initial phase of psychotherapy as predictors of treatment duration and outcome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44(4), 546-555. Shamir, B. (1991). Meaning, self and motivation in organizations. Organization Studies, 12(3), 405-424. Sherman, S., & Freas, A. (2004). The wild west of executive coaching. Harvard Business Review, 82(11), 82-90. Sonesh, S.C., Coultas, C., Salas, E. (2014). How Does Coaching Work? A Mixed Method Analysis. Poster submitted to the 29th annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and 45 Organizational Psychology, Honolulu, HI.Sowa, J.F. (1984). Conceptual structures: Information processing in min and machine. Reading, Mass: Addison-Wesley. Spence, G.B. (2007). Further development of evidence-based coaching: Lessons from the rise and fall of the human potential movement. Australian Psychologist, 42(4), 255-265. Stewart, L.J., Palmer, S., Wilkin, H., & Kerrin, M. (2008). The influence of character: Does personality impact coaching success? International Journal of Evidence Based Coaching & Mentoring, 6(1), 32-42. Sue-Chan, C., Wood, R.E., & Latham, G.P. (2012). Effect of a coach’s regulatory focus and an individual’s impact person theory on individual performance. Journal of Management, 38(3), 809-835. Theeboom, T., Beersma, B., & van Vianen, A.E. (2014). Does coaching work? A meta-analysis on the effects of coaching on individual level outcomes in an organizational context. Journal of Positive Psychology, 9(1), 1-18. Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E.L. (2006). Intrinsic versus extrinsic goal contents in selfdetermination theory: Another look at the quality of academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 41(1), 19-31. Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K.M., & Deci, E.L. (2004). Motivating learning, performance, and persistence: The synergistic role of intrinsic goals and autonomy-support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 246-260. 46 Van Woerkom, M. (2010). The relationship between coach and the coachee: A crucial factor for coaching effectiveness. In S. Billett (Eds.), Learning through practice models, traditions, orientations and approaches (pp. 256-267). Wasylyshyn, K.M. (2003). Executive coaching: An outcome study. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice & Research, 55(2), 94-106. Weinstein, A., Yemini, Z, & Greif, J. (2008). Motivational and behavioral factors predicting success in cigarette smoking-cessation treatment combining group therapy with bupropion. Journal of Groups in Addiction & Recovery, 3(1-2), 79-92. White, R.W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66(5), 297-333. Witherspoon, R. (2000). Starting smart: Clarifying goals and roles. In M. Goldsmith, L. Lyons, & A. Freas (Eds.), Coaching for leadership: How the world’s greatest coaches help leaders learn (pp. 165-185). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Wolters, C.A. (1999). The relationship between high school students’ motivational regulation and their use of learning strategies, effort, and classroom performance. Learning & Individual Differences, 11, 281-299. Wolters, C.A. (2003). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating an underemphasized aspect of selfregulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(4), 189-205.