COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 1 COMM 495: Communication Capstone PROFESSOR OFFICE OFFICE HRS E-MAIL Dr. Brian H. Spitzberg COMM Bldg 201 TTh 10-11:30, Th 1-2, & by Appt. spitz@mail.sdsu.edu SEMESTER CLASS TIME CLASSROOM MAILBOX IN: FALL 2015 Tue/Thu 12:30-1:45 GMCS 214 COMM BLDG. 236/237 Students are expected to be familiar with relevant contents, policies, and FURTHER INFORMATION announcements on the School website: http://communication.sdsu.edu/ TEXTS 1. The main text for this course is the Communication Inquiry & Theory Experience (CITE) document. This document is still in the process of being written and compiled. Most of the reading materials for this course will be made available for no cost to students. Whenever possible, additional readings will be selected on the basis of their availability through standard no-cost search processes. 2. i>clicker or i>REEF polling and current semester registration (http://support.reefeducation.com/ or http://support.iclicker.com/ or https://www1.iclicker.com/) PREREQUISITES Prerequisites for this course will be strictly enforced. Students must have completed Communication 300 and 350, been admitted to the major or minor in the School of Communication, and must have completed at least 21 units of upper division communication coursework. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 2 COMM 495: Communication Capstone COURSE LEARNING OBJECTIVES OVERVIEW: This course seeks to both refresh and assess knowledge competencies in regard to the field of communication, especially as represented in the SDSU School of Communication. Think of it as “The 150 things every student should know about communication,” covering the following: I. TOPIC-SPECIFIC COMMUNICATION COMPETENCIES: DISCIPLINE: Demonstrate knowledge of key persons and events in the history of the discipline of communication; COMMUNICATION: Specify and differentiate key principles regarding the definition and nature of communication, language, nonverbal communication, and several of their most prominent contexts of application; THEORIES: Specify and differentiate the nature of theories, their criteria for evaluation, and paradigms; METHODS: Specify and differentiate key assumptions and practices of the major methodological paradigms in the field of communication; RELATIONSHIPS: Specify and differentiate key principles of core societal relationships formed through communication; SOCIETY: Specify and differentiate key principles of societal uses of communication (e.g., persuasion, political communication, rhetorical movements, intercultural communication). II. CORE PROFESSIONAL COMPETENCIES: Demonstrate the following competencies. ABILITY TO WRITE: Demonstrates proficiency in grammar, syntax, semantics, academic voice, application of APA style guidelines. ABILITY TO FORMULATE CLAIMS: Demonstrates ability to articulate researchable claims specifying the interrelationship among variables. ABILITY TO ARGUE COMPETENTLY: Demonstrates ability to articulate comprehensive arguments that include relevant and appropriate claims, warrants, and evidence, and argue ethically (i.e., originality/avoiding plagiarism). ABILITY TO CONDUCT SCHOLARLY RESEARCH: Demonstrates ability to locate and appropriately cite and list recent, relevant, and reasonable scholarly research, consisting mostly of peer-reviewed journal sources. ABILITY TO SELECT RESEARCH TOPICS RELEVANT TO THE COURSE: Content demonstrates relevance to the assignment and to the communication-based focus expected of the assignment. SCOPE The scope of this class is to survey the primary learning objectives from most of the curricula representing the general communication major. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 3 COMM 495: CAPSTONE SCHEDULE* Week Date F:15 FALL 2015 Unit/Readings Assign-ments 08-25 08-27 Course Overview; Historical overview ESSENTIALS Preface/axioms 09-01 09-03 09-08 09-10 ESSENTIALS ESSENTIALS, cont. Human Communication as Discipline and Process Knowing What We Already Know 1: Eadie§ (CITE); Powers (FIND); 2: Spitzberg (CITE) 09-15 09-17 09-22 09-24 09-29 10-01 Cont. Rhetorical Ways of Knowing Conversation Analytic Ways of Knowing Performative/Ethnographic Ways of Knowing 3: Spitzberg 3(a): Renegar (HTTP) 3(b); Beach (CITE) 3(c) & (d): Scarduzio (HTTP) Experimental/Probabilistic Ways of Knowing 3(e): Levine (HTTP) 7 10-06 10-08 Theories, Models and Paradigms Cont. 4: Spitzberg (CITE) 8 10-13 10-15 Nonverbal Communication Cont. 5: Guerrero & Farinelli (HTTP) 9 10-20 10-22 Discussion of prop paper assignment Interpersonal/Relational Communication 10 10-27 10-29 Interpersonal/Relational Communication, cont. Cont. 11 Influence, Argument, Persuasion 12 11-03 11-05 11-10 10-12 Group/Organizational Communication 9: Snavely (CITE); Barge (HTTP) 13 10-17 10-19 10-24 10-26 Cultural & Intercultural Cont. 10: Winslow (CITE); Jandt (HTTP) Tu: PAPER 14 12-01 12-03 Health Communication Cont. 11: Geist-Martin et al. (CITE) Tu: IMPACCT 15 12-08 12-10 Public Address Communication & Careers 15: Winslow (CITE) 12-17 FINAL EXAM: 10:30-12:30, Thursday Dec. 17 1 2 3 4 5 6 16 17 * ** *** Tu: Quiz 6: Pauley (CITE) 7: Gass & Seiter (HTTP); Rybacki & Rybacki (HTTP) Conflict & Mediation Final Exam*** This Schedule may be revised as the semester ensues. Fair notice will be announced in class and Blackboard, and students are responsible for abiding by the most recent version of the schedule. Paper submission is due (through Blackboard Turnitin, by the beginning of class) NOTE: This is the final exam date! Inform family, friends, and the people who are getting married that travel during this time on this date is not possible because it is scheduled as of the first day of class! From the URL: http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 4 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment READINGS Preface: Orienting to Capstone Competence & Axioms of the Communication Discipline (Spitzberg) Chapter in uploaded “Capstone Text”; Chapter must be looked up On Knowing Where We’ve Been 1. A Synoptic History of Human Communication as Discipline and Process (Eadie) Read also: Eadie, W. R. (2009). Communication as a field and as a discipline. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 12-21). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf 2. Knowing What We Already Know—The Process of Research (the library, search engines, APA style, plagiarism, professional voice, etc.) (Spitzberg) On Knowing How We Know What We (Claim to) Know 3. On Coming to Know What We Don’t Know Yet—Major Methodological Paradigms: (Overview: Spitzberg) a. Rhetorical Ways of Knowing (Goehring et al.): Read instead: Renegar, V. R., & Malkowski, J. A. (2009). Rhetorical and textual approaches to communication. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 49-56). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf b. Conversation Analysis (Beach) c. Interpretive-Ethnographic (Geist-Martin). Read instead: Scarduzio, J. A., Giannini, G. A., & Geist-Martin, P. (2011). Crafting an architectural blueprint: Principles of design for ethnographic research. Symbolic Interaction, 34, 447-470. doi:10.1525/si.2011.34.4.447 http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf d. Performative Ways of Knowing (Lindemann). Read instead: http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf e. Experimental and Probabilistic Ways of Knowing (Martinez et al.). Read instead: Levine, T. R. (2009). Quantitative approaches to communication research. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 47-64)). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf 4. On Understanding What We Know—The Nature of Theories, Models and Paradigms (Spitzberg) COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 5 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment READINGS On Knowing Who Communicates What Where, and How 5. Nonverbal Communication (Andersen). Read instead: Guerrero, L. K., & Farinelli, L. (2009). The interplay of verbal and nonverbal codes. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 239-248). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf 6. Interpersonal/Relational Communication (Pauly) 7. Influence—Argument, Persuasion (XXXX, et al.). Read instead: Gass & Seiter. (2009). Persuasion and compliance gaining. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 156-164). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf Rybacki, K. C., & Rybacki, D. J. (2009). Rhetorical exigency, strategy, and argumentation. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 275-282). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf 8. Conflict Management (Spitzberg). 9. Group & Organizational Communication—Decision-making and Leadership (Snavely). Read also: Barge, K. (2009). Social groups, workgroups, and teams. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 340-348). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf 10. Cultural and Intercultural (Winslow) 11. Health Communication (Geist-Martin, Record, Pauley, Beach, Martinez, Moran). 12. Political Communication—Social Movements and Campaign Communication (Winslow). Read also: Jandt, F. (2009). Culture. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 396-404). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf 13. Public Address and Public Speaking (Winslow) On Knowing Why We Shouldn’t Know Everything 14. Ethics and Communication Makau, J. M. (2009). Ethical and unethical communication. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 435-443). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 6 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment ASSIGNMENT OVERVIEW This is a ‘total point’ grade system—a point, is a point, is a point. You are competing for overall points in the course. The points are accumulated from the following assignments: Engagement & Concept Comprehension: Each class period will involve lecture that emphasizes, elaborates upon, illustrates, exemplifies, applies, and otherwise explains concepts in the assigned readings for that week. o Embedded throughout each lecture will be 3 or more multiple-choice clicker test items over the concepts just lectured upon and/or from the reading(s) assigned for that week. o Each of these items is worth 2 points (i.e., up to 6 points per lecture day). o There are 30 class lectures, up to 25 of which are eligible for scoring (i.e., 25 x 6 = 150). Thus, up to 150 points is possible through clicker test items. o There will be other clicker items that will not count for points, and these will be designated by a different background. o As you will not know when in the lecture that key concepts may be tested, therefore, it is important that students engage the concepts as they are discussed in class, through attentiveness, questions, and discussion. o Once a clicker item appears, further discussion is ended until it has completed polling answers. o Given 30 class sessions during the semester, students can technically miss several classes, and still make the 150 points. All class clicker test items count, but a student cannot earn more than 150 points from them, and any points that show up in excess of this 150 during the semester will not count toward the student’s total points. o The instructor is not responsible for students who forget their clicker, their phone, or for their batteries dying, their inability to maintain their proper clicker registration, or for arriving late. o The instructor will not accept any other medium of response. o Further, answering for another student (by having any clicker not explicitly registered to a student responding) is a form of academic dishonesty, and the student doing the responding and the student to whom the points are attributed will both receive a failing grade for the course, and they will be reported to Student Rights and Responsibility. o See: http://support.reef-education.com/ or http://support.iclicker.com/ or https://www1.iclicker.com/ (150 points) APA/Plagiarism Quiz: A 10-to-25 item objective exam on the APA style of references format, and the indicators of plagiarism. APA Tests: http://www.lib.usm.edu/legacy/tutorials/apatutorial/tutorialindex.html https://awc.ashford.edu/apa-practice-quiz.html http://www.niu.edu/writingtutorial/style/quizzes/APA.htm (50 points). COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 7 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment ASSIGNMENT OVERVIEW GRADE SCALE Brief Proposition Paper: A 2-page (not including references page) version of the final proposition paper, with 2-3 hypotheses, and 5 relevant scholarly journal citations. Submitted online to course Blackboard Turnitin assignment site. Detailed instructions provided (100 points). Final Exam: cumulative, objective, multiple-choice, covering lecture/text. These items will bear considerable resemblance to, but will not be exactly the same, as the clicker test items throughout the semester. (100 points). Extra credit: Because the School of Communication seeks not only to distribute knowledge through teaching, but also generate it through original research, and because participation in such research provides important insights into this process of knowledge generation, students in this class will be allowed up to 10 extra credit points during the semester, out of the total number of points available, based on participation in research projects explicitly authorized and originating from the School of Communication. Extra credit cannot be guaranteed as it is dependent on the NEED of research participants in departmental research. If extra credit opportunities are made available, students can receive 2 points for each half-hour of research participation. Research opportunities are presented on the SONA Research Recruitment System which can be accessed through the School of Communication Research Participation website (< 10 points). Site: https://sites.google.com/site/commsdsuresearch (max. 10 points) Grades are based on a total point system, in which ‘a point is a point, is a point’. Letter grades are assigned based on .60, .70, .80, .90 main cuts, with .x3, and .x7 mid-grade cuts. There is no normative curving. 372-400 = A 348-359= B+ 308-319= C+ 268-279= D+ 360-371= A- LATE WORK INCOMPLETES 332-347= B 292-307= C 252-267= D 320-331= B- 280-291= C- 240-251= D- 000-239= F Any paper turned in late will be reduced 10 points per weekday that passes beyond the scheduled due date, excepted only for university or professor recognized excuses. The credibility of excuse will depend in part on diligence in apprising the instructor of the situation. In all instances in which an assignment is late, an email should be sent to the instructor as soon as possible, with a Word version of the assignment attached. I reserve the right to deduct a half-letter grade for any student who does not download the graded feedback version of their paper within a week of its posting. An “I” grade is assigned when a faculty member concludes that a student cannot meet a clearly identifiable portion of course requirements within the academic term for unforeseen reasons. An incomplete is not provided because a course or schedule is too difficult or because time was not managed sufficiently. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 8 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment WRITING STYLE GUIDE ATTENDANCE & PARTICIPATION REEVALUATIONS This course, like the entire School of Communication, requires all written assignments to use the APA (6th ed.) style guide for references, headings, and other format considerations not otherwise specified by the course assignment (see: http://flash1r.apa.org/apastyle/basics/index.htm). Students are recommended to possess an APA style guide. GRADING WILL TAKE USE OF APA STYLE INTO ACCOUNT. Attendance is counted as participation and assessment in clicker activities. Excuses are recognized for personal illness serious enough to see a physician (thus, warranting an appointment slip), family crisis, or participation in school-related activities (thus, warranting an official notification from the activity coordinator). The latter requires that prior arrangements be made. The former two are more credible with calls on the day of absence, and personal conference as soon as possible thereafter. Exams will not be given early for any reason. Record the dates of your final exam NOW, and plan travel schedules accordingly. In particular, the final exam date is set. DO NOT SCHEDULE ANYTHING AGAINST THIS DATE! Please be on time for class and stay for the entire period. If you should need to excuse yourself early from a class session, please let me know ahead of time. People walking in and out of the room are very distracting. Should you need to miss class for any reason, it is your responsibility to find out what you missed. You should get the notes from a classmate – the instructors will not “share” their notes with a student for any reason. Afterward, you can come to an instructor’s office hours to discuss the material covered in class. Assignments will be re-evaluated upon request of student, if reasonable rationale is offered. However, reevaluation opens the entire process of assessment—grades can increase or decrease as a result. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 9 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment CLASSROOM COMPORTMENT COMPORTMENT WITH PROFESSOR The School of Communication, as a representative of SDSU and higher education, expects students to engage in behaviors enhancing classroom learning environments. The Instructor is responsible for optimizing learning not only for individual students, but for all students comprising a class. Behaviors disruptive to the classroom instruction are thus not tolerated. Among the actions that are considered disruptive to the learning environment are: The use of cell phones, and/or computers/laptops/tablets, not directly related to the course and its instructional objectives, materials, or contents. Conversations with other students, during class lectures and related activities, that are distracting to shared attention and collaborative learning. Reading, sleeping, harassing, bullying, or related activities exhibiting disrespect to the instructor or fellow students. Consistently entering late, leaving early, or leaving often from class. Activities that are grossly inappropriate, threatening or dangerous, including the use of language that is racist, sexist, homophobic, or contains other forms of personal insults. When students’ actions distract from learning objectives, instructors may be required to intervene to minimize disruptive conduct. Certain other activities may be acceptable, but only with permission or by direction of the Instructor, who retains the authority to specify relevant restrictions. Such activities include: Filming, taping, or otherwise recording the class (without instructor permission); Accessing the Internet during class; The Instructor reserves the right to establish additional reasonable expectations deemed necessary to maintain optimal learning conduct in the classroom. Each faculty member is the primary arbiter of classroom comportment. The faculty member has the authority to enforce this policy in a manner deemed suitable to the particular class in question. Should repeat offenses occur, with fair warning, each faculty member will determine fair and appropriate consequences for these behaviors. Should an emergency occur or require monitoring, or if students observe violations of this policy, they are encouraged to inform the instructor as soon as possible. Communication Etiquette. Should you need to contact me at any point during the semester, the best alternative is to see me in person. I have regularly scheduled office hours and am frequently available on campus outside of these scheduled times (it is best to make an appointment to see me in this case). The next best alternative is to send an email. I check email very often during weekdays and periodically on weekends. To be respectful of the instructor’s time, it is expected that students will be fully prepared when meeting the professor in the office, with an agenda of specific issues to address, and relevant materials ready for the instructor to examine. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 10 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment SENSITIVE SUBJECTS & POTENTIAL STRESSORS STUDENTS WITH DISABILITIES: COURSE DESIGN: Throughout the course of this semester, we will be touching on a number of very sensitive issues including violence, sexuality, family, and loss. Our goal this semester is to create an open dialogue where we can discuss how the theories and concepts analyzed in research become real through lived experience; therefore, I ask that you keep the information shared in class privileged. Please be respectful of your classmates by treating their stories with care and sensitivity. Issues of personal violence and loss can be particularly difficult to manage. If you find yourself in need of extra care, counseling, or support this semester, please feel free to take advantage of one or more of the services listed below: At SDSU: Students who require immediate psychological help are seen on an emergency basis by calling C&PS at (619) 594-5220 during business hours. After hours, students can call the San Diego Access and Crisis 24hour Hotline at (800) 479-3339. Other emergency services include: Student Health Services Nurse Advisory Line at (888) 594-5281; or University Police at (619) 594-1991. Other Therapist referral lines: Counseling Referrals: 619.232.9622 Psychiatrist Referrals: sandiegopsychiatricsociety.org Psychologist Referrals: 619.291.3451 Psychotherapists Referral: 619.296.9011 If you are a student with a disability and believe you will need accommodations for this class, it is your responsibility to contact Student Disability Services at (619) 594-6473. To avoid any delay in the receipt of your accommodations, you should contact Student Disability Services as soon as possible. Please note that accommodations are not retroactive, and that accommodations based upon disability cannot be provided until you have presented your instructor with an accommodation letter from Student Disability Services. Your cooperation is appreciated. This is a traditional realspace didactic classroom course. The primary mode of instruction is lecture, combined with questions and answers in class. Learning outcomes are assessed predominantly by objective examinations and by a written research paper assignment. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 11 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment CLICKERS: i>clicker required for this course! This semester in this class, you have the option of using either the i>clicker2 handheld remote or REEF mobile. REEF enables you to vote via a webenabled device such as a laptop or app on your iOS smartphone. (Android currently not available). i>clicker2 is a handheld remote which you must purchase and bring to class every day. The SDSU Bookstore offers discounts for both REEF and i>clicker2. If you already have an i>clicker2 remote, you can use it in this class. Active i>clickerGO accounts have transitioned to REEF. i>clicker will be used every day in class, starting with the first day of class, and you are responsible for registering and bringing your device every day. Note: Not all faculty are allowing mobile use. Don’t buy REEF until you are sure you can use it. Get the two week FREE trial to make sure this works for you before buying an activation code. To receive participation credit in the course: i>clicker2 remote: register your remote on any of your SDSU courses by going to Tools =>i>clicker Registration => Enter your remoteID which is on the back of the remote => Submit. MAKE SURE YOU SEE A REGISTRATION DATE or your clicker might not be registered. Once registered, your i>clicker2 remote can be used in all classes that require i>clicker and should not need to be re-registered. REEF: Use the FREE two-week trial before purchasing to make sure REEF works in your classroom. You can purchase activation codes from the SDSU Bookstore. Go to https://www1.iclicker.com/ or download the iOS app from your app store. Create an account. Your REDID is your StudentID. Register your activation code on the Blackboard site the once you purchase it (Tools =>i>clicker Registration => Enter your activation ID.) If you have more questions about i>clicker or REEF, check http://support.reefeducation.com/ or http://support.iclicker.com/ first. If you do not find an answer there, contact the Student Computing Center at 619.594.3189 or scc@rohan.sdsu.edu IMPORTANT RESPONSIBILITIES & POLICIES: o DO NOT CHANGE FROM ONE SYSTEM TO ANOTHER DURING THE SEMESTER. o MONITOR YOUR PARTICIPATION POINTS REGULARLY. No clicker points will be negotiable after 3 weeks of their class date. o DON’T CHEAT. If you are caught clicking in for an absent student, that is a form of academic dishonesty; you will be failed, and reported to the Office of Student Rights & Responsibility. If you have more questions about i>clicker, visit http://clicker.sdsu.edu or http://support.iclicker.com. Come prepared to use your clicker on the first day of class.\ COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 12 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment CLICKERS: i> REEF Mobile Student i>clicker Registration (iOS only) If you already have a paid, long-term i>clickerGO account, it was moved to REEF automatically. If your professor has approved the use of the mobile “i>clicker” app, we highly recommend using the free REEF trial before paying for the activation. Just in case something goes wrong, you’ll know before you pay The clicker app, called REEF, is currently only available for iOS devices. However, you can use the REEF website on any web connected tablet, laptop, or other device to access your clicker polling session. The REEF website URL is https://app.reefeducation.com/#/login Steps for getting an account set up in REEF: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Go to https://www1.iclicker.com Use the links to go to the iOS store or set up a web account Use your REDID as your Student ID! After you purchase your activation code, go to any Blackboard course and register your clickerID using “Tools => Register your i>clicker => Submit” You should see a registration date if properly registered. Steps for using REEF in class: 1. 2. 3. 4. Use the app or login online (https://app.reef-education.com/#/login) Create a new account or login Select San Diego State University, your professor, and your course When the instructor starts a polling question, the corresponding input controls automatically appear on the students' screens. See http://bit.ly/reef-help for more info on student use of REEF COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 13 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment EXTRA CREDIT Rationale: Because the School of Communication seeks not only to distribute knowledge through teaching, but also generate it through original research, and because participation in such research provides important insights into this process of knowledge generation, students in this class will be allowed up to a total (not to exceed) 10 extra credit points during the semester, out of the total number of points available for the course, based on participation in School of Communication authorized research projects. Points will be provided upon evidence of completed participation, with 2 points for each half-hour of research participation. So, 0.5 credits = 2 points, 1 credit = 4 points, 1.5 credits = 6 points, 2 credits = 8 points, and 2.5 credits (i.e., 5 hours of participation) = 10 extra credit points, added to the total number of points for the semester. 1. Extra Credit: Students may obtain extra credit from participation in approved departmental research IF research opportunities are made available. Extra credit cannot be guaranteed as it is dependent on the NEED of research participants in departmental research. If extra credit opportunities are made available, students can receive 2 points for each half-hour of research participation (max. 10 points). Research opportunities are presented on the SONA Research Recruitment System which can be accessed through the School of Communication Research Participation website: https://sites.google.com/site/commsdsuresearch 2. Eligibility: Only research projects approved and listed on the site listed above are eligible. 3. Announcement of Opportunities: It is the students' responsibility to avail themselves of such opportunities--ongoing announcements and solicitations on the part of the instructor may or may not be made during the semester as opportunities arise. 4. Availability of Opportunities: Research in a program ebbs and flows. Participation is only available during the active windows of time specified by each study. Opportunities for participation may or may not be available in any particular semester, or at any particular time of the semester. 5. Record of Participation: It is the responsibility of each individual researcher to generate a valid list of student participation to return to the instructor of record. It should be apparent in each research project how the student's participation is to be recorded and evidenced. 6. Grade: No more credit is available than is indicated above—there are no "additional" projects or sources for achieving extra credit in the course. 7. Ethics: Any attempt to falsify participation in research for the sake of receiving unearned credit, or to surreptitiously claim credit for more than one course, are forms of academic dishonesty, and will be a basis for failure of a course and initiation of proceedings with the office of Student Rights & Responsibilities. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 14 COMM 495: Exams EXAM GRADING EXAM RETENTION EXAMS TIPS FOR STUDYING FOR EXAMS Items that are overly difficult, not discriminating or unreliable are adjusted based on statistical analyses (overall item discrimination, item reliability, item difficulty, response options with greater reliability than the keyed option). There will be no curving of grades after these adjustments are made. Grades are uploaded to Blackboard only after these adjustments are made. Please note that exams in this class will not be returned. However, you are welcome to make an appointment to see and review your exam. All exams and answer sheets will be destroyed at the end of the following semester. Exams are objective format (multiple-choice, true/false), covering lecture, and any materials distributed to the class. The final exam may be cumulative and comparative across topics of the semester. Detailed review sheets will be made available prior to each exam. Optical scanning answer sheets (ParSCORE “small red”) and No. 2 pencils are the student’s responsibility on exam days. No electronic devices (i.e., cellphones, earphones, MP3 devices, cameras, tape recorders, calculators, e-books, e-tablets, laptop computers, or electronic dictionaries) are permitted during exams. Any evidence of any such device in sight of a student will result in that student’s failure on that exam, and if there is evidence of any attempt at recording or copying exam materials, or making use of other stolen materials, the student may be failed for the course and reported to Student Rights & Responsibilities. First, study comparatively. A multiple-choice good item has to present a condition (i.e., root) that one and only one answer (i.e., stem) correctly fulfills. However, the remaining stems (i.e., foils) must seem correct to the unstudied mind. For foils to seem correct, they often use words and concepts that are legitimate content for the course, but that do not uniquely fit the condition identified in the item root. A student who merely skims or memorizes materials will see several stems that “look” correct, when only one stem actually legitimately fulfills the item root. In order to know which stem is correct requires not only that the stems are recognized, but also what makes the concepts distinct and different from one another, and how they relate to the condition specified in the root. This means studying comparatively, which in turn suggests several study techniques. Memorization is important, but only a small part of the picture. Memorization helps with definitions, lists, model components, and stages or sequences. However, it does not help much with comparison, contrast, analysis and synthesis. When studying a concept, ask how it relates to other similar but different concepts. It is important to analyze the differences between concepts, rather than just understanding the concept by itself. Successive integrative outlining may help. This means to outline the class notes into a more concise set of notes, and then outline text chapters into a concise set of notes, and then combining student outlines. This activity leads to compare and contrast “where things belong in relation to each other.” The resulting integrative outline can then serve as a final study document. This practice is time-consuming, but can also be very beneficial. Also, study your own past exams before taking the next one. The professor permits students to look over their own exams. Finally, use study groups to assess preparation, but not to study. This is clearly only an opinion, but study groups can be very inefficient, and are not likely to do much good unless the members have studied hard prior to studying as a group. The value of the group is to provide different ways of seeing the material, to test knowledge with questions any given student would not ordinarily derive, and to occasionally correct errors in thinking. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 15 THE ACADEMIC DISHONESTY POLICY OF THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION Overview SDSU Definitions In any case in which an instructor identifies evidence for charging a student with violation of academic conduct standards or plagiarism, the presumption will be with that instructor’s determination. The instructor(s) will confer with the School Director to confirm the evidence. Once confirmed, the student will be informed and presented with the evidence. Some conditions and terms below clarify the School policy and procedure. “Dishonesty”: Catalog: (1) “Dishonesty, including: A. Cheating, plagiarism, or other forms of academic dishonesty that are intended to gain unfair academic advantage. B. Furnishing false information to a university official, faculty member, or campus office. C. Forgery, alteration, or misuse of a university document, key, or identification instrument. D. Misrepresenting one’s self to be an authorized agent of the university or one of its auxiliaries.” “Cheating: Cheating is defined as the act of obtaining or attempting to obtain credit for academic work by the use of dishonest, deceptive, or fraudulent means. Examples of cheating include, but are not limited to copying, in part or in whole, from another's test or other examination; discussing answers or ideas relating to the answers on a test or other examination without the permission of the instructor; obtaining copies of a test, an examination, or other course material without the permission of the instructor; using notes, cheat sheets, or other devices considered inappropriate under the prescribed testing condition; collaborating with another or others in work to be presented without the permission of the instructor; falsifying records, laboratory work, or other course data; submitting work previously presented in another course, if contrary to the rules of the course; altering or interfering with the grading procedures; plagiarizing, as defined; and knowingly and intentionally assisting another student in any of the above. Plagiarism: Plagiarism is defined as the act of incorporating ideas, words, or specific substance of another, whether purchased, borrowed, or otherwise obtained, and submitting same to the university as one's own work to fulfill academic requirements without giving credit to the appropriate source. Plagiarism shall include but not be limited to: submitting work, either in part or in whole, completed by another; omitting footnotes for ideas, statements, facts, or conclusions that belong to another; omitting quotation marks when quoting directly from another, whether it be a paragraph, sentence, or part thereof; close and lengthy paraphrasing of the writings of another; submitting another person's artistic works, such as musical compositions, photographs, paintings, drawings, or sculptures; and submitting as one's own work papers purchased from research companies.” (source: http://www.sa.sdsu.edu/srr/cheating-plagiarism.html) COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 16 THE ACADEMIC DISHONESTY POLICY OF THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION Lying Evidence or determination of deliberate deception of the instructor for the purposes of self-gain or gain of fellow student(s) will be considered prejudicial in evaluating a student’s assignments, participation, and/or standing in the course. Intellectual contents include all forms of ‘text’ produced by another person or persons. It includes: writings, course syllabi, course lectures and recordings of lectures, visual information such as models, videos, lyrics, software, etc. The syllabus, lectures and lecture outlines are personally-copyrighted intellectual Intellectual property of the instructor, which means that any organized recording for anything Property other than personal use, duplication, distribution, or profit is a violation of copyright and fair use laws. Proper source Proper attribution occurs by specifying the source of content or ideas. This is done by (a) providing quotation marks around text, when directly quoted, and (b) clearly attribution designating the source of the text or information relied upon in an assignment. Students often practice some form of ‘double-dipping,’ in which they write on a Selfgiven topic across more than one course assignment. In general, there is nothing plagiarism wrong with double-dipping topics or sources, but there is a problem with doubledipping exact and redundant text. It is common for scholars to write on the same topic across many publication outlets; this is part of developing expertise and the reputation of being a scholar on a topic. Scholars, however, are not permitted to repeat exact text across papers or publications except when noted and attributed, as this wastes precious intellectual space with repetition and does a disservice to the particular source of original presentation by ‘diluting’ the value of the original presentation. Any time a writer simply ‘cuts-and-pastes’ exact text from former papers into a new paper, it is a form of self-plagiarism. Consequently, a given paper should never be turned in to multiple classes. Entire paragraphs, or even sentences, should not be repeated word-for-word across course assignments. Each new writing assignment is precisely that, a new writing assignment, requiring new composition on the student’s part. Solicitation for Any student who solicits any third party to write any portion of an assignment for this class (whether for pay or not) violates the standards of academic honesty. The ghost writing penalty for solicitation (regardless of whether it can be demonstrated the individual solicited wrote any sections of the assignment) is F in the course and reporting to Student Rights and Responsibilities. A good place to learn about plagiarism is the tutorial on academic integrity at Useful Aides http://plagiarism.org/plagiarism-101/overview/; and at http://www.yorku.ca/tutorial/academic_integrity/caseintro.html A good place to learn about APA writing and citation style is: http://owl.english.purdue.edu/owl/resource/560/01/ A good place to learn about making better arguments is: http://www.readwritethink.org/files/resources/interactives/persuasion_map/ Intellectual contents COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 17 THE ACADEMIC DISHONESTY POLICY OF THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION Secondary citations TurnItIn.com Secondary citation is not strictly a form of plagiarism, but in blatant forms, it can present similar ethical challenges. A secondary citation is citing source A, which in turn cites source B, but it is source B’s ideas or content that provide the unique basis for the claims the student intends to make in the assignment. For example, assume there is an article by Jones (2006) in the student’s hands, in which there is a discussion or quotation of an article by Smith (1998). Assume further that what Smith seems to be saying is very important to the student’s analysis. In such a situation, the student should always try to locate the original Smith source. In general, if an idea is important enough to discuss in an assignment, it is important enough to locate and cite the original source for that idea. There are several reasons for these policies: (a) Authors sometimes commit citation errors, which might be replicated without knowing it; (b) Authors sometimes make interpretation errors, which might be ignorantly reinforced (c) Therefore, reliability of scholarly activity is made more difficult to assure and enforce; (d) By relying on only a few sources of review, the learning process is short-circuited, and the student’s own research competencies are diminished, which are integral to any liberal education; (e) By masking the actual sources of ideas, readers must second guess which sources come from which citations, making the readers’ own research more difficult; (f) By masking the origin of the information, the actual source of ideas is misrepresented. Some suggestions that assist with this principle: When the ideas Jones discusses are clearly attributed to, or unique to, Smith, then find the Smith source and citation. When the ideas Jones is discussing are historically associated more with Smith than with Jones, then find the Smith source and citation. In contrast, Jones is sometimes merely using Smith to back up what Jones is saying and believes, and is independently qualified to claim, whether or not Smith would have also said it; in such a case, citing Jones is sufficient. Never simply copy a series of citations at the end of a statement by Jones, and reproduce the reference list without actually going to look up what those references report—the only guarantee that claims are valid is for a student to read the original sources of those claims. The papers in this course will be submitted electronically in Word (preferably .docx) on the due dates assigned, and will require verification of submission to Turnitin.com. “Students agree that by taking this course all required papers may be subject to submission for textual similarity review to TurnItIn.com for the detection of plagiarism. All submitted papers will be included as source documents in the TurnItIn.com reference database solely for the purpose of detecting plagiarism of such papers. You may submit your papers in such a way that no identifying information about you is included. Another option is that you may request, in writing, that your papers not be submitted to TurnItIn.com. However, if you choose this option you will be required to provide documentation to substantiate that the papers are your original work and do not include any plagiarized material” (source: language suggested by the CSU General Counsel and approved by the Center for Student’s Rights and Responsibilities at SDSU). COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 18 THE ACADEMIC DISHONESTY POLICY OF THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION Consequences of Plagiarism or Cheating Course failure: Soliciting or reproducing a whole paper, paragraph, or large portions of unattributed materials without proper attribution, whether represented by: (a) multiple sentences, images, or portions of images; or (b) by percentage of assignment length, will result in assignment of an “F” in the course in which the infraction occurred, and a report to the Center for Student Rights and Responsibilities (CSRR 2). Assignment failure: Reproducing a sentence or sentence fragment with no quotation marks, but with source citation, or subsets of images without source attribution, will minimally result in an “F” on the assignment, and may result in greater penalty, including a report to the CSRR, depending factors noted below. In this instance, an “F” may mean anything between a zero (0) and 50%, depending on the extent of infraction. Exacerbating conditions—Amount: Evidence of infraction, even if fragmentary, is increased with a greater: (a) number of infractions; (b) distribution of infractions across an assignment; or (c) proportion of the assignment consisting of infractions. Exacerbating conditions—Intent: Evidence of foreknowledge and intent to deceive magnifies the seriousness of the offense and the grounds for official response. Plagiarism, whether ‘by accident’ or ‘by ignorance,’ still qualifies as plagiarism—it is all students’ responsibility to make sure their assignments are not committing the offense. Assistance: Evidence that the student was not the original author of the work, due to soliciting the assistance or composition of another person or persons. Exceptions: Any exceptions to these policies will be considered on a case-by-case basis, and only under exceptional circumstances. THE ACADEMIC DISHONESTY POLICY OF THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION THIS IS SERIOUS! THERE ARE NO EXCUSES ALLOWED BASED ON IGNORANCE OF WHAT CONSTITUTES PLAGIARISM, OR OF WHAT THIS POLICY IS. PLAGIARISM IS A CRIME OF CONDUCT, NOT OF INTENT. THIS COURSE WILL HAVE ZERO-TOLERANCE FOR PLAGIARISM! WHY? BECAUSE: 1. A PLAGIARISM POLICY IS PUBLISHED IN THE UNIVERSITY CATALOG; 2. THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION PLAGIARISM POLICY, COMPLETE WITH ELABORATED EXAMPLES, DEFINITIONS, AND CONSEQUENCES FOR TYPES OF PLAGIARISM, IS: a. ON THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION BLACKBOARD SITE, b. ON THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION WEBSITE, AND c. IN THE COURSE SYLLABUS; 3. A POWERPOINT LECTURE PRESENTED IN THIS COURSE ON “THE ESSENTIALS” FURTHER SPECIFIES THE NATURE OF PLAGIARISM; 4. YOU CAN TURN IN YOUR PAPER BEFORE IT IS DUE, SEE ITS ORIGINALITY RATING, FIX IT, AND TURN IT IN AGAIN BY SUBMISSION DEADLINE; 5. FINALLY, YOU SHOULD SIMPLY KNOW THAT IT IS UNETHICAL AND A ‘HIGH CRIME’ IN ACADEME TO MISREPRESENT ANYONE’S WORDS OR IDEAS, THROUGH IMPLICATION, WHETHER INTENDED OR NOT, a. THAT THEY ARE YOUR OWN, OR b. THEY ARE SOMEONE ELSE’S WHEN THEY ARE NOT. THEREFORE, THERE IS NO EXCUSE FOR PLAGIARISM, AND NO EXCUSE WILL BE ACCEPTED. YOU ARE FOREWARNED. IF YOU PLAGIARIZE, YOU WILL FAIL THIS COURSE. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 19 Source: http://plagiarism.org/plagiarism-101/overview COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 20 INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE “PROP” PAPER A Propositional Parable D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: Daddy, what is an instinct? An instinct, my dear, is an explanatory principle. But what does it explain? Anything – almost anything at all. Anything you want it to explain. Don't be silly. It doesn't explain gravity. No. But that is because nobody wants "instinct" to explain gravity. If they did, it would explain it. We could simply say that the moon has an instinct whose strength varies inversely as the square of the distance . . . But that's nonsense, Daddy. Yes, surely. But it was you who mentioned "instinct," not I. All right – but then what does explain gravity? Nothing my dear, because gravity is an explanatory principle. Oh. Do you mean that you cannot use one explanatory principle to explain another? Hmm . . . hardly ever. That is what Newton meant when he said, "hypotheses non fingo." And what does that mean? Please. Well, you know what "hypotheses" are. Any statement linking together two descriptive statements is an hypothesis. If you say that there was a full moon on February 1st and another on March 1st; and then you link these two observations together in any way, the statement that links them is an hypothesis. Yes – and I know what 'non' means. But what's 'fingo.' Well, 'fingo' is a late Latin word for 'make.' It forms a verbal noun 'fictio' from which we get the word 'fiction.' Daddy, do you mean that Sir Isaac Newton thought that all hypotheses were just made up like stories? Yes – precisely that. But didn't he discover gravity? With the apple? No, dear. He invented it. D: Oh. Adapted from: Heinz Von Foerster (1981). Rigor and imagination: Essays from the legacy of Gregory Bateson. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 21 DON'T PANIC “Hypotheses are the scaffolds which are erected in front of a building and removed when the building is completed. They are indispensable to the worker; but he must not mistake the scaffolding for the building. J. W. von Goethe, Maxims and reflections, 1893 (in Kaplan: Science says) “For every fact there is an infinity of hypotheses. The more you look, the more you see. R. M. Pirsig, Zen and the art of motorcycle maintenance, 1974. “Hypothesis, where successful, is a two-way street, extending back to explain the past and forward to predict the future. What we try to do in framing hypotheses is to explain some otherwise unexplained happenings by inventing a plausible story, a plausible description or history of relevant portions of the world” (Quine & Ullian, 1980, “Hypothesis,” p. 197) “The purpose of models is not to fit the data but to sharpen the questions” S. Karlin, 11th R. A. Fisher Memorial Lecture, Royal Society, April 20, 1983 (in Kaplan: Science says) “Being able to examine our models, critically evaluate them, and even discard them is far more scientifically literate than being able to regurgitate facts for a standardized test. … Ultimately, our models and descriptions of reality must be subject to two overriding criteria. How useful is this model, and how much does this model resemble our observations? Scientific literacy requires an understanding that science is only a model. We have to be able to jettison our models when our critical thinking leads us to that conclusion. Saus, S. (2007). Camelot is only a model: Scientific literacy in the 21st century. Seed, (Sept/Oct), 77-78. “Without a sense, or without the thought, a proposition would be an utterly dead and trivial thing.” Wittgenstein, Blue and Brown Books, 1958, p. 4 “The meaning of exactness best founded in intellectual history is the possibility of constructing a theoretical system of idealized models containing abstract constructs of variables and of relations between variables, from which most or all propositions concerning particular connections can be deduced.” Machlup, 2004, Readings in the philosophy of social science p. 11 “All possible knowledge is only an approximation and does not reach ultimate reality. It only reflects certain aspects of reality in more or less appropriate models.” von Bertalanffy, 1975 Perspectives on general system theory, p. 114 COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 22 Overview The purpose of a proposition (i.e., hypothesis) paper is to develop a number of theoretical propositions, a significant proportion of which concern communication in relationship to the course topic (e.g., conflict, relationships, dark side of communication, communication theory, etc.). The paper in some ways is a review of literature; it is different from typical literature reviews in a few important ways. Most reviews just summarize research to provide a sense of ‘what is known’ in an area. In contrast, the proposition paper attempts to develop explanatory arguments in support of a number of hypotheses. Papers are primarily graded on four areas: (1) Writing: Good writing, composition, style, voice, and format (APA); (2) Proposition wording: Conceptual soundness of the propositions and their wording; (3) Scholarly research: Quality of the scholarly journal research brought to bear; and (4) Explanatory argument: The quality of the explanatory arguments developed for each proposition. For graduate student papers only, a fifth grading component is (5) Model value: The quality of the visual model in summarizing or generating the hypotheses. It is very important to emphasize that “evidence” and “argument” are separate elements of the assessment—evidence backs an argument, but the argument itself has to explain the proposition, independent of the evidence. Of course, papers must also be topical; that is, they must be relevant to the course content. Hypothesizing A hypothesis is a verbal or symbolic statement of relationship between two or more variables (e.g., Trust is positively related to the likelihood of using cooperative strategies during conflicts). A variable is any construct or concept that can be observed to take on different values (e.g., trust, anxiety, self-disclosure, assertiveness, strategy use, etc.). It takes the form of X = fY (i.e., “The value of ‘X’ is a function of ‘Y’). A definition (e.g., self-esteem is the degree to which self is perceived positively), in contrast, characterizes the nature of a variable, but not its relationship to other variables. It takes the form X = Y. All of this is just a fancy way of saying that the proposition paper attempts to develop, through review of scholarly research and personally formulated argument, a series of hypotheses on a given topic. Many of these hypotheses may be already in the existing research literature, they may be derived from conceptual models, or the products of creativity and imagination. There are two keys to this assignment: (1) Developing well-conceived and well-worded hypotheses, and (2) developing reasonable arguments for each hypothesis. Like any argument, it is made credible through the use of causal analysis, strong reasons, evidence, citations, and example. The most important thing is that every hypothesis should be carefully worded (read all these instructions to understand this warning) and arguments should answer two “why” questions: (1) Why are the variables related this way? (2) Why is the hypothesis likely to be true? The word “because” should figure prominently in every explanation. To explain a hypothesis means to make sensible how and why things are related the way they are. Hypotheses themselves explain nothing—they simply describe a relationship between two or more things—not why the relationship exists. Theories are basically sets of interrelated hypotheses that collectively provide an explanation for a phenomenon or process. For example, a COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 23 person may want to explain the hypothesis: “As jealousy increases, the likelihood of relational violence increases.” Guided by an understanding of argument (e.g., see recommendations for writing that follow later in these instructions), this hypothesis could be argued thusly: Jealousy is a complex blend of emotions based on the perception that a person’s valued relationship is threatened by a third party or rival. [Backing]: Retzinger (1991) has reviewed evidence [Grounds]: that violence is likely to result from a combination of anger and shame, but neither alone [Backing]. Because [Warrant]: anger creates an inner sense of expressive frustration and arousal, and shame provides a target for this expression (i.e., the partner and/or the rival), the jealous person is much more likely to engage in violence than non-jealous persons. [Claim]: This hypothesis may not apply to contexts in which strong moral, religious, or public restraints are in place (e.g., even jealous persons tend not to be overtly violent in public places). [Rebuttal]: The rebuttal is not necessary, but sometimes illustrates relative objectivity and openness of the author. In this explanation, other concepts (i.e., the theory of rage as proposed by Retzinger, with its components of anger and rage) are used to make the link between jealousy and violence sensible. More explanation could be provided for these concepts, more evidence reviewed in favor of Retzinger’s hypothesis, and so forth, but the basic elements of explanation and argument are there. The point is that explanation is neither mere restatement nor mere review of research or expert opinion. Please note, there is no need for quotations. CITE OFTEN, QUOTE SPARINGLY, AND PREFERABLY, NOT AT ALL. Form, Organization and Style One or more model paper(s) will be uploaded on Blackboard, and student papers should follow the model(s) as ideal analogues to their own papers. In general, the following procedures adhere: (1) Margins: Use normal 1-inch margins; (2) Font: Use 11-point Arial or Calibri or 12-point Times Roman font; (3) Spacing: With double spacing throughout; (4) Page length: With no more than 2 pages text (not including reference page); (5) Title information: With the top 3 lines of the first page of the paper with five pieces of information (name, course, email address, date, and paper title); (6) Running head: With headers programmed into the paper, indicating a brief title and pagination; (7) Propositions: With 2 to 3 propositions—if 3 are developed, they must be in the form of a valid syllogism; otherwise, just develop 2 propositions; (8) References: With a minimum of 5 relevant scholarly, peer-reviewed journal publication references, all of which must be cited in text, and properly listed on the third page “References” list, all in proper APA style. There may be additional references to scholarly books or chapters in scholarly books, or other sources deemed vital to the paper. (9) Appendix: A screen-print of the primary search engine summary (see example) (10) Submission: The assignment will be submitted to Turnitin—no hardcopy is needed. It is recommended you submit your paper at least 3 days early to receive a Turnitin originality report so it is possible to correct and re-submit if there are any problems. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 24 Use A.P.A. throughout (6th ed.), except for format specifications above. Do not leave big blank spaces between sections or hypotheses. The text should run continuously. Save your work often during writing—there are no excuses for late work. Use the insert running head procedure in Word—do not just type the header at the top of the page for each page. Learn to use internally formatted hanging indents for the references and hypotheses (as opposed to hitting ‘enter’ at the end of a line and tabbing in from the next line), and page and section breaks for new pages (as opposed to just hitting ‘enter’ a bunch of times). There is no excuse for ignorance about Word— learn to use its formatting resources and tools! There is no “Title Page;” instead, at the top of the first page insert the following: Last name, First name Email address Course #: Brief course title Date: Title of Paper Next, some form of introduction is presented. An introduction may briefly examine the history and importance of the concepts, discuss the relevance of these concepts to the course subject, introduce relevant theories, and/or explain any basic terms, assumptions, or limitations that may be important. Assume this class as the audience, which means that the writing should be clear enough for them to comprehend fully the content, and intent, of the paper. Next, the body of the paper consists of the propositions and the arguments for each proposition. This section of the paper proceeds by presenting a proposition and then developing a paragraph (or two) of explanatory argument (warrants; explanations) and support (evidence). The propositions should be numbered and set off from the text (e.g., bolded and/or italicized). Propositions are never prima facie; that is, they never stand on their own. Propositions always require some degree of explanation. Next, the conclusion section develops any of several points, such as a brief summary, a consideration of the limitations of the analysis, a critical conjecture about the status of the theory and/or research associated with the concepts examined, future implications of the analysis, or the importance of the analysis. Finally, the References section will provide the complete citations in A.P.A. style. Please save the paper with the following nomenclature: “C495-PROP-semester-yearLastname-BriefTopicTitle. For example, if it is the Fall 2014 semester, and the paper is on sexual coercion, and your last name is Johnson, then the file would be saved as: “C495-PROP-SP13Johnson-Sexual Coercion” Submission The final version of the paper will be turned in electronically, in Word 2007, through Turnitin in Blackboard. If difficulties are experienced turning it in online, it is essential to send an email to the professor with the paper attached, by the time the assignment is due. No hard-copy is required. Papers may be turned in 2-3 days early to check originality and resubmit if needed. Types of Hypotheses COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 25 Below is a series of hypotheses to illustrate how they can differ. These hypotheses may seem sophisticated right now. However, research of a topic has begun, hypothetical relations will begin to emerge either stated directly (i.e., in the form of hypotheses being tested) or indirectly (i.e., implied by the explanation and discussion of the concepts). The simplest, and weakest, form of hypothesis is a "non-directional" statement of relationship. For example: H1: Self-esteem is related to communication competence. While this is a hypothesis, it provides minimal information regarding the precise nature of the relationship. Avoid such statements in the paper. A more precise form is: H2: Self-esteem is positively related to interpersonal communication competence. (Note: This can be reworded as: “Persons high in self-esteem are significantly higher in communication competence than persons low in self-esteem”). Hypotheses can also vary by the form (or shape) of the relationship: H3: Motivation, knowledge and skills are curvilinear to perceived communication competence. (That is, they are positively related up to a moderately high range of competence, after which higher values lead to lower perceived competence). Finally, deductive construction allows the development of stronger, and more tightly controlled, theoretical arguments. For example, H4: As [TV exposure] increases, [verbal skills] decrease. [A] [B] H5: As [verbal skills] decrease, use of [physical violence] in conflict increases. [B] [C] H6: As [TV exposure] increases, use of [physical violence] in conflict increases. [A] [C] On the Art of Explanation “We cannot infer the events of the future from those of the present. Belief in the causal nexus is superstition” (Wittgenstein, Tractatus, 5.1361) Explanation implies the identification of some “bridge” (i.e., warrant, “because...,” etc.) that serves as an account of why one concept is related to another concept. It is an “animating mechanism,” that gives life to a concept. For example, one popular concept is that exposure to violence in the media causes violence in society. The natural question is why. Answering the COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 26 “why” question gets at the concept of explanation. Even though seeing or hearing the equation: “media violence causes societal violence” seems like an explanation, it is not. It is missing the bridge, or animating mechanism. To illustrate, consider a few “non-explanations.” It is not an explanation to say media violence causes societal violence because: (1) media depictions of violence are increasing; (2) societal violence is increasing; (3) some people have engaged in crimes they saw on TV (“copy cat crimes”); (4) people believe what they see on TV; (5) research shows media violence increases societal violence. Each of these merely suggests correlation (rather than causation), repeats the original assertion, or simply provides backing for an explanation that is itself missing. To illustrate legitimate explanations, consider the following: Cultivation: Social learning theory claims that people learn both by direct experience (e.g., touching a hot stove) or observation of others (e.g., observing one’s sister touch a hot stove). Since we cannot or do not always directly experience things (e.g., robbing a bank), we look to real and imaginative role models (e.g., media figures and narratives). Thus, the crime and violence in the media provide models, and rewarding ones at that, for us we otherwise would not have had. Cognitive Availability: Our brain can be viewed as a library. If 90% of this library is filled with science fiction, and we are asked to respond to someone’s question for information, we are most likely to give them a fanciful piece of information, because that is what is most represented in our mind’s library. If, in watching media, most of the acts or ways of dealing with conflict we observe are violent in nature, it simply supplies more acts of violence to store in our mental repertoires, and thus, when we find ourselves in a conflict, violent acts are the most “available” to draw upon. Disinhibition: Growing up in a culture is largely about learning what one cannot or is not supposed to do. The very nature of culture is conformity and normative action within a given body of beliefs and behaviors. Exposure to violence in the media may make violence appear more normative than it is, and thus, make engaging in violence seem less deviant. In essence, it is not making violence seem more rewarding (cultivation) or available, but merely makes the violence within a person more acceptable. Fantasy Fulfillment: Freud and others might argue that daily life, as well as the normal tortures of growing up, fill us with tensions that are constantly repressed. These tensions struggle to “get out,” managing to manifest themselves mainly in the form of fantasy. Media violence may give form to these fantasies, making them more vivid and “real,” thereby stimulating the enactment of fantasies in life (e.g., who has not “fantasized” about hitting someone in the face—seeing it done may both “suggest” it, but stimulate the enactment of it). Desensitization: One of the reasons people avoid violence is that it hurts others. However, when violence is everywhere in the media, it can numb one’s sensibilities, and lower one’s ability to empathize. Further, the more cartoonish the violence, the more it seems it does not really hurt anyone. Such exposure to violence may take the “brakes” off using by making violence seem less hurtful, and thus, less costly to use. Excitation: Violence is a thrill. Through 5 million years of evolution, we have come to find violence a potential threat, a potential path to victory, and therefore, arousing. Thus, seeing violence in the media is itself, arousing, and this arousal stimulates our own arousal. Our COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 27 arousal (adrenaline, muscular tension, etc.) needs to find an outlet, and is expressed in violence. Peer Group Mediation: Research on sexual violence indicates that people who engage in sexual violence are much more likely to have friends who approve of, and have engaged in, sexual violence. The suggestion is that peer groups mediate the transfer of violence. That is, if a peer group watches violence in the media, this peer group may then become self-reinforcing through its interaction, thereby serving as the proximal stimulus to its members’ violence. Spillover: Violence in the media provides models of behavior in certain contexts that are then available for generalization to contexts experienced in everyday life, both directly comparable to those observed in the media, as well as contexts largely unrelated. Thus, for example, a person may “copycat” behaviors witnessed in the media, but may also extend the function of the behaviors observed in one context (e.g., a drop kick in a professional wrestling program) to another context (e.g., a dispute with one’s brother). Cultural Chaos: Violence in the media may reflect the very breakdown of society, norms, and culture. If everyone is violent to everyone in the media, then this communicates a sense of despair, hopelessness, alienation and angst to the viewing public. This has the effects of both (1) inciting some to exploit the lawlessness (e.g., looting during natural disasters), (2) join the crowd (e.g., mob violence during the L.A. riots), (3) bystander apathy providing a permissive environment for violence. Each of these explanations offers slightly and sometimes substantively distinct sources of causation. For example, fantasy fulfillment, excitation and disinhibition tend to assume that we are by nature violent, and all that is needed is something in the media to take the restraints of society away. Cultivation and availability argue media instills violence in us. Peer-group mediation and cultural chaos tend to view media as having an effect on society at large, which only then affects our individual behavior. In all these examples concepts are elaborated to make sensible the link between violence in the media and in society. Co-occurrence makes no sense without an explanation, and it matters which explanation is offered. Do all of the explanations above seem equally believable? Were a politician to make a speech on fantasy fulfillment would it seem as reasonable as the cultivation explanation? Why? The “theory” cannot be evaluated until its explanatory rationale has been specified, and the brief examples above illustrate how significant, and distinct, such explanations can be. Finally, use theories, use concepts discussed in class, use concepts, rather than personal experience, anecdotes, examples, or intuitive explanations. The concepts and theories discussed in class represent an attempt to provide explanatory frameworks specifically to explain hypotheses. So use theories when possible to help explain the hypotheses. As Another Example Some explanations seem, on the surface, to make complete sense. For example, the following argument was offered by a student: First-born children often reach a vocabulary of 50 words at a faster rate than later born children, with a range variation of 14 to 21 months (Pine, 1995). First-born children develop a greater range an understanding of respective words at a younger age, while later born children tend to develop “frozen phrases.” “Frozen COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 28 phrases” are utterances that contain two or more words that have not previously occurred alone in the child’s vocabulary, or that contain one such word, provided it has not occurred in the same position in a previous multi-word utterance (Pine, 1995). Since first-born children achieve a greater vocabulary earlier, they tend to take on a mentor role with younger siblings, thus mentally stimulating themselves (Travis & Kohli, 1995). (Source: Anonymous.) Here the student did a good job of evidencing that first-born children develop a higher vocabulary level, but the student offered no explanation for why they do so. In this case, the student could have provided explanatory arguments such as the following: Given an assumption of a relatively fixed amount of time available to the parents, first-born children receive more parental attention and interaction, which later born children may be relatively deprived of, as larger numbers of children shrink the available time for any one child. Further, it is reasonable to assume that parental vocabulary resources are more valuable than sibling resources, due to the relatively advanced nature of parental grammar, and given the “mistakes” that might be built into a sibling’s vocabulary. Further, an older sibling will play some role in mentoring the younger sibling’s linguistic development, and the process of reflection and ruleconformity involved in teaching another person is likely to reinforce self-learning. Finally, the competition brought about by potential older sibling resentment of younger siblings may interfere with the learning of the younger sibling, as conflict and emotional disturbance distracts attention away from the more fundamental aspects of vocabulary building. These explanations build a conceptual substrate or foundation that bridges, or warrants, the claim that first-born children develop higher levels of vocabulary, and prevent such a claim from being purely tautological (i.e., tautology: first born children develop higher levels of vocabulary earlier than later born children because first born children mature faster than later born children). So explanations are the conceptual ladder that bridges evidence and claim so as to make their relationship sensible. They answer the question why. Yet Another Example A student argued in a paper: “Berger and Calabrese (1975) also believe that uncertainty plays a significant role in the preservation and dissolution of close relationships.” My question to the student was: WHY do they say uncertainty is important? Because uncertainty means not being able to predict things. Not being able to predict things means not knowing what effects behaviors have in the world. Not knowing what effects behaviors or actions have means not knowing how to control the world, or make the world work better through self-initiated actions. This results in the potential for bad things to happen, which creates anxiety. So, Berger and Calabrese say that uncertainty creates anxiety, and therefore we are motivated to reduce this uncomfortable state of anxiety by reducing our uncertainty. This is what is meant by an explanatory argument. It answers why people seek to reduce uncertainty, and it does this through a chain of causal predicates, each of which is linked thematically, coherently to the next, until a logical framework exists in which the proposition—the relationship between concepts, is sensible and satisfies the question: “but why?” COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 29 SELECTED EXAMPLES OF PUBLISHED HYPOTHESIS PAPERS Berelson, B., & Steiner, G. A. (1973). Human behavior: An inventory of scientific findings. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World. Berger, C. R., & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and beyond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human Communication Research, 1, 99-112. Burgoon, J. K., & Jones, S. B. (1976). Toward a theory of personal space expectations and their violations. Human Communication Research, 2, 131-146. Burr, W.R., Leigh, G. K., Day, R. D., & Constantine, J. (1979). Symbolic interaction and the family. In W. R. Burr, R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & I. L. Reiss (Eds.), Contemporary theories about the family (pp. 42-111). New York: Free Press. Deutsch, M. (1949). A theory of co-operation and competition. Human Relations, 2, 129-152. Devers, C. E., Dewett, T., Mishina, Y., & Belsito, C. A. (2009). A general theory of organizational stigma. Organization Science, 20, 154-171. doi: 10.1287/orsc.1080.0367 Fincham, F. D. (1992). The account episode in close relationships. In M. L. McLaughlin, M. J. Cody, & S.J. Read (Eds.), Explaining one’s self to others: Reason-giving in social context (pp. 225244). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Fisher, R. J. (1990). Chapter 5: An eclectic model of intergroup conflict. The social psychology of intergroup and international conflict resolution (pp. 87-115). New York: Springer-Verlag. Greene, J. (1984). A cognitive approach to human communication: An action assembly theory. Communication Monographs, 51, 289-306. Kaplowitz, S. A. (1978). Towards a systematic theory of power attribution. Social Psychology, 41, 131-148. Keyton, J., Ford, D. J.,& Smith, F. L. (2008). A mesolevel communicative model of collaboration. Communication Theory, 18, 376-406. Kuo, C-C. (2004). Research on Impacts of Team Leadership on Team Effectiveness. Journal of American Academy of Business, 5(1/2), 266-277. Levine, T. R. (2014). Truth-default theory (TDT): A theory of human deception and deception detection. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 33, 378-392. Liden, R. C., & Mitchell, T. R. (1988). Ingratiatory behaviors in organizational settings. Academy of Management Review, 13, 572-587. Mack, R. W., & Snyder, R. C. (1957). The analysis of social conflict--Toward an overview and synthesis. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 1, 221-248. Millar, F. E., & Rogers, L. E. (1988). Power dynamics in marital relationships. In P. Noller & M. A. Fitzpatrick (Eds.), Perspectives on marital interaction (pp. 98-120). Philadelphia: Multilingual Matters Ltd. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 30 Nahapiet, J., & Ghoshal, S. (1998). Social capital, intellectual capital, and the organizational advantage. Academy Of Management Review, 23(2), 242-266. doi:10.5465/AMR.1998.533225 Phillips, A. P., & Dipboye, R. L. (1989). Correlational tests of predictions from a process model of the interview. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 41-52. Rodgers, R. H. (1987). Postmarital reorganization of family relationships: A propositional theory. In D. Perlman & S. Duck (Eds.), Intimate relationships: Development, dynamics, and deterioration (pp. 239-268). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Schlenker, B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982). Social anxiety and self-presentation: A conceptualization and model. Psychological Review, 92, 641-669. Seibold, D. R., & Spitzberg, B. H. (1982). Attribution theory and research: Formalization, critique, and implications for communication. In B. Dervin & M. J. Voight (Eds.), Progress in communication sciences (Vol. 3, pp. 85-125). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Spitzberg, B. H. (1994). Intercultural effectiveness. In L. A. Samovar, & R. E. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader (7th ed., pp. 347-359). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Stech, E. L. (1979). A grammar of conversation with a quantitative empirical test. Human Communication Research, 5, 158-170. Treadway, D. C., Adams, G. L., Ranft, A. L., & Ferris, G. R. (2009). A meso-level conceptualization of CEO celebrity effectiveness. Leadership Quarterly, 20, 554-570. Doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.04.008 Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1980). The acquisition and use of social knowledge: Basic postulates and representative research. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 558-573. Wyer, R. S., Jr., & Srull, T. K. (1986). Human cognition in its social context. Psychological Review, 93, 322-359. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 31 ANYONE CAN FORMULATE THEORETICAL AXIOMS AND PROPOSITIONS Laws of Biological Inconvenience: 1. Law of Mechanical Repair: The greasier your hands, the more your nose itches and/or the greater your need to pee. 2. Law of Bio-Mechanics: The severity of an itch is inversely proportional to one’s reach. 3. Law of the Bath: The more fully immersed in water, the more likely a phone will ring. 4. Law of Gravity: Dropping any tool, nut, bolt, screw, when dropped, increases the likelihood it will roll to the least accessible place in the universe. Laws of Social Activity and Impressions 5. Law of Probability: The probability of being seen by others is directly proportional to the stupidity of your act. 6. Law of Close Encounters: The more you don’t want to be seen with someone is directly proportional to the likelihood of being seen with that person. 7. Law of Random Numbers: Dialing a wrong number increases the likelihood that (a) it won’t return a busy signal, and (b) someone will answer. Laws of (Bad) Luck & Self-Selection: 8. Law of Physical Surfaces: The chances of an open-faced jelly sandwich landing face down on a floor are directly correlated to the newness and cost of the carpet or rug. 9. Line Variation Law: Changing traffic lanes or lines increases the likelihood that it will move more slowly than the one you left. 10. Seat Selection Corollary: At any event, the people whose seats are furthest from the aisle, always arrive last. They are the ones who will leave their seats several times to go for food, beer, or the toilet and who leave early before the end of the performance or the game is over. The folks in the aisle seats come early, never move once, have long gangly legs or big bellies and stay to the bitter end of the performance. The aisle people also are very surly folk. 11. Murphy's Law of Lockers: If there are only 2 people in a locker room, they will have adjacent lockers. 12. Wilson's Law of Commercial Marketing Strategy: As soon as you find a product that you really like, they will stop making it. Law of (Dis)Proof: 13. Law of the Result: When you try to prove to someone that a machine won't work, it will. 14. Doctors' Corollary: If you don't feel well, make an appointment to go to the doctor, by the time you get there you'll feel better. But don't make an appointment, and you'll stay sick. 15. Law of Logical Argument: Anything is possible IF you don't know what you are talking about. Source: indeterminate—adapted from an extensively forwarded email. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 32 SELECTIONS FROM SPITZBERG'S "CHAMBER OF HORRORS" Try to avoid the following pitfalls: (1) avoid statements of non-relationship (e.g., 'X is not related to Y'); (2) avoid including explanations in the hypothesis itself (e.g., "X is positively related to Y because of theory Z's rationale that..."); (3) avoid using prescriptive (e.g., "S's should do X when Y occurs") or purely descriptive non-probabilistic (e.g., "X may/can be related to Y") wording; (4) avoid introducing terms into the hypothesis that have not been adequately explained or defined yet; and (5) avoid introducing categorical variables that distort the form of the relationship (e.g., "LOW X is positively related to HIGH Y's"); (6) avoid introducing set/subset redundancies in multiple hypotheses (e.g., P1: Females disclose more than males; P2: Androgynous females disclose more than androgynous males). Below are some of the "best of the worst" that have crossed my tired eyes. H1?: Knowledge also relates to mindlessness in an inverse parabolic function, in that they are positively related to each other until the mindlessness ceases to be mindless. H2?: Empathizing with co-workers strengths, weaknesses, and working style relates negatively towards escalating conflict resulting in an inability to see another's view. H3?: Equity restoring to a relationship is negatively related to low self-esteem individuals. H4?: Reactions in adolescents relate back to anxious, neurotogenic, disparging, masochistic, and manic depressive parents. H5?: Individual’s interaction with a caregiver as a child affects their relationship bondage with a potential partner as an adult. H6?: The purpose of negotiation is to resolve a disagreement between two or more parties. H7?: In certain negotiation circumstances nonverbal aspects of communication assume a greater significance than does verbal language. H8?: Levels of immediate physical and psychological trauma suffered by victims will vary. H9?: Child abuse is determined by the factors involved. H10?: The use of threats as an influence tactic does not exacerbate conflicts in general, but is limited to the occasions on which they are used. H11?: Conflicts are likely to be influenced by power. H12?: Sex differences are loosely related to relationship goals. H13?: Attraction is positively related and influential in choice of sexual partners. H14?: Males are negatively related to female interviewers of equal status; whereas females are not affected significantly to status and the sex of the interviewer. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 33 H15?: Mindlessness is negatively related to information processing in interactions. H16?: Deception is negatively related to the truth. H17?: Proximity increases physical closeness. H18?: Viewing the mediatior’s role as folkloric trickster is positively related to their job title. H19?: Guests of a television talk show depend on the reaction of the audience for support of their heinous actions, therefore after the show is over whether or not they take the advice of the audience and change their way, depends on the reaction they received. H20?: Family hour will need to be changed back to true family hour. H21?: Hip hop is a response to conditions of property and disempowerment. H22?: Media coverage of the violent topics in Rap music helps alert people to the possible problems. H23?: Families no longer have a need for face-to-face communication. H24?: Self-esteem of men and women in a romantic relationship. H25?: The way that conflict is managed in marriages can lead to divorce if not managed in the right way. H26?: Preoccupied attached individuals have a negative correlation with self-perception. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 34 WHAT NOT TO DO WITH HYPOTHESES What follows are examples of flawed hypotheses taken from previous student papers. Each hypothesis is analyzed to illustrate common mistakes observed in the conceptualization and verbalization of hypotheses. Students are responsible for studying these examples, and thereby avoiding the types of mistakes indicated. If there are questions about any of the issues raised, please raise them at the appropriate time in class or professor office hours. Recall that a hypothesis is any statement of relationship between or among variables. A variable is a concept that takes on different values. The entire function of science is to make the nature of the relationships among things understandable. The more precise the statement of relationship, the more understandable the hypothesis is. What follows is an effort to assist in making the hypotheses more precise. 1. MOOD IS POSITIVELY RELATED TO SEXUAL INITIATION. Overly generic variables: "Mood" may be positive or negative. Since mood is not specified, the hypothesis, as worded, implies that any increase in mood state will correspond to an increase in sexual initiation. Thus, as worded, an increase in depression increases sexual initiation, as would sadness anger, and so forth. The key is to understand that the term "mood" is a concept that can take on different values or levels (e.g., low to high, negative to neutral to positive, etc.). A hypothesis is an attempt to specify how their values or levels correspond to the values or levels of another concept, in this case, the occurrence of sexual initiation. The hypothesis intends to say: "The experience of positively valenced or labeled states is positively related to the likelihood of sexual initiation." 2. LOW SELF-ESTEEM IS POSITIVELY RELATED TO INTERPERSONAL HOSTILITY. Mixing variable labels: This is one of the most common, yet subtle problems in writing hypotheses. By specifying only "low" self-esteem, this hypothesis ignores "medium" and "high" self-esteem. When two-thirds of a variable's possible values are removed, its ability to "relate" to anything is eliminated because there is no real opportunity for the variable to vary beyond a very narrow range of values. The above statement intends to say simply: "Self-esteem is negatively related to interpersonal hostility" or "Low self-esteem persons are significantly more interpersonally hostile than high self-esteem persons." Think about what the terminology means! If something is categorized as "low" it needs to be compared to something that is "high." As an additional note, it is unclear if self-esteem relates to the frequency with which all hostile actions occur, the intensity of hostile actions, the duration of hostile actions, all three, some combination, or some other aspect of hostility. Hostility is likely to take on many different features. It assists the hypothesis to make these features clear in the hypothesis. Thus, "Self-esteem is negatively related to the frequency and breadth of interpersonally hostile behaviors." COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 35 3. SELF-DISCLOSURE IS POSITIVELY RELATED TO INTIMACY. Overly unidimensional concepts: On the face of it, this is sensible, right? Yet, this hypothesis is deeply flawed. First, anyone looking into the two concepts involved here, self-disclosure and intimacy, should soon find that they are multidimensional. This means that a valid use of the term actually implies many distinct but related concepts. For example, self-disclosure has been found to vary importantly in terms of breadth (number of topics), depth (intimacy of topics), reciprocity (degree to which partner discloses in response), honesty, and valence (positive or negative). Similarly, intimacy can be viewed in terms of domain (e.g., recreation I, sexual, social, emotional, etc.) or type (e.g., caring, commitment, interdependence, physical, etc.). The point is not that student papers have to agree with "the" view with which I am familiar, but that any responsible research effort will uncover the complexity (i.e., multidimensionality) of the concepts. In this case, the hypothesis implies that all forms of self-disclosure are equally related to all forms of intimacy. Is this sensible? It seems unlikely that as disclosure of negative information increases that sexual intimacy increases, yet this is exactly what the hypothesis implies until it is specified. Second, this is a perfect candidate for a curvilinear relationship. That is, extremely low or extremely high amounts of disclosure are likely to impede or inhibit intimacy respectively. Yet, this hypothesis implies that even at extremely high levels of disclosiveness that intimacy will also be extremely high. Third, would this hypothesis apply reasonably to all contexts? For example, does it apply to task-oriented groups, or superior-subordinate, or disengaging, relationships? Unlikely. 4. MALES ARE POSITIVELY CORRELATED TO APPRECIATION OF OBSCENE HUMOR. Concepts not variable: "People" are not variables; their characteristics are. This hypothesis intends to compare 'maleness" to "femaleness. It makes the mistake of correlating categories. Categories are nominal level variables, whereas correlation requires ordinal level variables (recall COMM 350?). Gender, religion, ethnic group, etc. are categorical variables, and as such, should be framed as "difference" hypotheses rather than correlational hypotheses. Thus: "Males appreciate obscene humor significantly more than females." 5. FRIENDSHIPS ARE HIGHLY VALUABLE TO OLDER MALES AND FEMALES. Overly definitional: This is overly definitional in form. First, does it imply the friendships are more valuable to older persons than younger person? (e.g., "The value of friendships for males and females increases as age increases"). Second, if a relationship is being identified that holds for all members of a set (i.e., persons), there is no need to specify all categories of that set (i.e., "males and females"). Thus, "The value of friendships increases as age increases." Third, to what is friendship "valuable"? Valuable in psychological health, physical health, social health, etc.? Thus, for example, "As age increases, the perceived value of friendship in preventing depression and loneliness increases." COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 36 6. ADULTERY, ALCOHOL ABUSE, AND FINANCIAL DIFFICULTIES ARE POSITIVELY RELATED TO DIVORCE. ‘NS’ hypothesis: This qualifies in the realm of an “NS” hypothesis (“No Shit!”). Technically there is nothing wrong with this hypothesis. However, it is about as unsurprising, and therefore, uninformative, as a statement can be. It’s a little like saying “Oxygen is important to human life.” Science has a healthy respect for demonstrating what is supposed to be true actually is true. But come on! Put a little imagination into the paper and come up with a statement that allows parents to think their children are learning something not already known without the degree. 7. AS MISUNDERSTANDINGS INCREASE, POOR COMMUNICATION INCREASES. Tautology: It is a little like saying that “X is true because it is X” (i.e., something is taken to be true because of the way in which it is defined). In this hypothesis, the question arises because for most commonsense notions, “misunderstanding” would be viewed as a form or subset of “poor communication.” Thus, if X “includes” Y, then it tends to make little logical sense to say that with more X, there will be more Y, since this is virtually true by definition of X. 8. A COWORKER WILL USE MORE INDIRECT COMPLIANCE-GAINING STRATEGIES WHEN ADDRESSING A SUBORDINATE. Lacking comparative condition: This sounds sensible. However, it is incomplete. Whenever the words “more” or “less” are used, the question arises “less (or more) than what?” In this case, it is not entirely clear which of the following phrases should be added to the end of the hypothesis: “...THAN WHEN ADDRESSING A SUPERIOR,” “...THAN DIRECT STRATEGIES,” or “THAN A COWORKER.” Complete the comparison implied by “more” or “less.” 9. SCHOLARS BELIEVE THAT ALCOHOLISM IS A DISEASE, WHILE OTHERS BELIEVE IT TO BE A BAD HABIT. Descriptive, not relational: This fails in three important senses. First, although it sounds like it is saying something substantive about the nature of alcoholism, it really is only saying something about what certain people believe about alcoholism, which was not the point of the paper. Second, it is not about communication, and therefore is not very topical to the assignment or major. Third, the hypothesis is definitional in content, not a hypothesis. There is no “relationship between variables” being described here. This hypothesis is stating something about what alcoholism is rather than what it is related to and how it is related. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 37 A NOTE ABOUT STRUCTURALLY CORRECT SYLLOGISMS THAT ARE NOT THEORETICALLY SOUND. A student constructed the following propositions in syllogistic form: H1: A strong relationship with in-laws is positively related to the quality of a marriage. H2: The quality of a marriage is positively related to the similarity of the partners’ socioeconomic status. H3: Therefore, a strong relationship with in-laws is positively related to the socioeconomic status of the couple. This is a structurally sound syllogism (i.e., [A] [B] [C]), but it makes no conceptual or explanatory sense as a coherent whole. It makes sense that similarity in socio-economic factors might lead to a ‘stronger’ (e.g., more satisfying) relationship, but it makes little sense to suggest that a stronger relationship would lead to a partner who was previously uneducated, poor, or from male to become educated, rich, and female. SES factors (sex, education, wealth, etc.) are distal variables—variables that have long duration and tend to precede current relationship status. So the implicit order of the syllogism could easily be made more sensible: H1: Similarity in partners’ SES is positively related to partner relationship satisfaction. H2: Relationship satisfaction is positively related to satisfaction with in-law relationships. H3: Therefore, similarity in partners’ SES is positively related to satisfaction with in-law relationships. Or: H1: Similarity in partners’ SES is positively related to likelihood of satisfaction with in-law relationships. H2: In-law relationship satisfaction is positively related to satisfaction with relational satisfaction. H3: Therefore, similarity in partners’ SES is positively related to relational satisfaction. The point is to consider the validity of a syllogism both at a structural (i.e., formal logic) and explanatory (i.e., narrative) level. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 38 Name: Mortimer Shnerd Email: wild&crazy@girlsgonewild.com Class: COMM 495 Date: today’s date YOUR PAPER TITLE HERE (e.g., “IT’S ALL ABOUT ME”: NARCISSISM, ENTITLEMENT, AND SURVEILLANCE) In mythology, Narcissus was a hunter renowned for his beauty and very self-aware of this feature. Feeling himself so attractive, those who loved him were viewed as undeserving. Nemesis, realizing this personal flaw, lured Narcissus to a body of water in which Narcissus was able to gaze at his own reflection. Falling in love with the reflection, Narcissus perishes, unable to remove himself from the gaze of self-love. In modern parlance, narcissism is a personality trait indicating a grandiose sense of self, and it influences a variety of interpersonal behaviors. H1 : Narcissism is positively related to relational proprietariness and entitlement. New media have introduced the potential for anyone to construct an idealized identity in online environments. In such a context, narcissism is both enabled and reinforced. Narcissism is a grandiose and exaggerated sense of uniqueness, need for recognition, and entitlement (Salzman, 1993). This need for love and recognition leads paradoxically to a defensiveness to criticism or deprivation, resulting in reactionary aggression toward any source perceived as threatening the face of the narcissist (Twenge & Campbell, 2003). Narcissistic defensiveness is commonly manifest in strategies such as confrontation and derogation of others, while reactions to a lack of positive feedback are often characterized by threats and antagonism (Twenge & Campbell, 2003). Narcissism is likely to lead to a lack of empathy for partners, and a belief that the partner is obliged to serve the narcissist’s needs. As such, narcissism is expected to produce a tendency to view a partner as a type of relational object, or property. Therefore: H2 : Relational proprietariness and entitlement are positively related to romantic cybersurveillance of relational partner(s). COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 39 Proprietariness refers to a set of beliefs and values that view a partner as a form of property (Hannawa, Spitzberg, Wiering & Teranishi, 2006). Statements as benign as “you belong to me,” can co-occur with more chilling statements such as “you’ll never escape from me.” Closely related are beliefs representing rights or responsibilities that obtain to property, such as “I have a right to know where you are all the time” and “if I can’t have you, no one can” (Hannawa et al., 2006). The underlying beliefs and values reflect various forms of entitlement. Entitlement is a belief that, like property, a person can legitimately control a partner’s behavior, access and use information and social relationships. One way in which such entitlement could find expression is cyber-surveillance, a process of frequent or obsessive monitoring of another’s social network site(s) for the purposes of uncertainty reduction. It is distinct from cyber-stalking, in which the intent is to evoke fear or communicate threat (Spitzberg & Hoobler, 2002). Instead, cyber-surveillance is primarily intended to provide the ‘lurker’ information about a potential partner’s activities, commitment, or relational intentions. Therefore: H3 : Narcissism is positively related to romantic cyber-surveillance of partner(s). Cyber-surveillance is likely to increasingly become a source of conflict in people’s relationships. Such surveillance not only potentially provides information that permits a partner’s commitment to a relationship to be questioned, but when a partner’s surveillance is discovered, it may communicate a lack of trust. The negotiation of self and of relationship will likely continue to become a contested site for the emergence of relationship problems, as people adjust to the ‘brave new world’ enabled by such media. COMM 495 (Fall 2015): Capstone—p. 40 References Hannawa, H. F., Spitzberg, B. H., Wiering, L., & Teranishi, C. (2006). “If I can’t have you, no one can”: Development of a relational entitlement and proprietariness scale (REPS). Violence and Victims, 21(5), 539-560. doi:10.1891/vivi.21.5.539 Salzman, L. (1993). Narcissism and obsessionalism: An interpersonal psychoanalytic approach. In J. Fiscalini & A. L. Grey (Eds.), Narcissism & the interpersonal self (pp. 241-253). New York: Columbia University. Spitzberg, B. H., & Hoobler, G. (2002). Cyberstalking and the technologies of interpersonal terrorism. New Media & Society, 14, 67-88. doi: 10.1177/14614440222226271 Stern, L. A., & Taylor, K. (2007). Social networking on Facebook. Journal of the Communication, Speech, & Theatre Association of North Dakota, 20, 9-20. Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2003). “Isn’t it fun to get the respect that we’re going to deserve?” Narcissism, social rejection, and aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(2), 261-272. doi:10.1177/0146167202239051 COMM 495: Capstone—p. 41 Appendix: Search History COMM 495: Capstone—p. 42 WRITING/APA: FORM: Demonstrates proficiency in grammar, syntax, semantics, academic voice, and application of APA style guidelines. Form displays…: ARGUMENT: CLAIMS: Demonstrates ability to articulate researchable claims specifying the interrelationship among variables. Form displays…: ARGUMENT: COMPOSITION: Demonstrates comprehensive arguments that include relevant and appropriate claims, warrants, & evidence. Form displays…: ARGUMENT: RESEARCH: Demonstrates ability to locate and appropriately cite and list recent, relevant, and reasonable scholarly research, consisting mostly of peerreviewed journal sources. Form displays…: CONTENT & SKILLS: Demonstrates knowledge and skills of a competent ‘citizen’ communicator germane to course in applying learning objectives to course-relevant topical or audience domain. Form displays…. ORIGINALITY (note: By SDSU policy, all instances of plagiarism invoke mandatory reporting to the Office of Student Rights and Responsibilities. 1: 0-20 (F) multiple types & instances within type of writing errors in expository text, &/or displays inconsistency in rule application; frequent re-editing or rephrasing to achieve more professional voice is suggested. 1: 0-20 (F) Key claims that are not clearly articulated or delineated. Propositions fail by level of scaling, relationship, or entailment. “Object lessons” are repeated. 1: 0-20 (F) Key claims of source(s) not clearly articulated or delineated. Specific reference to passages are not consistently or sufficiently provided, in detail or accuracy to test the claims. 1: 0-20 (F) No more than 1 or 2 directly or peripherally related external sources establishing the validity of the selected source claims, &/or those sources applied are distantly relevant to source claims; &/or sources lack recency, relevance, or scholarly imprimatur. 1: 0-20 (F) no representation of lecture, text, or syllabus-based content or learning objectives of course in the content of the paper. Paper lacks topicality or internalization of principles or claims established in course. Course: F COURSE FAILURE: Reproducing a whole paper, paragraph, or large portions of unattributed materials without proper attribution, whether represented by (a) multiple sentences, images, or portions of images, or (b) by percentage of assignment length, will result in assignment of an “F” for the course in which the infraction occurred. PERFORMANCE ASSESSMENT SCALE (PAS) 2: 21-40% (D) 3: 41-60% (C) moderately low level of professional Few types & instances within type of voice, composition, and grammatical writing errors in expository text, &/or form with moderate number of errors, inconsistency in rule application; inconsistency of rule application, or occasional re-editing or rephrasing required editing (intermediate to to achieve more professional voice is scales 1 & 3). suggested. 2: 21-40% (D) 3: 41-60% (C) Moderately low level of relationship Only minor or one or two claims or specification or implications of propositions need editing for sake of claims or propositions, or makes clarity. several errors in wording (intermediate to scales 1 & 3). 2: 21-40% (D) 3: 41-60% (C) moderate level of incomplete, vague, Some key claims of the source(s) or poorly evidenced arguments or articulated or delineated, but there claims (intermediate to scales 1 & 3). are some inconsistencies in the detail, gravity, or evidential basis provided in explicating the claims. 4: 61-80% (B) Moderately high level of professional voice, composition, and grammatical form with moderately few errors, inconsistency of rule application, or required editing (intermediate to scales 3 & 5). 4: 61-80% (B) Moderately high level of relationship specification or implications of claims or propositions, or makes few errors in wording (intermediate to scales 3 & 5). 4: 61-80% (B) Moderately high level of consistent, coherent, and consistent development of comprehensively formulated arguments in support of primary claims (intermediate to scales 3 & 5). 4: 61-80% (B) moderately high level of development of sound, articulated, and evidential warrants for claims, with high status and appropriate sources (intermediate to scales 3 & 5). 5: 81-100% (A) Writing displays consistent use of professional voice, composition, and grammatical form. 5: 81-100% (A) Propositions that are both logically sound, and sophisticated in their thematic connection &/or articulation of complex relationships. 5: 81-100% (A) Consistent, coherent, and consistent development of comprehensively formulated arguments in support of primary claims. 2: 21-40% (D) moderately low number and quality of location, citation, and listing of recent, relevant, and reasonable scholarly sources (intermediate to scales 1 & 3). 3: 41-60% (C) At least 1 to 2 studies relevant to each selected proposition, argument, component, or source claim, &/or some sources applied are distantly relevant to source claims; &/or sources lack recency, relevance, or scholarly imprimatur. 2: 21-40% (D) intermediary (to scales 1 & 3) mastery of course content, claims & established principles and practices, and learning objectives. 3: 41-60% (C) moderate representation of lecture, text, or syllabus-based content or learning objectives of course. Paper demonstrates only moderate topicality or internalization of principles or claims established in course. 4: 61-80% (B) intermediary (to scales 3 & 5) mastery of course content, claims & established principles and practices, and learning objectives. 5: 81-100% (A) excellent representation of lecture, text, or course content or learning objectives of course in the content of the paper. Paper demonstrates direct and central topicality or internalization of principles or claims established in course. Exacerbating conditions include: Amount: Evidence of infraction, even if fragmentary, is increased with a greater: (a) number of infractions, (b) distribution of infractions across an assignment, or (c) proportion of the assignment consisting of infractions. Exacerbating conditions include: Intent: Evidence of foreknowledge and intent to deceive magnifies the seriousness of offense and grounds for official response. Plagiarism, whether by accident or by ignorance still constitutes plagiarism—it is all students’ responsibility to make sure their assignments are not committing the offense. Exceptions: Any exceptions to these policies will be considered on a case-by-case basis, and only under exceptional circumstances. Assignment: F ASSIGNMENT FAILURE: Reproducing a sentence or sentence fragment with no quotation marks, but with source citation, or subsets of visual images without source attribution, will minimally result in an “F” on the assignment. 5: 81-100% (A) Each major claim is evidenced by sources high in scholarly credibility (i.e., relevance, recency, peer review, etc.) COMM 495: Capstone—p. 43 PAPER GRADING FEEDBACK BOILERPLATE Argument: Needs evidence/citation: This is a very specific empirical claim and needs evidence/backing (i.e., a citation). Comparative clause: If you have a “more than” or a “less than” formulation, you must specify the comparison group; otherwise it the question arises: “more than what?” or “less than what?”. Tautological: This is argument by definition—which makes the argument rather tautological— entity X is more Y because X’s engage in more Y. It’s a little bit like saying “Democracies are governments that engage in participative representation, so democracies are positively related to greater participation.” This doesn’t tell us anything other than what a democracy is. We need to know something about the nature of what factors lead to more democratic societies or governments for us to understand what democracy is. Thin conclusion: A little thin as a conclusion. Future directions? Research limitations? Theoretical implications? Grammar/Style: Nonparallel pronouns: “They” and “them” and “their” are PLURAL pronouns. If they are used to refer to “him,” “her,” “individual,” “you,” “me,” “I,” “one,” “person,” or other such singular pronoun, then it is nonparallel, and grammatically incorrect. Personalized “pronouns”: Just me? Gee. Avoid the generic “you” (and other self-referential pronouns, such as “we” and “our”). Of course it “personalizes” writing style [please note, the former sentence could have been written “Of course, you may think it personalizes your writing style.”]. But “you” also implies that whatever is being claimed [whatever you claiming] is specific just to the reader, and not more generally. Removing the “you” transforms the [your] claims into more universal and more assertive rhetorical forms. Rhetorically it is tempting to identify with the reader/audience, but scholars are attempting to establish general principles, and their writings are intended for not only a universal audience, but for all time. “We,” and “our,” “you,” “us,” and “I”, etc., refer to particular people reading the text—not anyone else, which seems exclusionary and temporary. Generic pronoun “one”: For the most part, it is best to avoid use or reliance on the generic pronoun “one” (as in: “One should not use generic pronouns,” or “Pronouns are often misused by someone”). The term is passive in construction, and creates complications because the word has multiple potential referents or meanings. That/Which: The word “which” is intended for interrogatives (i.e., questions; e.g., “Which of the following is not a style of conflict management?”) and for unrestricted clauses (e.g., “The word ‘which’ is intended for unrestricted clauses, which are set off by a comma.”). So, if the word “that” fits the sentence, use it instead. Effect/Affect: OK, so I know that each term has two distinct meanings, but they are distinct both within and between them. It really ought not be too difficult to learn the four meanings, and when they are appropriate. Effect (verb): rarely used because it is awkward, it means “to cause” (e.g., “The use of disclosure can effect positive change in relationships”). Effect (noun): an outcome or result of some cause (e.g., “Disclosure has the effect of producing greater satisfaction in relationships”). Affect (verb): to influence (not to cause, which is specific, but to influence, have some unspecified ‘effect’ upon) or change something (e.g., “Disclosure affects satisfaction in a variety of ways”). Affect (noun): a fairly technical term used in social sciences as a synonym for “emotion” (e.g., “Jealousy is an affect blend of insecurity and fear”). COMM 495: Capstone—p. 44 Media/Medium: The term “media” refers to the plural—i.e., multiple means through which communication occurs. The singular form is “medium.” It is common for laypersons to make this mistake, but it would be nice if a major in communication might recognize the difference. Apostrophe: The apostrophe is your friend! No contractions are permitted in APA, but possessives require the apostrophe. Contractions: APA does not allow contractions, except in quotations or deliberately informal speech (e.g., in titles). Single ‘space’ after punctuation: APA calls for a single space after words and punctuation—not two spaces. Page breaks: Learn to create page breaks rather than slop a bunch of empty spaces into the formatting! Header: Learn to insert legitimately ‘embedded’ headers rather than just typing them at the tops of page breaks. Often times: Given that “often” means something happening many “times,” isn’t this a redundant phrase? It: Try not to begin paragraphs with the indefinite pronoun “It,” as “it” has no immediate referent for the reader. Quotation Practices: Voice: Part of developing your argumentative competence is promoting your own ‘voice’ in formulating your writing. One of the ways of doing this is to subordinate authors whenever reasonable to do so. This means putting them in a parenthetical position at the end of the sentence, rather than drawing attention to the fact that you are relying on someone else to do your thinking and writing for you. There are obvious reasons for noting authors explicitly, such as when citing specific findings of specific studies, but otherwise, they can usually be subordinated. Quotation bracketing: Quotation marks surround the quote—not the authors of the quote. The authors are not part of the quotation itself. Page #!: Direct quotations require page numbers. Otherwise, you are expecting any reader to have to read an entire article or even book in order to verify your quotation! Minimize quotations: Apparently you didn’t see the comment in the instructions: “please note, there is no quotation, and no need for quotation. Quotations should be used extremely sparingly—instructors want to see the students’ writing, not someone else’s.” Page #: You only use page numbers for direct quotations, but if there is a direct quotation, there must be a page number (web-based html sources are not allowed as sources, so you should have .pdf or hardcopy with pages). Scholarly sources: The paper instructions state: “Evidence claims with scholarly journal sources… There must be a minimum of three A.P.A. style (6th ed.) citations* of scholarly journal references you used to support your explanations of the propositions (there may be additional citations and the additional sources do not need to be from journals).” Textbook cites: Undergraduate textbooks are not considered scholarly because they are written for a relatively “lay” audience, rather than for scholars. URL: For any source available as a .pdf through the library search engines, there is no need to provide a URL address. A .pdf is a digital scan of the original hard copy of the published work, and therefore, it is the same as having the original source. The function of a URL is primarily to assist reader review when the original is not available. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 45 Citation/Reference Formatting Practices: Ruler Tabs!: Learn to use the ruler/tabs or [Ctl-T] to create ‘hanging indents’ for your references—do NOT use hard returns and tab indents, because if there are any changes to font or format, it screws up the formatting. Page #: Direct quotations require a specified page number. Volume #: The Volume # is italicized too in APA; the issue number is not part of APA references (except when the journal is paginated from page 1 for every issue such as with popular magazines; something that rarely occurs among scholarly journals). et al.: The first time you cite the source, cite all the authors (unless there are 6 or more authors). Secondary citations: There should be no secondary citations. If Jones cites Smith, and you only have Jones informing you about what Smith says, but you want to cite Smith, then go find Smith. If it is important enough to include as part of your argument, it is important enough to go find so you can (a) verify its content and claims, and (b) learn more about the claim you are arguing. Retrieval: If you can download the .pdf of a document, this is a legitimate version of the “hardcopy,” and therefore you do not need to include the online retrieval information. Citing the online retrieval information is only necessary for “html” based documents. APA reference format: It is so extremely discouraging to see this. I lecture over it. I provide elaborate instructions, and elaborate example. I even test over it. And yet, it appears that no real attention is paid to the APA format requirements, thereby requiring extensive correction on my part, taking my valuable time and effort away from providing more substantive commentary on your ideas. Formatting matters! Why is it important to do “deep” research on topics? Self-Esteem: You may want to see: Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4, 1-44. Why is it important to do “deep” research on topics? Self-disclosure: Research decades ago by Wheeless and others demonstrated that self-disclosure occurs along a number of different dimensions, including: (1) depth, (2) breadth, (3) positive valence/content, (4) negative valence/content, (5) honesty/dishonesty, (6) reciprocity, and (7) frequency. So, when it is claimed that “self-disclosure” is positively or negatively related to X, the writer is implicitly claiming that each and every one of these forms of disclosure are similarly related to X. It is highly unlikely, however, that both disclosure of positive things and disclosure of negative things about self are similarly related to much of anything, just as an example. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 46 Why is it important to do “deep” research on topics? Media & Body Image: You may want to see: Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2009). “Everybody knows that mass media are/are not [pick one] a cause of eating disorders”: A critical review of evidence for a causal link between media, negative body image, and disordered eating in females. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 9-42. It’s about communication: Despite being a communication major, many students continue to commit the fallacy that relationships do not work because they “lack” communication. It is rarely a matter of lacking communication, but lacking competent communication. Remember, abusive relationships have plenty of communication—it’s just bad communication. Avoiding communication is itself sending a message—“I don’t want to talk to you.” Please, please, please, use the concepts we teach in the way in which you understand and appreciate the communication process. Related, research and theory have increasingly been identifying the specific adaptive role that bias and inaccuracy play in the success of certain relationships (see., e.g., [a] Boyes, A. D., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (2007). Meta-perceptions of bias in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 286-306. [b] Spitzberg, B. H. (1993). The dialectics of (in)competence. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 137-158. [c] Spitzberg, B. H. (1994a). The dark side of (in)competence. In W. R. Cupach & B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.), The dark side of interpersonal communication (pp. 25-49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.). COMM 495: Capstone—p. 47 DIFFERENTIATING SCHOLARLY JOURNALS FROM POPULAR PERIODICALS FEATURES SCHOLARLY JOURNALS Authorship Author(s) typically experts or specialists in field, from recognized academic institution, with ‘terminal academic degrees’ Articles usually reviewed and critically evaluated by a board of experts in the field (i.e., refereed, or “peer reviewed”); statement of article submission procedures provided. The “gold standard”: blind peer review A reference list (works cited) and/or footnotes are always provided to ground the article in the existing research literature Written in the technical or theoretical jargon of the field for scholarly readers (e.g., professors, researchers, students, etc.) Articles typically more structured, may include ‘boilerplate’ sections (e.g., abstract, literature review, method, results, conclusion, references or bibliography) Longer articles, providing in-depth analysis Author typically a staff writer or journalist; credentials often not provided Illustrations that support the text such as tables of statistics, graphs, maps, or photographs, labeled numerically as Table 1, Table 2, etc. Illustrations with glossy or color photographs; typically include advertisements between articles Editors Credits/ Citations Language/ Audience Format/ Structure Length Special Features POPULAR PERIODICALS Articles are not evaluated by experts in the field, but by the staff editors A reference list is typically not provided, although names of reports or references may be listed for “suggested reading” Written in non-technical language for anyone to understand; written for broad appeal Articles often do not follow a specific format or structure Shorter articles, providing broader overviews of topics Serialization Typically, “Vol.” and “Issue” numbers are identified, and pagination of the articles is continuous from one issue to the next within a volume or year. Each new issue begins with page 1, and individual issues most likely referred to by “month” and/or day/date rather than volume(issue) numbers Vs. Textbook Scholarly books and scholarly edited books, are written for other scholars and tend to be heavily referenced throughout. Textbooks, particularly undergraduate textbooks, are written for students— not for scholars. They are NOT acceptable for citation in proposition paper assignments. Adapted from SDSU Library handout COMM 495: Capstone—p. 48 SPITZBERG’S TIPS FOR RESEARCHING YOUR ASSIGNMENT 1. Use the best search engines: use PsycINFO, Communication & Mass Media Complete (CMMC), Web of Science (and selective other search engines, depending on relevance to the topic; e.g., if researching a topic about communication in organizations, Business Source Premier may be useful). 2. Use multiple search engines: Select multiple search engines simultaneously. 3. Narrow the search parameters: Restrict to “scholarly peer-reviewed” sources. 4. Keep record of search terms: Formulate a search thesaurus—come up with as many related search terms as possible, and then try multiple (Boolean) combinations 5. Optimize search terms: Use ‘wild cards’ (e.g., use an asterisk--*--to allow multiple versions of search terms to be returned) 6. Scan search returns: Always scan several pages of returned sources, rather than settling for the first few sources that pop up. 7. Never forget the value of ingratiation: See if your professor has conducted research on your topic—know your audience. 8. Be responsible for your sources: Remember—do not cite any source in which you have not read the original (hardcopy or .pdf); you cannot know what the article says by its abstract alone 9. Download sources in APA style: Do put your sources in folder and save in APA style, but do not assume they will be downloaded in accurate APA style—you still have to edit to assure correctness 10. Experience the treasure hunt: There are dozens to thousands of scholarly sources on almost every imaginable topic. With a little effort, you can find more than an “adequate” source—you can find “the perfect” sources relevant to your topic. 11. Select based on arguments that need to be made: In general, in regard to empirical claims, your arguments can be no better than the relevance, recency, representativeness, and validity of the research you use to support your argument, so investment in research is almost always worth it. 12. Think creatively: You cannot know what needs to be argued, and what can be argued, until you know what has already been done on a topic. 13. Understand that this is a fundamental competence: Learning research tools is a fundamental competence that will help your literacy, employability, and writing ability for the rest of your life. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 49 A SAMPLING OF POSSIBLE CONCEPTUAL ENTRIES INTO AN ENCYCLOPEDIA OF COMMUNICATION TOPIC/ENTRY (N > 700) Acculturation processes & communication Accuracy Action Assembly Theory Activist Media Addiction and Exposure Advertising, History of Aesthetics Affective Disposition Theories Affects and Media Exposure Agenda Building Agenda Setting Effects Aging and Cognitive Processing Aging & Message Production & Processing Americanization of media Anxiety-Uncertainty-Management Theory Apologies, Remedial Episodes Appraisal Theory Argumentative Discourse Aristotle (Murray?) Arrangement and Rhetoric Art as Communication Artifacts Attending to Mass Media Attention to Media Content by Life-Span Attention Attitude – Behavior Consistency Attitude Accessibility Attitudes and Values, Media Effects on Attitudes Attribution Processes Audience Segmentation Bad News, Communicating Political Communication Bi- and multilingualism Bias in the News Black Feminist Media Studies Body Images in the Media Branding Bureaucracy and Communication Business Discourse DISCIPLINARY CATEGORY Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Media Production and Content Information Processing and Cognitions Development Communication Exposure to Communication Content Media History Communication Theory and Philosophy Exposure to Communication Content Exposure to Communication Content Political Communication Media Effects Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions International Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Language and Social Interaction Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Visual Communication Popular Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Developmental Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Media Effects Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Exposure to Communication Content Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Media Production and Content Feminist and Gender Studies Reality Perception through the Media Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Organizational Communication Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 50 Candidate Image Cartoons Catharsis Theory Celebrity Culture Celebrity Stalking Censorship Change Management and Communication Child Protection, Media Regulations Classroom Instructional Technology Classroom Management Techniques Classroom Power Classroom Questioning Classroom Student-Teacher Interaction Cognition Cognitive Dissonance Cognitive Science Collective Action and Communication Collective Opinion and the Media Comforting Communication Comm Apprehension and Social Anxiety Comm Apprehension in the Classroom Comm App., Intervention Techniques Comm Professions and Academic Research Communication Satisfaction Commodification of the Media Commodity Feminism Communibiology Communication Accommodation Theory Communication Apprehension Communication as a Field and Discipline Communication Education, Goals of Communication Evaluation Research Communication Inequality Communication Management Communication Networks Communication Research and Politics Communication Skills Across The Life Span Comm. Strategies For Empowerment Comm. Technology and Development Communication Technology Communication Theory and Philosophy Communication, Relationship Rules Communicative Aggression Communicator Style Political Communication Visual Communication Media Effects Popular Communication Interpersonal Communication Communication and Media Law and Policy Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Developmental Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Exposure to Communication Content Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Reality Perception through the Media Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Communication as a Field and Discipline Interpersonal Communication Media Economy Feminist and Gender Studies Information Processing and Cognitions Language and Social Interaction Interpersonal Communication Communication as a Field and Discipline Instructional/Educational Communication Development Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Organizational Communication Communication as a Field and Discipline Developmental Communication Development Communication Development Communication Communication and Technology Communication Theory and Philosophy Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 51 Communities of Practice Community Integration Community Structure Model Competence/Performance Distinction Compliance Gaining Comprehension Computers-User Interaction Conflict and Cooperation Across Life-Span Conflict as Media Content Conflict Resolution Consistency Theories Construction of Reality through the News Constructivism in Information Processing Constructivism Contingency Model of Conflict Control and Authority in Organizations Conversation Analysis Credibility Effects Crisis Communication Critical Rationalism Cultivation Effects Cultivation Theory Cultural Imperialism Theories Cultural Patterns and Communication Cultural Studies Cultural Studies, Feminist Popular Culture Culture & Comm, Ethnography Culture and Health Communication Culture Industries Culture, Definition and Concepts Cumulative Media Effects Cyberfeminism Cybernetics Dating Relationships Death, Dying and Communication Deception Detection Accuracy Deception in Discourse Deceptive Message Production Decision Making processes in organizations Deduction vs Induction vs Abduction Deep Structure Deliberativeness: Political Communication Delivery and Rhetoric Dependency Theories Language and Social Interaction Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Language and Social Interaction Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Exposure to Communication Content Developmental Communication Media Production and Content Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Exposure to Communication Content Media Production and Content Information Processing and Cognitions Communication Theory and Philosophy Strategic Communication Organizational Communication Language and Social Interaction Media Effects Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Communication Theory and Philosophy Reality Perception through the Media Communication Theory and Philosophy International Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Feminist and Gender Studies Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Popular Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Media Effects Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Interpersonal Communication Developmental Communication Interpersonal Communication Language and Social Interaction Interpersonal Communication Organizational Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Language and Social Interaction Political Communication Rhetorical Studies Development Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 52 Desensitization Design Theory Development and Gender Development Communication Campaigns Development Communication Development Discourse Development, Resistance to Developmental Communication Dialogic perspectives Diffusion of Information and Innovation Digital Divide Digitization and Media Convergence Direct and Indirect Effects Directives and Requests Disability and Communication Disasters and Communication Disclosure and Communication Disclosure in Interpersonal Communication Discourse Comprehension Discourse Markers Discourse Discourse, Cognitive Approaches Discursive Psychology Dissent in organizations Distance Education Diversity in the Workplace Doctor-Patient Talk Double-Bind Communication Drama in Media Content Dual Coding Theory E-Democracy Educational Communication Educational Media Content Educational Television: Children Election Campaign Communication Electronic Mail Emic vs Etic Research Emotion & Communication in organizations Emotion and Discourse Emotion Emotional Arousal Theory Emotions, Media Effects on Empathy Theory English Only Movements Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Development Communication Development Communication Development Communication Development Communication Development Communication Developmental Communication Organizational Communication Media Effects Communication and Technology Communication and Technology Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Language and Social Interaction Communication Theory and Philosophy Language and Social Interaction Language and Social Interaction Organizational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Language and Social Interaction Interpersonal Communication Popular Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Political Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Developmental Communication Political Communication Media History Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Language and Social Interaction Information Processing and Cognitions Media Effects Media Effects Exposure to Communication Content Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 53 Enjoyment/Entertainment Seeking Exposure to Communication Content Entertainment Education Exposure to Communication Content Entertainment, Effects of Media Effects Environment and Social Interaction Interpersonal Communication Environmental Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Escapism Exposure to Communication Content Ethnic Media and their Influence Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Ethnicity and Exposure to Communication Exposure to Communication Content Ethnography of Communication Language and Social Interaction Ethnolinguistic Vitality and Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Ethnomethodology Language and Social Interaction Ethos and Rhetoric Rhetorical Studies Evolutionary Theory Exposure to Communication Content Excitation & Arousal Exposure to Communication Content Excitation Transfer Theory Media Effects Exemplification and Exemplars, Effects of Media Effects Exemplification in Health Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Expectancy Violation Interpersonal Communication Expectancy-Value Models Exposure to Communication Content Extended Parallel Process Model Information Processing and Cognitions Eye Behavior Interpersonal Communication Facial Expressions Interpersonal Communication Family Communication Patterns Developmental Communication Family Decision Making Developmental Communication Fandom Popular Communication Fantasy-Reality Distinction Developmental Communication Fear Induction on Children Through Media Content Developmental Communication Fear Induction through Media Content Media Effects Feedback Processes in Organizations Organizational Communication Feminine Mystique Feminist and Gender Studies Femininity and Feminine Values Feminist and Gender Studies Feminist Communication Ethics Feminist and Gender Studies Feminization of Media Content Feminist and Gender Studies Fetishiziation Popular Communication Freedom of Communication Media History Friendship and Communication Developmental Communication Friendship and Peer Interaction Interpersonal Communication Frustration Aggression Theory Media Effects Functional Analysis Communication Theory and Philosophy GLBT Media Studies Feminist and Gender Studies Gaze in Interaction Language and Social Interaction Gender and Discourse Language and Social Interaction Gender and Journalism Journalism Gender and Media Organizations Feminist and Gender Studies COMM 495: Capstone—p. 54 Gender, Representation in the Media Genre Gestures and Kinesics Gestures in Discourse Girl Culture Globalization of the Media Goals & Social Interaction Goals Gossip and Small Talk Groupthink Habituation Hacktivism Hate Speech and Ethnophaulisms Health Behavior Change Health Belief Model Health Campaigns For Development Health Campaigns, Communication in Health Communication and Internet Health Communication Health Communication, Ethics in Health Disparities, Communication in Health Literacy Hermeneutics Hostile Media Phenomenon Human-Computer Interaction Hybridity Theories Identification Identities and Discourse Identity Development and Communication Identity Politics Idiographic vs Nomothetic Science Image Management Image Restoration Theory Imagined Interactions Immediacy Impersonal Effects Implicit Personality Theories Impression Management Individual Differences & Info. Processing Information Technology, Development Information Technology, Economics Of Information Literacy Information Overload Information Processing Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Interpersonal Communication Language and Social Interaction Popular Communication Media Economy Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Language and Social Interaction Small Group/Decision-Making Exposure to Communication Content Communication and Technology Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Development Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication Theory and Philosophy Reality Perception through the Media Communication and Technology International Communication Exposure to Communication Content Language and Social Interaction Developmental Communication Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Interpersonal Communication Nonverbal Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Information Processing and Cognitions Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Communication and Technology Communication and Technology Communication and Technology Communication and Technology Information Processing and Cognitions COMM 495: Capstone—p. 55 Information Processing, Self-Concept Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing, Stereotypes Information Processing and Cognitions Information Science Communication Theory and Philosophy Information Seeking Exposure to Communication Content Information Society Communication and Technology Information Communication Theory and Philosophy Infotainment Media Production and Content Ingratiation and Affinity Seeking Interpersonal Communication Initial Interaction Interpersonal Communication Interaction Adaptation Theory Interpersonal Communication Interaction Communication Theory and Philosophy Interactional Sociolinguistics Language and Social Interaction Interactivity, Concept of Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural & Intergroup Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intercultural Communication in Health-Care Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intercultural Communication Training Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intercultural Conflict Styles and Facework Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intercultural Norms Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Interethnic Relationship in Families Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intergenerational Communication Developmental Communication Intergroup Accommodative Processes Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intergroup Comm. & Discursive Psychology Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intergroup Contact and Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intergroup dimensions of organizational life Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intermediality Communication Theory and Philosophy International Comm. Association (ICA) Communication as a Field and Discipline International Communication International Communication International Political Communication Political Communication Internet and Popular Culture Popular Communication Internet Use Across Life-Span Developmental Communication Internet, Technology of Communication and Technology Interorganizational Networks Organizational Communication Interpersonal Attraction Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Competence & Social Skills Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Comm., Sex & Gender Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Conflict Interpersonal Communication Intimacy, Communication Characteristics of Interpersonal Communication Intimate Talk with Family and Friends Language and Social Interaction Intimate Violence Interpersonal Communication Intrinsic Motivation and Volition Exposure to Communication Content Invention and Rhetoric Rhetorical Studies Issue Management in Politics Political Communication Issue Voting Political Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 56 Jealousy Job Satisfaction, Communication Predictors Knowledge Gap Effects Knowledge Interests Knowledge Management Language Acquisition in Childhood Language and Social Interaction Language and the Internet Language Attitudes in Intergroup Contexts Language Varieties Latitude of Acceptance Leadership in Organizations Learning and Communication Learning Organizations Libel and Slander Limited Capacity Model Linear and Nonlinear Models Linguistic Pragmatics Linguistics Listening Logos and Rhetoric Long-Distance Relationships Marginality, Stigma and Communication Marital Communication Marital Typologies Masculinity and Media Meaning Media and Group Representations Media and Perceptions of Reality Media as Political Actors Media Content and Social Networks Media Content in Interpersonal Comm. Media Dependency Theory Media Events and Pseudo Events Media Literacy Media Messages & Family Communication Media Use Across Life-Span Media Use and Child Development Media Use by Children Media's Role in Society Mediated Social Interaction Mediated Terrorism Mediating Factors Mediation Discourse Interpersonal Communication Organizational Communication Media Effects Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Developmental Communication Language and Social Interaction Communication and Technology Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Language and Social Interaction Media Effects Organizational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Organizational Communication Communication and Media Law and Policy Information Processing and Cognitions Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Communication Theory and Philosophy Information Processing and Cognitions Rhetorical Studies Interpersonal Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Reality Perception through the Media Political Communication Reality Perception through the Media Reality Perception through the Media Media Effects Political Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Reality Perception through the Media Developmental Communication Developmental Communication Developmental Communication Media Systems in the World Interpersonal Communication Political Communication Media Effects Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 57 Mediatization of Organizations Theory Mediatization of Politics Mediatization of Society Medium Theory Meeting Technologies Memory and Rhetoric Memory, Message Memory Memory, Person Memory Mentoring Message Design Logics Message Discrimination Message Effects, Structure of Message Production Metaphor Meta-Pragmatics Metonymy Microethnography Mindlessness and Automaticity Models of Communication Modernity Multitasking Navigation Negative Campaigning Negativity Negotiation and Bargaining Network Organizations & Technology News as discourse Nonverbal Communication and Culture Nonverbal Signals, Effects of Objectivity in Science Obsessive Relational Intrusion On-Line Relationships Opinion Leader Order of Presentation Organizational Assimilation Organizational Change Processes Organizational Comm., Critical Approaches Organizational Conflict Organizational Crises, Communication In Organizational Culture Organizational Discourse Organizational Ethics Organizational Identification Organizational Image Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Political Communication Media Effects Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Rhetorical Studies Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Instructional/Educational Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Media Effects Information Processing and Cognitions Visual Communication Language and Social Interaction Visual Communication Language and Social Interaction Information Processing and Cognitions Communication Theory and Philosophy Development Communication Exposure to Communication Content Exposure to Communication Content Political Communication Media Production and Content Interpersonal Communication Communication and Technology Political Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Media Effects Communication Theory and Philosophy Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Media Effects Media Effects Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement COMM 495: Capstone—p. 58 Organizational Metaphors Organizational Communication Organizational Structure Organizational Communication Organizational Symbolism Organizational Communication Organization-Public Relationships Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Organizations, Cultural Diversity In Organizational Communication Paradigm Communication Theory and Philosophy Paralanguage Interpersonal Communication Para-Social Interactions and Relationships Exposure to Communication Content Parental Mediation Strategies Developmental Communication Participative Processes in Organizations Organizational Communication Participatory Action Research Development Communication Participatory Communication Development Communication Party Political Communication Political Communication Pathos and Rhetoric Rhetorical Studies Patient-Provider Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Pedagogy, Communication in Instructional/Educational Communication Perceived Reality as Comm. Process Reality Perception through the Media Perceived Reality as a Social Process Reality Perception through the Media Perception Exposure to Communication Content Personal Communication by CMC Communication and Technology Personality & Exposure to Communication Exposure to Communication Content Personality Development & Communication Developmental Communication Personalization of Campaigning Political Communication Persuasion and Resistance Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Persuasion Media Effects Phatic Communication Language and Social Interaction Physical Effects of Media Content Media Effects Planned Behavior, Theory of Information Processing and Cognitions Planned Social Change & Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Playing Exposure to Communication Content Pluralistic Ignorance and Ideological Biases Reality Perception through the Media Pluralistic Ignorance Reality Perception through the Media Politainment Political Communication Politeness Theory Interpersonal Communication Political Advertising Political Communication Political Cognitions Political Communication Political Communication Culture Political Communication Political Communication Systems Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Consultant Political Communication Political Cynicism Political Communication Political Discourse Political Communication Political Economy of the Media Media Economy Political Efficacy Political Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 59 Political Knowledge Political Language Political Media Content, Quality Criteria Political Media Use Political Personality in Media Democracy Political Persuasion Political Socialization Through The Media Political Symbols Politics in Popular Communication Popular Communication and Social Class Popular Communication Popular Culture and News Media Popular Culture Populism and Responsiveness Pornography, feminist debates on Positioning Theory Postcolonial Theory Postdevelopment Postfeminism Postmodernism and Communication Power and Discourse Power in Intergroup Settings Power, Dominance & Social Interaction Pragmatism Prejudiced & Discriminatory Comm. Presence Press Conference Prevention and Communication Priming Theory Propaganda in World War II Propaganda Propaganda, Visual Communication of Proxemics Public Opinion, Media Effects on Public Sphere Public Sphere, Fragmentation Of Publics, Situational Theory Questions and Questioning Realism Reality and Media Reality Reciprocal Effects Reciprocity & Compensation in Interaction Reification Relational Control Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Political Communication Feminist and Gender Studies Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Communication Theory and Philosophy Development Communication Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Language and Social Interaction Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Interpersonal Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Exposure to Communication Content Journalism Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Media Effects Media History Political Communication Visual Communication Interpersonal Communication Media Effects Political Communication Political Communication Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Language and Social Interaction Communication Theory and Philosophy Media Production and Content Media Effects Interpersonal Communication Popular Communication Interpersonal Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 60 Relational Dialectics Relational Maintenance Relational Schemata Relational Termination Relational Uncertainty Relationship Development Religion and Popular Communication Remediation Rhetoric and Dialectic Rhetoric and Epistemology Rhetoric and Ethics Rhetoric and Ethnography Rhetoric and Gender Rhetoric and Language Rhetoric and Logic Rhetoric and Media Studies Rhetoric and Narrativity Rhetoric and Orality-Literacy Theorems Rhetoric and Philosophy Rhetoric and Politics Rhetoric and Psychology Rhetoric and Religion Rhetoric and Semiotics Rhetoric and Social Protest Rhetoric and Social Thought Rhetoric and Technology Rhetoric and Visuality Rhetoric of Science Rhetoric, Argument and Persuasion Rhetoric, Epideictic Rhetoric, Nonverbal Rhetoric, Postmodern Rhetorical Criticism Rhetorical Studies Rhetorics, New Rhetorics Right to Know Risk Communication Risk Perceptions Rituals in Popular Communication Schema and Media Effects Schemas Schemata, Knowledge Structures Scripts Secular Social Change Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Popular Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Communication and Media Law and Policy Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Popular Communication Media Effects Information Processing and Cognitions Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Communication and Social/Behavioral Change COMM 495: Capstone—p. 61 Selective Attention Information Processing and Cognitions Selective Exposure Exposure to Communication Content Selective Perception & Selective Retention Exposure to Communication Content Self Presentation Interpersonal Communication Semiotics Communication Theory and Philosophy Sensation Seeking Exposure to Communication Content Sense-making Organizational Communication Sex/Pornography as Media Content, Effects Media Effects Sex and Pornography Online Communication and Technology Sex Role Stereotypes in the Media Feminist and Gender Studies Sexual Compliance-Gaining & Safe Sex Talk Interpersonal Communication Sexism in the Media Feminist and Gender Studies Sexual Violence in Media Feminist and Gender Studies Sexualization in Media Feminist and Gender Studies Sibling Interaction Interpersonal Communication Sign Systems Visual Communication Sign Visual Communication Sleeper Effect Media Effects Soap Operas Media Production and Content Social Behavior, Media Effects on Media Effects Social Capital and Communication in Health Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Capital, Media Effects on Media Effects Social Cognitive Theory Exposure to Communication Content Social Comparison Theory Exposure to Communication Content Social Conflict and Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Exchange Interpersonal Communication Social Identity Theory Exposure to Communication Content Social Interaction Structure Interpersonal Communication Social Judgment Theory Media Effects Social Marketing Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Mobilization Development Communication Social Movements and Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Networks Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Norms Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Perception Reality Perception through the Media Social Stereotyping and Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Social support in Health Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Support & Interpersonal Comm. Interpersonal Communication Socialization by Media Reality Perception through the Media Sound Bites Media Production and Content Spectator Gaze Visual Communication Speech Acts Language and Social Interaction Speech Anxiety Instructional/Educational Communication Speech Codes Theory Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 62 Speech Fluency and Speech Errors Spin and Double Speak Spin Doctor Spiral of Silence Spirituality and Development Sports and the Media, History of Sports as Popular Communication Stages of Change Model Stakeholder Theory Stereotypes Stereotyping and the Media Stimulus-Response Model Storytelling and Narration Strategic Communication Strategic Framing Structuralism Structuration Theory Student Communication Competence Style and Rhetoric Supervisor-Subordinate Relationships Support Talk Suspension of Disbelief Sustainable Development Symbol Systems Symbolic Annihilation Symbolic Interaction Symbolic Politics Symbolism Systems Theory Tabloidization Taste Culture Teacher Affinity-Seeking Teacher Assertiveness Teacher Clarity Teacher Comforting and Social Support Teacher Communication Style Teacher Confirmation Teacher Feedback Teacher Immediacy Teacher Influence and Persuasion Teacher Self-Disclosure Teacher Socio-Communicative Style Teacher Use of Humor Technologically-Mediated Discourse Information Processing and Cognitions Strategic Communication Political Communication Reality Perception through the Media Development Communication Media History Popular Communication Exposure to Communication Content Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Media Production and Content Reality Perception through the Media Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Rhetorical Studies Organizational Communication Language and Social Interaction Exposure to Communication Content Development Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Political Communication Visual Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Media Production and Content Visual Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 63 Technology, Social Construction Of Communication and Technology Telephone Talk Language and Social Interaction Televised Debates Political Communication Television as Popular Culture Popular Communication Television for Development Development Communication Terrorism and Communication TechnologiesCommunication and Technology Text and Intertextuality Communication Theory and Philosophy Theory of Reasoned Action Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Third Person Effects Reality Perception through the Media Transcribing and Transcription Language and Social Interaction Two-Step-Flow of Communication Media Effects Uncertainty and Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Uncertainty Management Interpersonal Communication Uncertainty Reduction Theory Interpersonal Communication Uses-and-Gratifications Exposure to Communication Content Verbal Aggressiveness Interpersonal Communication Victimization Media Effects Video Games Popular Communication Videomalaise Reality Perception through the Media Violence as Media Content, Effects of Media Effects Virtual Communities Communication and Technology Virtual Reality Media History Visuals, Cognitive Processing Of Information Processing and Cognitions Voice, Prosody, and Laughter Language and Social Interaction Voyeurism Visual Communication War Propaganda International Communication Women in the Media, Images of Feminist and Gender Studies Women’s Movement and Media Media History Women's Communication and Language Feminist and Gender Studies Zapping and Switching Exposure to Communication Content COMM 495: Capstone—p. 64 Abortion Assessment of Couples Cohabitation Abuse and Violence in Relationships Assessment of Families Cohesiveness in Groups Abused Women Remaining in Relationships Assortative Mating Collectivism, Effects on Relationships Attachment Theory Commercial Channels for Mate Seeking Attachment Typologies, Childhood Commitment, Predictors and Outcomes Attraction, Sexual Commitment, Theories and Typologies Attribution Processes in Relationships Communal Relationships Aunts and Uncles, Relationships With Communication Accommodation Theory Balance Theory Communication Processes, Verbal Bank Account Model Communication Skills Barrier Forces to Relationship Dissolution Communication, Gender Differences in Accommodation Accounts Accuracy in Communication Acquaintance Process Adolescence, Romantic Relationships in Adoption Adult Attachment, Individual Differences Batterers Communication, Instant Messaging and other New Media Advice, Self-Help and Media Advice about Relationships Behavioral Couple Therapy Communication, Nonverbal Affection Behavioral Parent Training Communication, Norms and Rules Affiliation Beliefs about Relationships Community Involvement Affiliation in Non-human Species Beliefs, Destiny vs. Growth Comparison Levels Affinity Seeking Belonging, Need for Compassionate Love Affirmation Bereavement Compatibility African-American Families Betrayal Complementarity Age at First Marriage Biological Systems for Courtship, Mating, Reproduction, and Parenting Computer-Mediated Communication Adulthood, Sibling Relationships in Aggressive Communication Aging Processes and Relationships Agreeableness AIDS, Effects on Relationships Alcohol and Sexual Assault Birth Control, Relational Aspects Blended Families Body Image, Relationship Implications Bogus Stranger Paradigm Conflict Measurement and Assessment Conflict Patterns Conflict Prevalence and Sources Conflict Resolution Conflict, Family Alcoholism, Effects on Relationships Borderline Psychopathology, in Relationships Conflict, Marital Alienation and Anomie Boredom in Relationships Connectedness, Tension with Autonomy Alternative Relationship Life-Styles Boston Couples Study Conscientiousness, Effects on Relationships Altruistic Love Bullying Alzheimers, and Relationships Capitalization Ambivalence Caregiver Role American Couples Study Care-giving across the Life Span Anger in Relationships Casual Sex Apologies Celibacy, in Long-term Relationships Approach and Avoidance Orientations Change in Relationships over Time Arguing Child Abuse and Neglect Aristotle and Plato on Relationships Children's Peer Groups Arousal and Attraction Closeness Arranged Marriages Cognitive Behavioral Couple Therapy Courtship and Dating, Cross-Cultural Differences in Asian-American Families Cognitive Processes in Relationships Courtship, History of Contextual Influences on Relationships Cooperation and Competition Coping, Developmental Influences Couple Identity Couple Therapy Couple Therapy for Substance Abuse Couples in Later Life Couples in Middle Age Coupling Policies COMM 495: Capstone—p. 65 Courtship, Models and Processes of Dual-Earner Couples Extradyadic Sex Covenant Marriage Dyadic Data Analysis Extraversion and Introversion Criticism in Relationships Dyssemia Facework Culture and Relationships Early Years of Marriage Project Facial Expressions Dark Side of Relationships Economic Pressures, Effects on Relationships Fairness in Relationships Dating and Courtship in Adolescence and Young Adulthood Dating and Courtship in Mid- and Later Life Dating Services Dating, First Date Deception and Lying Egalitarian Relationships Elder Abuse and Neglect Embarrassment Emotion in Relationships Emotion Regulation in Relationships Decision-Making in Relationships Emotion Regulation, Developmental Influences Dependence Emotional Communication Dependency Paradox Emotional Contagion Depression and Relationships Emotional Intelligence Deteriorating Relationships Emotionally Focused Couple Therapy Developing Relationships Empathic Accuracy and Inaccuracy Developmental Designs (Longitudinal, Cross-Sectional, Retrospective) Empathy Deviance, Relationship Effects Dialectical Processes Employment Effects on Relationships Empty Nest, Effects on Marriage End-of-life, Relationship Issues During Disabilities, Chronic Illness, and Relationship Functioning Enemies Discipline in Families Engagement, as a Relationship Stage Discourse Analysis Envy Disillusionment, in Long-Term Relationships Equity Theory Display Rules Ethical Issues in Relationship Research Dissolution of Relationships, Breakup Strategies Evolutionary Perspectives on Women's Romantic Attraction Dissolution of Relationships, Causes Evolutionary Perspectives, Applications to Relationships Dissolution of Relationships, Coping and Aftermath Equivocation Falling in Love Familiarity Principle of Attraction Families, Coping with Cancer Families, Definitions and Typologies Families, Demographic Trends Families, Intergenerational Relationships in Families, Public Policy Issues and Family Communication Family Data, Analysis of Family Functioning Family Life Cycle Family Relationships in Adolescence Family Relationships in Childhood Family Relationships in Late Adulthood Family Relationships in Middle Adulthood Family Relationships in Young Adulthood Family Routines and Rituals Family Therapy Family Therapy for ADHD in Children and Adolescents Family Therapy for Adult Psychopathology Family Therapy for Noncompliance in Children and Adolescents Exchange Orientation Family Therapy for Substance Abuse in Adolescents Dissolution of Relationships, Processes Exchange Processes Fatal Attractions Diversity in Relationships Excitation Transfer Theory Father-Child Relationships Division of Labor in Households Ex-Partner and Ex-Spouse Relationships Fear of Death, Relational Implications Divorce and Preventive Interventions for Children and Parents Expectation States Theory, Applied to Relationships Feminist Perspectives on Relationships Divorce, Children and Expectations about Relationships Fictive Kinship Divorce, Co-parenting after Field of Availables and Eligibles Divorce, Effects on Adults Experimental Designs for Relationship Research Divorce, Prevalence and Trends Expressed Emotion Double Standard in Relationships Extended Families Fertility and Family Planning First Impressions Flirting COMM 495: Capstone—p. 66 Food and Relationships Hostility Iowa Youth and Families Project Forgiveness Hurt Feelings Isolation, Health Effects Foster Care, Relationships in Idealization Jealousy Friends with Benefits Ideals about Relationships Job Stress, Relationship Effects Friendship Formation and Development Illness, Effects on Relationships Justice Norms Applied to Relationships Friendship, Conflict and Dissolution Imaginary Companions Kin Relationships Friendships in Adolescence Incest Kin Selection Friendships in Childhood Individuation Kinkeeping Friendships in Late Adulthood Infant-Caregiver Communication Kissing Friendships in Middle Adulthood Infatuation Language Usage in Relationships Friendships in Young Adulthood Information Seeking Leadership Friendships, Cross-Sex Ingratiation Leisure Activity Friendships, Sex Differences and Similarities Initiation of Relationships Lesser Interest, Principle of In-laws, Relationships with Lies in Close and Casual Relationships Insight-Oriented Couple Therapy Life Review, Role of Relationships Integrative Behavioral Couple Therapy Life-Span Development and Relationships Fun in Relationships Gain-Loss Theory of Attraction Gay, Lesbian, and Bisexual Relationships Gender Roles in Relationships Gender Stereotypes Gender-Role Attitudes Goal Pursuit, Relationship Influences Interaction Analysis Interdependence Theory Intergenerational Family Relations Intergenerational Transmission of Abuse Liking Listening Loneliness Loneliness, Children Goals in Relationships Intergenerational Transmission of Divorce God, Relationships with Internet and Social Connectedness Long-Distance Relationships Gossip Internet Dating Grandparent-Grandchild Relationship Internet, Attraction on Longitudinal Studies of Marital Satisfaction and Dissolution Gratitude Interpersonal Attraction Group Dynamics Interpersonal Dependency Guilt and Shame Interpersonal Influence Happiness and Relationships Interpersonal Process Model of Intimacy Hard-to-Get Phenomenon Interpersonal Psychotherapy Health and Relationships Interpersonal Sensitivity Health Behaviors, Relationships and Interpersonal Spread of Interracial and Interethnic Relationships Interracial Friendships, in Adolescence Health, Relationships as a Factor in Treatment Interruptions, Conversational Helping Behaviors in Relationships Intervention Programs, Domestic Violence Hispanic/Latino Families Loneliness, Interventions Loss Love, Companionate and Passionate Love, Prototype Approach Love, Typologies Love, Unreciprocated Lust Maintenance Behaviors in Relationships Marital Satisfaction and Quality Marital Satisfaction, Assessment of Marital Stability, Prediction of Marital Typologies Holidays and Relationships Intervention Programs, Satisfaction and Stability Marketplace Approaches to Courtship, Love and Sex Homelessness and Relationships Intimacy Marriage and Health Honeymoons Intimacy, Individual Differences Related to Marriage and Sex Hooking Up, Hookups Hormones Related to Relationships Investment Model Marriage Markets Marriage, Benefits of COMM 495: Capstone—p. 67 Marriage, Expectations about Obsessive Relational Intrusion Marriage, Historical and Cross-Cultural Trends Opening Lines PREP (Premarital Relationship Enhancement Program) Openness and Honesty Prevention and Enrichment Programs for Couples Optimism, Effects on Relationships Privacy Ostracism Proximity and Attraction Mate Preferences PAIR (Process of Adaptation in Intimate Relationships) Project Psychodynamic Theories of Relationships Mate Selection Parent-Adolescent Communication Materialism and Relationships Parental Investment Theory Psychopathology, Genetic Transmission of Media Depictions of Relationships Parent-Child Communication about Sex Media Influences on Relationships Parent-Child Relationships Mediation, Marriage Dissolution Parenthood, Transition to Memories and Relationships Parenting Mental Health and Relationships Personal Idioms Mentoring Programs Personal Relationships Journals Mentoring Relationships Personal Relationships, Defining Characteristics Reciprocity of Liking Personal Space Reciprocity, Norm of Personality Traits, Effects on Relationships Rejection Minding the Relationship Misattribution Perspective-Taking Relational Aggression Money and Relationships Persuasion Relationship Distress and Depression Mood and Relationships Pet-Human Relationships Relationship Messages Morality and Relationships Physical Attractiveness Stereotype Mother-Child Relationships Physical Attractiveness, Defining Characteristics Relationship Science, Disciplines Contributing to Marriage, Transition to Matching Hypothesis Mate Guarding and Poaching Metacommunication Military and Relationships Motivation and Relationships Multi-Generational Households Mutual Cyclical Growth Physical Attractiveness, Role in Relationships Psychopathology, Influence on Family Members Psychotherapists, Relationships with Public Policy and Relationships Rape Rapport Reassurance-Seeking Rejection Sensitivity Relationship Types and Taxonomies Religion, Spirituality, and Relationships Remarriage Narcissism, Effects on Relationships Physical Environment, Effects on Relationships Need Fulfillment in Relationships Physicians, Relationships With Resilience Negative Affect Reciprocity Play Fighting Resource Theory Negative Interactions During Late Life Polygamy Respect Negotiation Popularity Responsiveness Neighbor Relations Pornography, Effects on Relationships Retirement, Effects on Relationships Neuroticism, Effects on Relationships Positive Affectivity Revenge Newlyweds Post-Divorce Relationships Rewards and Costs in Relationships Nonverbal Communication, Status Differences Power Distribution in Relationships Risk, in Relationships Power, Predictors of Rochester Interaction Record Predicting Success or Failure of Relationships Role Theory and Relationships Norms about Relationships Nostalgia Pregnancy and Relationships Obsessive Love Prejudice Nonverbal Involvement Repairing Relationships Romanticism Rules of Relationships Safe Sex COMM 495: Capstone—p. 68 Satisfaction in Relationships Social Anxiety Technology and Relationships Secret Relationships Social Capital Temperament Secret Tests of Relationship Status Social Comparison, Effects on Relationships Touch Secrets Security in Relationships Self-Concept and Relationships Self-Disclosure Self-Esteem, Effects on Relationships Self-Evaluation Maintenance Model Self-Expansion Theory Social Convoy Theory Social Exchange Theory Social Identity Theory Social Inequalities and Relationships Social Isolation Social Learning Theory Transference Transformation of Motivation Transgressions Trust Turning Points in Relationships Uncertainty Reduction Theory Unconditional Positive Regard Self-Monitoring and Relationships Social Networks, Changes in Relationships Understanding Self-Presentation Social Networks, Dyad Effects on Unmitigated Communion Self-Regulation in Relationships Validation in Relationships Self-Verification Social Networks, Effects on Developed Relationships Sex and Love Social Neuroscience Vengeance Sex Differences in Relationships Social Penetration Theory Virginity Sex in Established Relationships Social Relations Model Vulnerability-Stress-Adaptation Model Sex Ratio Social Skills in Childhood Waist-to-Hip Ratio and Attraction Sex-Role Orientation Social Skills, Adults Warmth, Interpersonal Sexual Aggression Social Support and Health Weak Ties Sexual Communication between Adults Social Support, Intervention Groups Weddings Sexual Dysfunctions Social Support, Nature of Work-Family Conflict Sexual Harassment Socialization Work-Family Spillover Sexual Intent, Perceptions of Socialization, Role of Peers Workplace Relationships Sexual Intercourse, First Experience of Socioeconomic Status Sexual Motives Socioemotional Selectivity Theory Sexual Prejudice Sociometer Hypothesis Sexual Standards Sociosexual Orientation Sexuality Speed-Dating Sexuality and Attachment Stage Theories of Relationship Development Sexuality in Adolescent Relationships Stalking Sexually Transmitted Diseases and Relationships Story-telling Shared Activities Stress and Relationships Shyness Substance Use and Abuse in Relationships Sibling Relationships Similarity in Ongoing Relationships Similarity Principle of Attraction Singlehood Single-Parent Families Social Allergies Suicide and Relationships Symbolic Interaction Theories Systems Theories Taboo Topics Teacher-Student Relationships Values and Relationships COMM 495: Capstone—p. 69 ILLOGICAL ASTROLOGICAL? The sun’s annual path (caused by the earth’s orbit) takes it across a band of the sky known as the zodiac. Along the way, the sun passes in front of a number of constellations. If we could see the sun and stars at the same time, our “sign” would be the constellation against which the sun lay at the time we were born. Thus, we have such “signs” as Aries, Taurus, each of which corresponds to a star grouping in the heavens. What most people don’t realize is that, since astrology’s origin millennia ago, it has not kept up with our knowledge of the cosmos. Scientists today know the earth undergoes a 25,800-year wobble that alters the positions of celestial bodies. CALENDAR DATES (Your birthday is in?) TRADITIONAL HOROSCOPE DATES (What you THINK you are) Jun 21—Jul 20 Jul 21—Aug 10 Aug 11—Sep 16 Sep 17—Oct 30 Oct 31—Nov 22 Nov 23—Nov 28 Nov 29—Dec 17 Dec 18—Jan 19 Jan 20—Feb 16 Feb 17—Mar 11 Mar 12—Apr 18 Apr 19—May 13 May 14—Jun 21 Source: May 21—Jun 21 Jun 22—Jul 22 Jul 23—Aug22 Aug 23—Sep 22 Sep 23—Oct 23 Oct 24—Nov 21 ACTUAL ASTROLOGICAL SIGN (The sign you were ACTUALLY born in) GEMINI CANCER LEO VIRGO LIBRA SCORPIUS OPHIUCHUS Nov 22—Dec 21 Dec 22—Jan 19 Jan 20—Feb 18 Feb 19—Mar 20 Mar 21—Apr 19 Apr 20—May 20 Reuben H. Fleet Science Museum SAGITARIUS CAPRICORNUS AQUARIUS PISCES ARIES TAURUS COMM 495: Capstone—p. 70 NO “CORRELATIONAL” EVIDENCE FOR ASTROLOGY Investigated folklore concepts of personality in 2600 adults. Specifically, the relationship between MMPI profiles and astrological sign, the year of birth in the Oriental yearly cycle, and the day of the week of birth was studied…. [Analyses] showed no effect size larger than 2% on any MMPI scale. No evidence was found in support of these folklore concepts. [Source: Dahlstrom, W. G., Hopkins, D., Dahlstrom, L., Jackson, E., & Cumella, E. (1996). MMPI findings on astrological and other folklore concepts of personality. Psychological Reports, 78, 1059-1070.] Examined the astrological doctrine of aspects in 3 studies with 562 university students and 349 astrologers or astrology students. … In Study 1, Ss were asked to specify how personality varies with specific aspects between 32 pairs of planets; these predictions were tested against scores on 10 personality dimensions. In Study 2, a comparison of soft and hard aspects was made using multivariate methods. Study 3 ignored the traditional aspects and investigated whether personality varied as a function of the angular separation between planets at birth. No evidence for the validity of astrological ideas was found in any of these studies.[Startup, M. (1985). The astrological doctrine of "aspects": A failure to validate with personality. British Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 307-315.] NO “EXPERIMENTAL” EVIDENCE FOR ASTROLOGY In Exp I, Ss provided information from which their natal charts and interpretations were constructed by astrologers. Each S then attempted to select his/her own natal chart interpretation from a group consisting of his/her own and 2 others. In Exp II, the astrologers were separately given the natal chart of a random S and a California Psychological Inventory (CPI) description of the S's personality traits along with CPI descriptions of 2 other Ss. The astrologers selected the 2 CPIs (1st and 2nd choice) that described personalities closest to the personality indicated by the natal chart. The astrologers also rated the CPIs for closeness of fit to the natal chart descriptions. Results indicate that Ss and astrologers scored at a level consistent with chance. The data support arguments against natal astrology as practiced by astrologers.[Carlson, S. (1985). A double-blind test of astrology. Nature, 318(6045), 419-425.] NO “PEER” OR “EXPERT” VALIDITY FOR ASTROLOGY [Asked] university students and a [family] member … or a close friend (the nominee) to try to identify which 1 of 5 cast horoscopes was based on the S's birth data. In addition, each S was rated on 5 personality dimensions by the astrologer who cast the horoscopes, the S herself, and her nominee. Finally, the S completed [a]… Personality Questionnaire. Neither the Ss nor the nominees were able correctly to identify the S's horoscope. Regarding the 5 personality dimensions, the astrologer's rating did not correlate with the psychometrically obtained scores, the Ss' self-rating, or the ratings of the S by the nominee. [Tyson, GA. (1984). An empirical test of the astrological theory of personality. Personality & Individual Differences, l 5, 247-250.] One reason people may “see validity” in their signs is because they already “know” their sign, and this leads them to “read themselves into” such descriptions students … were divided into a group of examiners and a group of examinees. Each examiner was given a packet consisting of the 12 astrological sign descriptions, while each examinee completed a questionnaire, which the examiners used to ascertain their astrological sign. Examiners then gave their examinees 2 sign descriptions, the correct astrology description and the description immediately following it. Results show that Ss who knew their astrological sign judged the personality profile corresponding to their sign to be a more accurate description of their personality than an alternative astrological sign profile. [Hamilton, M. M. (1995).Incorporation of astrology-based personality information into long-term self-concept. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 10, 707-718.] BELIEVERS MAY NEED IT AS A COPING RESPONSE TO THEIR OWN LIMITATIONS 2 astrologers [and] 67 of their clients. The demographic data of these consultees were then compared with population data obtained from the census, and personality and attitudinal data were compared with those of 136 controls. … The variable that explained the most variance was stress. This was followed by a measure of political control and then a measure of religiosity. It is concluded that, in many cases, consulting an astrologer is a response by an individual to the stresses with which he/she is faced and that these stresses are linked mainly to the individual's social roles and to his/her relationships. It is suggested that these stresses arise partially as a result of the person's lack of social skills. [Tyson, GA. (1982). People who consult astrologers: A profile. Personality & Individual Differences, 3, 119-126.]