Sex Without Love: The ‘Whys’ and ‘Why Nots’ Snezana Vrangalova (Cornell University), Ritch C. Savin-Williams (Cornell University) Abstract Method Participants Reasons for engaging in casual sex were examined in a group of University students and staff. 17 distinct types of motives were identified from previous literature and classified using Self Determination Theory. Results indicate that main motives for engaging in CS are Intrinsic or highly internalized extrinsic, whereas reasons for not engaging in it are primarily non-autonomous, particularly for males and those with prior CS experience. Demographics Sex – Fem = 138, Mal = 17 Sexual orientation – Hetero = 78.4%; Non-hetero = 21.6% Age – M = 24.7 (6.1), R = 18-56 CS experience – Yes = 204 (74%); No = (71) 26% Ethnicity – Caucasian = 65%; Asian = 15%; Hispanic = 8.5%; Other = 11.5% Religiousness on a scale 1 (Not at all) to 5 (Very) – M = 2.01 (1.1) Number of total lifetime sex partners Number of lifetime CS partners R = 1-57; M = 11 (11.4); Med = 7.5 R = 1-57; M = 7.9 (10.2); Med = 5 -Defined In this study CS as any form of genital sexual contact including, but not limited to, mutual masturbation, oral sex, penile-vaginal intercourse, or anal intercourse between two or more people who are not currently dating nor emotionally attached to one another. Instruments Motivations For Casual Sex. 54 reasons for engaging in CS identified from previous literature, representing 17 types of motives, were classified according to the level of self-determination. Respondents rated the likelihood of engaging in CS for each (see Table 2). Why is studying CS and CS motivation important? Self-Determination Theory – SDT (Ryan & Deci, 2000) Human behaviors can be carried out with varying degrees of autonomy (selfdetermination), ranging from Intrinsic motivation, doing something for the inherent satisfaction of the activity itself, to Extrinsic motivation, doing something to attain some other goal, which can be internalized to various extent, to Amotivation performing an act without conscious intention (see also Table 1). -Performing an activity for more autonomous reasons is related to greater effectiveness, well-being, and social adjustment. Motivation Against Casual Sex. 17 reasons for NOT engaging in CS identified from previous literature, representing 5 types of motives, were classified according to the level of self-determination. Respondents rated the likelihood of NOT engaging in CS with someone they find physically attractive for each reason (see Table 4). Explore reasons for and against CS engagement and create an meaningful typology of motives using SDT as a framework. Table 1. Types of motivation according to SDT Autonomous Non-autonomous Instrinsic Extrinsic – Introjected Extrinsic – Integrated Extrinsic – External Extrinsic – Identified Amotivation Predictions P1. Autonomous motivation types are more important for engaging in CS, whereas non-autonomous are more characteristic for choosing not to engage in it. P2. Motivation types nearer to one another in the self-determination continuum are more highly, positively correlated than those further away. P3. Male sex and previous CS experience are expected to predict higher endorsement of motivations for and lower endorsement of motivations against CS. Chart 2. Motivation for engaging in CS by sex 3.5 3.5 Experienced Inexperienced Cron. α 0.8046 Type of motivation (# items) Intrinsic (5 items) CS as sexually pleasurable CS as an exciting activity Integrated (7 items) Definition and examples CS as fun and enjoyable e.g. Because casual sex satisfies my sexual needs e.g. Because I like the thrill and novelty of new partners CS as important part of one’s sexuality and overall identity Sexual learning opportunity Physical tension decrease CS as rebellion Identified (16 items) e.g. To gain sexual experience e.g. To relieve sexual tension e.g. I like doing the ‘forbidden’ Achieving an important personal, but separable outcome Mood & self-esteem regulation Sociality enhancement Domination/Submission Intimacy enhancement Introjected (12 items) e.g. It helps me deal with disappointment and loneliness. e.g. To get to know different people and make new friends. e.g. It feels great when the other person finally surrenders to me. e.g. To get emotionally closer to my partner. Motivated by guilt, anxiety, obligation, pride or self-pressure Partner satisfaction Status increase Peer conformity Intimacy avoidance External (6 items) e.g. Because I wouldn’t want to upset my partner. e.g. To score another success. e.g. Because all my friends are also having casual sex. e.g. I am not emotionally ready for a serious relationship. Motivated by desire for direct rewards or fear of punishment Material reward Partner retribution Amotivation (5 items) Coercion Unconscious Total (51 items) e.g. It was a fun way to get free gifts. e.g. To revenge against a current partner for his/her infidelity No autonomy (intentionality) in CS engagement e.g. Because of verbal pressure from the other person. e.g. I was too drunk or on drugs to know what I was doing. Males Females 3 3 2.5 2.5 0.7548 0.8434 2 2 1.5 1.5 0.8018 1 1 Autonomous* 0.7606 0.7110 0.9313 Graph 3. Endorsment of Motivation for Engaging in Casual Sex among CS experienced respondents 4 Non-autonomous Overall Autonomous** Non-autonomous Overall* Intrinsic and internalized motives dominate all respondents engagements in casual sex, although male and CS experienced respondents value autonomous motives more highly than female and the CS inexperienced (Graph 1 & 2). Men endorse several types of motives more than women, including intrinsic, integrated, and introjected reasons, although these gender differences are attenuated among CS experienced respondents (see Graph 3). Males 3.5 Chart 5. Motivation for not engaging in CS by prior CS experience Motivations AGAINST casual sex Females Table 3. Intercorrelation among types of motivation in CS experienced respondents Intrinsic Integrated Identified Introjected External Amotivation Intrinsic 1.0000 0.6762 0.5947 0.5529 0.2790 0.0617 Integrated 1.0000 0.6771 0.6235 0.3189 0.1532 Identified 1.0000 0.6726 0.4326 0.3239 Introjected 1.0000 0.5065 0.4564 External 1.0000 0.4704 Amotivation 1.0000 3 4 Inexperienced Experienced 2.5 3.5 Chart 4. Endorsment of Motivations for not engaging in casual sex 2 3.5 Females 3 Males 1.5 3 2.5 1 Intrinsic** 2.5 Integrated Identified Introjected* External Amotivation Overall As expected, strongest positive correlations were found between motivational forms adjacent to one another on the continuum of self-determination (Table 3). 2 2 Conclusions and future research 1.5 1 1.5 Non-autonomous Overall Autonomous Current study Chart 1. Motivation for engaging in CS by prior CS experience Table 1. 281 subjects filled out an online survey asking them about their attitudes toward and experiences with casual sex, including their reasons for engaging or not engaging in it. Casual Sex (CS) -Many people are having it. More than 70% of undergraduates report at least one CS encounter (e.g. Paul & Hayes, 2002). -It has been proposed that some people possess a relatively stable tendency toward it: sociosexual orientation (Simpson & Gangestad, 1991). -CS engagement and orientation are often accused of being cause and/or indicator of mental health and personality problems (e.g. Grello et al, 2003; Paul & Hayes, 2002). Yet, evidence for such links is very inconclusive (e.g. Schmitt, 2005). We propose that one reason for this is overlooking motivations for people’s involvement/interest in CS. Few studies have explored reasons people engage in CS (Greiling & Buss, 2000; Regan & Dreyer, 1999; Weaver & Herold, 2000) and even fewer the reasons for NOT engaging in it (Weaver & Herold, 2000). These have been primarily descriptive and provided no organizational framework for understanding CS motivation. Motivations FOR casual sex 1 Autonomous Non-autonomous Overall Table 4. Categorization of types of motivation for NOT engaging in CS using SDT Type of motivation (# items) Definitions and examples Cronb α Intrinsic (3) Not enjoyable Identified-Integrated (3) CS against personal morality CS as an unhealthy activity Introjected (7) Fear for physical health Fear for emotional well-being External (5) Fear of social sanctions Overall (18) CS as not sexually pleasurable e.g. I don't like sex without love. Not having CS as important part of overall identity e.g. I think CS is wrong. e.g. I think CS is psychologically unhealthy. Motivated by desire to protect oneself e.g. I'm afraid of getting HIV/AIDS. e.g. I'm afraid I'd feel guilty afterwards. Motivated by fear of punishment e.g. I don't want to get a 'loose' reputation. 0.8509 0.9057 0.7596 0.8142 0.9038 Respondents without CS experience have significantly more reasons of all types for not engaging in it, and females have more than men (not shown here). For all men and CS experienced women, non-autonomous reasons are significantly more important than autonomous ones for not having CS (Graph 4 & 5). Only CS inexperienced women consider both types of reasons equally important. The intercorrelation among motivation types (not shown here) demonstrates a simplex structure for all respondent subgroups. The many reasons people have for engaging and not engaging in casual sex can be placed along a continuum of self-determination. Relatively autonomous reasons (such as finding CS pleasurable or achieving important personal goals) are significantly more important in CS behaviors, than non-autonomous ones (such as conforming to peer or partner pressure, material profit, or being unconscious. On the contrary, reasons for refusing to have CS with an attractive person are primarily of external nature and include fear of emotional and physical hurt and of social repercussions. Future research should examine more closely the structure of the different types of motivations in women and men separately, and to take into consideration the different levels of self-determination when investigating the link between health and CS engagement. References Greiling, H. & Buss, D. (2000). Women's sexual strategies: The hidden dimension of extra-pair mating. J of Personality and Indvidual Differences, 28, 929-963. Grello, C. M., Welsh, D. P., Harper, M. S., & Dickson, J. W. (2003). Dating and sexual relationship trajectories and adolescent functioning, 3, 103-11. Adolescent and Family Health, 3, 103-111. Paul, E. L., & Hayes, K. A. (2002). The casualties of `casual' sex: A qualitative exploration of the phenomenology of college students' hookups. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 19(5), 639-661. Regan, P. C., & Dreyer, C. S. (1999). Lust? Love? Status? Young adults’ motives for engaging in casual sex. JPHS, 11, 1-24. Ryan, R. M. & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development and well-being. Am Psych, 55, 68-78. Schmitt, D. P. (2005). Is short-term mating the maladaptive result of insecure attachment? A test of competing evolutionary perspectives. PSPB, 31, 747-768. Simpson, J. A. & Gangestad, S. W. (1991). Individual differences in sociosexuality: Evidence for convergent and discriminant validity. JPSP, 60, 870-883. Weaver, S. J. & Herold, E. S. (2000). Casual sex and women: Measurement and motivational issues. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 12, 23-41