Cognitive Development David F. Bjorklund Some Basic Concepts in Cognitive Development Cognition: The processes or faculties by which knowledge is acquired and manipulated. Cognition is usually thought of as being mental. That is, cognition is a reflection of a mind. It is not directly observable but must be inferred. Development: Changes in structure or function over time. Structure refers to some substrate of the organism, such as nervous tissue, muscle, or limbs, or—in cognitive psychology—the mental knowledge that underlies intelligence. Function denotes actions related to a structure and can include actions external to the structure being studied, such as neurochemical or hormonal secretions, and other exogenous factors that can best be described as “experience”—that is, external sources of stimulation. Development is characteristic of the species and has its basis in biology. Its general course, therefore, is relatively predictable. Development progresses as a result of bidirectional, or reciprocal, relationship between structure and function, and can be expressed as structure function. Developmental function The species-typical form that cognition takes over time. Individual differences: Differences in patterns of intellectual aptitudes among people of a given age. Issues in Cognitive Development Nature versus nurture, or more properly, how do genes/biology and environment interact to yield the adult phenotype. Stages of Development How Does Cognitive Change Occur? Dynamic Systems Approaches to Development Domain-General versus Domain-Specific Abilities Stability and Plasticity of Intelligence Changes in Representation Changes in Intentional Control Nature versus nurture, or more properly, how do genes/biology and environment interact to yield the adult phenotype. Framing the Nature/Nurture Issue Nature: heredity (nativism) Maturational processes guided by genes Biologically based predispositions Biological unfolding of genes Genetic determinism Nurture: environment (empiricism) Learning: experiences cause changes is thoughts, feelings, and behaviors Environmental determinism Interactionist view: nature & nurture interact Caspi et al., 2002 monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) associated with antisocial behavior in rats and humans Gene on X chromosome controls MAOA (high versus low levels) Examined antisocial behavior in boys with high and low levels of MAOA as function of childhood maltreatment Relationship between childhood maltreatment (none, probable, severe) and MAOA activity (low versus high) on antisocial behavior. Composite Index of Antisocial Behavior 1.25 1 Low MAOA Activity 0.75 0.5 High MAOA Activity 0.25 0 -0.25 -0.5 None Probable Severe Childhood Maltreatment What Does it Mean to Say Something is “Innate”? Representational constraints (or representational innateness) Representations that are hardwired into the brain so that some types of “knowledge” are innate. Knowledge of objects Universal grammar Architectural constraints (or architectural innateness) Ways in which the architecture of the brain is organized at birth, thus limiting the way information can be processed and understood. Inhibitory versus excitatory neurons Global organization (what’s connected to what) Chronotopic constraints (or chronotopic innateness) Limitations on the developmental timing of events, affecting what can be acquired when. Critical (sensitive) periods Some cognitive abilities, such as language, might be most easily acquired during a critical period in development Stages of Development Continuity versus Discontinuity Quantitative versus Qualitative Differences Homogeneity of Cognitive Function Discontinuous (stage-like) versus Continuous changes Dynamic Systems Approaches to Development “Patterns and order emerge from interactions of the components of complex systems without explicit instruction either in the organism itself or from the environment” (Thelen & Smith, 1998, p. 564) Self-organization Phase transitions Nonlinear Relatively abrupt Matrix problem (Siegler & Svetina, 2002) Analogical reasoning problem with 6- to 8-year olds, with objects varying in form, size, orientation, & color Light-colored, large, right-facing mouse is to a light-colored, large, right-facing bird as a light-colored, small, left-facing mouse is to: Light-colored, small,left-facing bird (correct choice) Light-colored, small, left-facing mouse (duplicate error) Light-colored, small, right-facing bird Light-colored, large, left-facing bird Dark-colored, small, left-facing bird Dark-colored rabbit Pattern of changes on matrix problem (Siegler & Svetina, 2002) Domain-General versus Domain-Specific Abilities The Stability and Plasticity of Human Behavior Stability refers to the degree to which people maintain their same rank order in comparison to other children with respect to some characteristic Plasticity refers to the ability to change as a result of experience. Changes in Representation Changes in Intentional Control Basic tenet of evolutionary psychology: The human mind has been prepared by natural selection, operating over geological time, for life in a human group. Some Assumptions of Evolutionary Psychology What evolved are psychological mechanisms (information processing mechanisms are the “missing link” in evolutionary explication) Information processing are relatively independent domain-specific modules designed to solve recurrent problems faced by our ancestors Psychological mechanisms evolved in the “environment of evolutionary adaptedness” An emphasis on adaptationist thinking, which stresses the function of a behavior or trait Evolution occurs via mechanisms described by the Modern Synthesis, with special emphasis on the role of natural selection as proposed by Darwin Basic Tenets of Natural Selection (from Darwin, 1859): Superfecundity Variationtion is characteristic among members of a species Variation is heritable Characteristics that result in individuals surviving and reproducing tend to be selected as a result of the interaction between the individual and the environment Proposed Domains of Mind (From Geary) Predicted Developmental Features of Evolved Cognitive Modules Hierarchical in Nature Sensitive Periods and Child-Initiated Activity Based on Implicit Knowledge Observed Universally Universal Developmental Ontogeny Biologically primary vs. biologically secondary abilities Biologically primary abilities: Cognitive abilities selected in evolution. Acquire universally Universal developmental course Children have high motivation to perform them Biologically secondary abilities: Cognitive abilities built on primary abilities that are culturally determined. Not universal Tedious repetition and external pressure often needed for their mastery Models of Gene-Environment Interaction in Cognitive Development Developmental Systems Approach Development proceeds as a result of the bidirectional interaction between structure and function over time at all levels of organization, from the genetic through the cultural. Epigenesis: “any gene-regulating activity that doesn’t involve changes to the DNA code and that can persist through one or more generations” (Pennisi, 2001) Genetic activity (DNA <-> RNA <-> proteins) <-> structural maturation <-> function, activity A simplified schematic of the developmental systems approach, showing a hierarchy of four mutually interacting components (from Gottlieb) Ducklings still in egg that are afforded sound of mother, brood mates, or self-vocalizations will approach maternal conspecific call hours after hatching. Ducklings denied any species-typical acoustic experience shows no preference Previously described “instinctive” (i.e., no previous experience necessary) behavior is critically dependent upon species-typical experience for its expression Premature Visual Stimulation and Subsequent Acoustic “imprinting” (Lickliter, 1990) Bobwhite quails had part of shell removed 2-3 days before hatching Experimental group: pattern light Control group: no patterned light Auditory preference test: Bobwhite quail maternal call Chicken call No preference Percentage of bobwhite quail chicks that approached the bobwhite quail maternal call, the chicken call, or showed no preference as a function of premature visual exposure (Lickliter, 1990) Percent Preference 100 80 Premature visual stimulation No Premature visual stimulation 60 40 20 0 Bobwhite Call Chicken Call No Preference Discrimination learning set performance for monkeys as a function of the age at which testing was begun (from Harlow, 1959) “There is a tendency to think of learning or training as intrinsically good and necessarily valuable to the organism. It is entirely possible, however, that training can be either helpful or harmful,depending upon the nature of the training and the organism’s stage of development (Harlow, 1959, p. 472). Age at which infants reached the criterion as a function of when training was begun (from Papousek, 1977) “Social contexts evolved in the course of human phylogeny are surprisingly fine-tuned in specificity to provide goodenough environments for the human cortex to unfold, initially intrauterinely, then extrautaurterinely. . . With advances in medical technology, that is, material culture, even very immature nervous systems exist and develop outside the womb. However, the social context of traditional special care nurseries bring with them less than adequate support for immature nervous systems . . . Leading to maladaptations and disabilities, yet also to accelerations and extraordinary abilities (Als, 1995, p. 462) Scarr & McCartney’s model of behavioral development Three types of genotype -> environment effects Passive Biological parents provide both genes and environment for child. Passive effects decrease with age Evocative Temperamental characteristics of child evokes responses from others. Evocative effects remain constant with age, Active Children seek out environments consistent with their genotype Active effects increase with age. Changes in correlations of IQ between siblings at different times Correlation of IQs of adopted siblings (from Scarr & Weinberg, 1978) in childhood: range between .25 - .39 Correlation of IQs of dizygotic (nonidentical) twins (from Mathney et al., 1981): range between .60 - .75 Correlation of IQs of adopted siblings (from Scarr & Weinberg, 1978) in adolescence: .00 Correlation of IQs of dizygotic (nonidentical) twins (from Mathney et al., 1981) in adolescence: .55 “Good enough” parents “ordinary differences between families have little effect on children’s development, unless the family is outside of a normal developmental range. Good enough,ordinary parents probably have the same effects on their children’s development as culturally defined superparents” (Scarr, 1992, p. 15) Resilient children Children who develop social and intellectual competence despite growing up in impoverished “high-risk” environments “Through the process of evolution, parenting has been shaped to protect development; nature has created in ordinary parents a powerful protective system for child development (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998, p. 213) Neuronal Development (neurogenesis) Proliferation Migration Differentiation Synaptogenesis Selective Cell Death The Role of Experience in Brain Development Experience-expectant processes (experienceexpectant synaptogenesis) (Greenough et al.) Experience-dependent processes (experiencedependent synaptogenesis) Neural Darwinism (Edleman) Brain Plasticity Neuronal plasticity Recovery of function from brain damage Slow growth and plasticity Sociocultural Perspectives on Cognitive Development “Cognitive development is an active constructive process that involves beings who are evolutionarily predisposed to live and learn in social context with other ‘like-minded’ beings. They are like-minded in terms of both the neurological system available and the social requirements that are in place” Mary Gauvain, 2001 Lev Vygotsky Development should be evaluated from the perspective of four inter-related levels: Phylogenetic Ontogenetic Microgenetic Sociohistorical Tools of Intellectual adaptation Methods of thinking and problem-solving strategies that children internalize from their interactions with more competent members of society that permit them to use basic mental functions more adaptively Chinese and English number words from 1 to 20 Number Chinese word English word 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 yee uhr sahn suh woo lyo chee bah jyo shi shi yee shi uhr shi shan shi suh shi woo shi lyo shi chee shi bah shi jyo ershi one two three four five six seven eight nine ten eleven twelve thirteen fourteen fifteen sixteen seventeen eighteen nineteen twenty Median level of counting (highest number reached) by age for Chinese and U.S. preschoolers (Miller et al., 1995 Median Counting Level 100 80 60 China US 40 20 0 3 4 Age in Years 5 Zone of Proximal Development ZPD: the difference between a child’s actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and his or her level of potential development determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers. Scaffolding: When experts are sensitive to abilities of a novice and respond contingently to the novice’s responses in a learnikng situation, so that the novice gradually increases his or her understanding of a problem Apprenticeship in Thinking Guided participation refers to adult-child interactions, not only during explicit instruction, but also during the more routine activities and communication of everyday life. Guided participation is “the process and system of involvement of individuals with others, as they communicate and engage in shared activities” (Rogoff et al., 1993, p.6) Some functions of shared remembering in children’s memory development from Gauvain, 2001) Children learn about memory process, for example, strategies Children learn ways of remembering and communicating memories with others, for example, narrative structure Children learn about themselves, which contributes to the development of the self-concept Children learn about their own social and cultural history Children learn values important to the family and the community, that is, what is worth remembering Promotes social solidarity Some Assumptions of Piaget’s Theory Structures (schemes) are unobservable mental systems that underlie intelligence. Structures are most simply viewed as some enduring knowledge base by which children interpret their world. Intrinsic Activity Children are active initiators and seekers of stimulation. Structures are intrinsically active and must be exercised so that they can be strengthened, consolidated, and developed. Piaget made the child not only the focus of development but also its major perpetrator. Piaget on Education Piaget emphasized that the role of teachers should not be to instruct children (that is, to transmit knowledge), but rather to provide opportunities for them to discover knowledge. “Children should be able to do their own experimenting and their own research. Teachers, of course, can guide them by providing appropriate materials, but the essential thing is that in order for a child to understand something, he must construct it for himself; he must reinvent it” (Piaget, 1972, p. 27). The Constructive Nature of Cognition We interpret the world through our own personal perspective, and reality is a construction based on the information in the environment and in our heads. Knowing is an active, constructive process—an interaction between the environment and the active individual – constructivism. Functional Invariants Organization The tendency to integrate structures into higher-order systems or structures. Every intellectual operation is related to all other acts of intelligence. Adaptation Assimilation is the incorporation of new information into already-existing schemes. Accommodation is the modification of a scheme to incorporate new information. Equilibration the organism’s attempt to keep its cognitive structures in balance. Stages of Development The Sensorimotor Stage – birth ~ 2-years Preoperational Period – 2 ~ 7-years Concrete Operational Period – 7 ~ 11-years Formal Operational Period – 11 ~ 16-years Sensorimotor Period Substage 1: The use of reflexes (birth to 1 month). Infants enter the world with a set of inherited action patterns, or reflexes, through which they interpret their experiences. Substage 2: Primary circular reactions (1 to 4 months). Reflexes are extended so that new patterns of behavior are acquired that were not part of the basic biological apparatus with which the child was born. Substage 3: Secondary circular reactions (4 to 8 months). repetitive behaviors that are not based on reflexes but represent the first acquired adaptations of new (that is, not reflexive) behaviors. Sensorimotor Period Substage 4: Coordination of secondary circular reactions (8 to 12 months). One circular reaction can be used in the service of another. The first incidence of goal-directed behavior and the beginning of the differentiation between means and ends (that is, cause and effect). Substage 5: Tertiary circular reactions (12 to 18 months). Children can now make subtle alterations in their existing schemes that are directly related to obtaining a solution to their conundrum. This, Piaget stated, reflects a process of active experimentation. Substage 6: Invention of new means through mental combinations (18 to 24 months). Advent of symbolic function and transition to preoperational period. Expressions of the Symbolic Function deferred imitation language symbolic play mental imagery Object permanence the knowledge that objects have an existence in time and space independent of one’s perception or action on those objects. A-not-B task Invisible displacement Imitation Imitation is the purest example of accommodation. Mutual imitation Neonatal imitation – Meltzoff & Moore (1977) active intermodal mapping Innate releasing mechanisms (fixed-action patterns) ontogenetic adaptations Deferred Imitation Information-Processing Approaches Limited capacity; limited amount of: Space Time Speed Information flows through the system Sensory register Short-term store (STM) Long-term store (LTM) Executive control processes (strategies) Representation of knowledge Declarative (explicit) memory Episodic memory Semantic memory Nondeclarative (implicit, procedural) memory Automatic and Effortful Processes Automatic processes Require no mental effort Are not available to consciousness Do not interfere with other processes Do not improve with practice Do not vary with individual differences in intelligence, motivation, and education Effortful processes Require mental effort Are potentially available to consciousness Interfere with other processes Improve with practice Vary with individual differences in intelligence, motivation, and education Short-term store Digit span Role of knowledge Chi and chess champions Span of apprehension Working memory Baddeley’s model Central executive Articulatory loop Visuo-spatial sketch pad Capacity and Cognitive Development Pascual-Leone’s M-space model Case’s model of developmental differences in efficiency of processing Total processing space Storage space Operating space The Role of Knowledge The Development of Children’s Cognitive Strategies: The Origins of Self-control Cognitive strategies are defined as: goal-directed mental operations used to aid task performance. They are usually viewed as nonobligatory, being deliberately implemented, and potentially available to consciousness. The dominant perspective of strategy development through the mid 1980s Children use a single, dominant strategy on a task Development proceeds by more sophisticated and efficient strategies replacing less sophisticated ones There is a strong, positive relationship between levels of strategy use and task performance Factors such as knowledge base and metacognition have their greatest impact on performance through the implementation of strategies New Directions for Strategy Development Research Assessing multiple and variable strategy use Examining more closely the developmental relationship between strategy use and task performance Developing dynamic systems models of strategy development The application of strategies in more “real-world” contexts Integrating executive function and brain development with strategy development Assessing multiple and variable strategy use There is increasing evidence that children have available to them multiple strategies for any particular task, and that strategies do not develop in a stage-like way. Siegler’s adaptive strategy choice model A “selectionist” approach, based on Darwin’s theory of natural selection Children generate a variety of strategies for any particular task These strategies “compete” for use Some strategies as “selected” and used more frequently than others The average level of strategy sophistication increases with age Less effective strategies are sometimes used when dominant strategies don’t work Siegler’s overlapping wave model of strategy development Microgenetic method Looking at strategy change over brief period of time (months, weeks, or even trials) as opposed years Examples of Arithmetic Strategies Strategy Typical Use of Strategy to Solve 2 + 3 SUM, adding from 1 Say “1, 2, 3, 4, 5” MIN, adding from larger addend Say “4, 5” (or “3 . . . 4, 5”) MAX, adding from smaller addend Say “3, 4, 5” (or “2 . . . 3, 4, 5”) Fact Retrieval Say “5” (within latency guidelines) Finger recognition Hold up 5 fingers and say “5” Decomposition Say “2 and 3 is like 3 + 3 minus 1” Mental Arithmetic Say “5” (beyond lat. guidelines) Guessing Say “4” or “8” Sielger & Jenkins (1989) Microgenetic study with 4- and 5-year-old children, none of whom were using MIN at beginning of study Children given series of single-digit addition problems over 11 weeks All children used multiple strategies, with average sophistication of dominant strategy used increasing over weeks Percentage use of ech strategy (from Siegler & Jenkins, 1989) 40 35 Percentage Use 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 Sum Retrieval Shortcut sum Finger recognition Min Guessing Other Overview of “Chutes and Ladders” study (Bjorklund & Rosenblum, 2002) Participants First grade children (6 years 10 months) Chutes and Ladders Three (3) games of “Chutes and Ladders” over 3 weekly sessions; at least 25 moves per session Math Context Three (3) sets of 12 orally presented arithmetic problems, immediately following the conclusion of each game Number of different strategies used Game context: 2.65 Math Context: 2.89 F(1,21) = 2.65, p > .12 Development of multiple and variable strategy use in a memory task (Coyle & Bjorklund, 1997) Children in grades 2, 3, and 4 5 sort/recall trials with different items and categories on each trial Strategies coded during 2-minute study period: sorting category naming rehearsal Strategies coded during recall clustering Mean number of strategies used by grade Second Grade: Third Grade: Fourth Grade: F(2, 102) = 5.14, p < . 05 1.62 1.91 2.40 Mean number of strategies used by trial Trial 1: Trial 2: Trial 3: Trial 4: Trial 5: 1.72 2.07 2.15 2.10 2.02 F(4, 408) = 5.23, p < .05 Examining more closely the developmental relationship between strategy use and task performance Utilization deficiency (Patricia Miller): Children use a strategy (either spontaneously or following training) but fail to experience any benefit in performance, or experience less benefit from using a strategy than older children. Contrast with: Mediational deficiency: Children are unable to benefit from strategy training Production deficiency: Children fail to produce a strategy spontaneously, but can use a strategy and improve task performance as a result of training Evidence of utilization deficiencies Utilization deficiencies documented for a wide variety of tasks, including arithmetic, memory, reading, selective attention, analogical reasoning) Miller & Seier (1994): Utilization deficiencies observed in 90% of all memory studies examining children’s spontaneous strategy use Bjorklund, Miller, Coyle, & Slawinski (1997). Utilization deficiencies observed in 50% of all memory training studies Patterns of clustering and recall over trials indicative of a utilization deficiency (from Bjorklund, Gaultney, & Coyle, 1992) Clustering and Percentage of Recall 1 0.8 0.6 Recall Clustering 0.4 0.2 0 1 2 3 Trials 4 5 Utilization deficiency in a memory training study (Bjorklund, Schneider,Cassel, & Ashley, 1994) Third and fourth grade children Sort-recall task with categorized words Phase 1: Baseline sorting by category clustering recall Phase 2: Training in organizational strategy Phase 3: Near extension Phase 4: Far (one-week) extension Developing dynamic systems models of strategy development “Patterns and order emerge from interactions of the components of complex systems without explicit instruction either in the organism itself or from the environment” (Thelen & Smith, 1998, p. 564) Self-organization Phase transitions Nonlinear Relatively abrupt Matrix problem (Siegler & Svetina, 2002) Analogical reasoning problem with 6- to 8-year olds, with objects varying in form, size, orientation, & color Light-colored, large, right-facing mouse is to a light-colored, large, right-facing bird as a light-colored, small, left-facing mouse is to: Light-colored, small,left-facing bird (correct choice) Light-colored, small, left-facing mouse (duplicate error) Light-colored, small, right-facing bird Light-colored, large, left-facing bird Dark-colored, small, left-facing bird Dark-colored rabbit Pattern of changes on matrix problem (Siegler & Svetina, 2002) The application of strategies in more “realworld” contexts School-related tasks (how to study for tests; how to remember important information; how to get meaning from text) Children’s strategies in everyday contexts (playing video games; playing games with parents or peers) How do parents or peers “teach” children strategies in realworld contexts? Integrating executive function and brain development with strategy development Infant research on self-regulation and associated brain development (A-not-B object permanence task) Executive-control (inhibition, working memory) tasks during preschool years and performance on cognitive and behavioral tasks (e.g., false-belief, resistance to temptation) Self-regulation in adolescence, related to emotional valance of task and associated brain development