The Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variation Presenters: 2003 NYSSLHA Convention Harry N. Seymour Univ of Massachusetts Thomas Roeper Univ of Massachusetts Jill deVilliers Smith College Peter deVilliers Smith College *supported by NIH grant N01-DC-8-2104 *webpage:www.umass.edu/aae African American English: A Definition African American English (AAE) is one of many varieties of English, whose status as a dialect is defined by a commonality of speech spoken primarily by African Americans, but not by all. AAE is less geographically defined than other dialects of English , rather it has emerged as a commonality of speech and grammar of a culturally defined group, though there are differences by geographic regions. Of course, children or adults of other races who have strong cultural identification or primary social interaction with African Americans may speak AAE too. Thus, AAE may be defined in terms of the features that distinguish a pattern of grammar (morphology, semantics, syntax and phonology) in the speech used by culturally identified African Americans. Definition Myths Behind the Controversy • • • • AAE is simply bad or broken English. AAE jeopardizes learning Standard English AAE is political correctness gone amuck. AAE is a cruel self-esteem hoax. Clinical Problem • Standardized tests for children who speak African American English • The deficit/difference dilemma • Too Many African American children fail Misdiagnosis • Over-representation –14.8 % of general population –20.2% of special education • Under-representation –Unclear Clinical Solution • Make the tests harder – Avoid somewhat superficial aspects of language • Contrasts between dialects – Focus on deep principles of language every child should know • Noncontrastive elements between dialects The DELV (Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variation) -- Goals • To develop a comprehensive language assessment of syntax, semantics, pragmatics, and phonology between ages 4 and 9. • To be able to determine whether language variation in children is due to Development, Dialect, Delay, or Disorder. • To create a test that is not biased against dialect speakers, especially African-American English speakers. Collaborators Peggy Speas Angelika Kratzer Barbara Pearson Eliane Ramos Lisa Green Lamya Abdulkarim Toya Wyatt Bart Hollebrandse Mike Dickey Linda Bland Mike Terry Tempe Champion Janice Jackson Laura Wagner D’Jaris Coles Robin Schafer Valerie Johnson Kristen Asplin Tim Bryant Frances Burns The Psychological Corporation Christina Foreman Lisa Selkirk Shelley Velleman Fred Hall Debra Garrett Minjoo Kim Ida Stockman Deanna Moore Joe Pater Caroline Jones Uri Strauss Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variation (DELV) • Variation in speech and language development • Variation in speech and language disorders • Variation in speech and language dialects The DELV Tests • DELV-Screening Test (3/25/03) – Identifies language variation status – Identifies students at risk for a disorder • DELV-Criterion Referenced Test (Spring, 03) – Diagnose speech and language disorders • Syntax, Semantics, Pragmatic, Phonology • DELV-Standardized Version (2005) – Standardized exclusively on AAE children General Results of the DELV Field Testing -- The Subject Sample • • • • • • 1014 four to nine year olds, most of them from working class backgrounds and from all regions of the USA. There were 217 four-year-olds, 266 five-year-olds, 300 six-year-olds, 56 sevenyear-olds, 101 eight-year-olds and 74 nine-year-olds. Approximately 60% of the children were characterized by the testing clinicians as speakers of African American English (AAE), the other 40% as speakers of Mainstream American English (MAE). AAE and MAE children were matched for parental education level. Approximately 1/3rd of the children at each age and in each dialect group were identified by the participating clinicians and schools as having a specific language-impairment and were receiving language services. 10 to 15% of the children spread equally across ages and dialect groups were diagnosed as having phonological or articulation problems. DELV-SCR Structure • The test has a screener version (DELV-SCR) that takes 15 to 20 minutes to administer. • The screener contains morphosyntax and phonology Identifier Items on which AAE-speaking children produce systematically different responses from MAE. • It also contains a set of Diagnostic Items designed to tell the clinician whether further testing is needed because the child is at risk for language delay or impairment. Screener Morphosyntax Identifier Items • Have/has (“The girl have (has) a big kite”) • 3rd person present tense ‘s (“The girl always sleep(s).”) • Doesn’t/don’t (“This girl don’t (doesn’t) like to play basketball.”) • Be copula forms (They was (were) sick”) Screener Phonology Identifier Items • Substitution f/th: bath --->baf • Substitution v/th: breathe --->breav or bread • Zero Cluster Element: gift--->gif Language Variation Status • Mainstream American English (MAE) • Some Variation from MAE • Strong Variation from Mae Performance of the different dialect and impairment status groups on the Identifier Items on the DELV-SCR (Non-mainstream responses). Screener Identifier Items 12 Average Score /16 10 8 IMPAIRED AAE IMPAIRED MAE 6 TYPICAL AAE TYPICAL MAE 4 2 0 4 5 6 7 Age 8 9 Major Theories of SLI • Difficulty with morphosyntax: (Leonard, Rice). • Difficulty repeating nonsense words (Bishop) • Difficulty with variables and embedded clauses (Penner, Roeper & Seymour, van der Lely) Screener Diagnostic Items • Past tense was/were auxiliary and copula forms (obligatory in both MAE and AAE). • Elliptical Possessive pronoun (obligatory in both MAE and AAE). Thsee are for morphosyntax. • Non-word Repetition (for memory problems) • Wh-Question Comprehension (for variables and embedding) Performance of typically developing and language impaired children on the Diagnostic Items on the DELV-SCR (Errors) Screener Diagnostic Items 14 Average Score 12 10 8 IMPAIRED TYPICAL 6 4 2 0 4 5 6 7 Age 8 9 Performance of MAE and AAE speaking children on the Diagnostic Items on the DELV-SCR (Errors) Screener Diagnostic Items 10 9 Average Score 8 7 6 AAE MAE 5 4 3 2 1 0 4 5 6 7 Age 8 9 Is a screener enough? • A screener is just that: it does not diagnose. • A practitioner needs to know more precisely what the child’s areas of difficulties are, for both accurate diagnosis and design of remediation. • The DELV-CR goes deeper, and checks what the results of the screener mean. Characteristics of the DELV-CR The DELV-CR (criterion-referenced test) has 11 subtest components organized into four language domains. • Syntax = Wh-Q comprehension, Passives comprehension, Articles production. • Pragmatics = Wh-Q asking, Communicative Role Taking (production), Narrative. • Semantics = Verb contrast production, Preposition contrast production, Quantifier comprehension, and Syntactic Bootstrapping/Fast mapping. • Phonology Components of The DELV SYNTAX Domain Question Type Core Concepts WH-QUESTION COMPREHENSION Variables Movement PASSIVES Movement Hidden properties ARTICLES Discourse properties (something in a prior sentence making requirements on an element in a subsequent sentence) ARTICLES: TESTING REQUIREMENTS OF DISCOURSE PROPERTIES Does the child carry information from one sentence into another? Ex. A bird flew out of a cage because something was open? What was it? THE door (nor A door) Has the child learned to interpret articles as reference to context? Examples of eliciting questions Part-the: Sally was eating an ice-cream cone when suddenly- slosh! something fell out and she only had the cone left. What was it? (THE icecream) Familiar-the: A cat and a bird were sitting in a tree. They were friends. One of them flew out of the tree. Guess which. (THE bird) Specific-a: I'll bet you have something hanging on the wall of your room at home. What is it? (A picture) Non-referential-a: Tyrone is going to take a nap, and he wants to cuddle with something,. What does he need? (A blanket) Predicational-a: Think of a baseball player. Can you imagine what one looks like? What does he have? (A glove) Wh-factors: Query: what is that Echo: you ate WHAT Exclamative: What nice clothes you have! Indirect question: he knows what to do => not answered Relative clause: the man who you saw Discourse connected: John has 3 hats. Which is best? Core CONCEPT #1 IN SYNTAX on the DELV • • • • • • I. Principles of MOVEMENT Simple: “I saw a boy, a girl, and a dog.” => “What did I see ( - ) ?” Complex: What did she say she saw ( - )? • Does the child get complex movement right? • Core Syntactic Concept #1 on the DELV (con’t) Does the child know… 1. Where the WH word originates • What did he eat ( - )? • When did she say ( - ) she lost her purse (- )? 2. When certain structures "block" certain meanings: Ex. When did she say how she lost her purse? can only mean "when did she SAY it” not “When did she lose it?” Core Syntactic CONCEPT #2 on the DELV • • • • • • • • • • II. VARIABLES (words that are intended to refer not to a single referent, but to members of a set) Examples: Simple Question (1 variable) . (“I saw a boy, a girl, and a dog.”) “What did I see?” “what” = set of objects (boy, girl, dog) “Who was at dinner?” “who” = the 5 or 6 individuals at dinner Core Syntactic CONCEPT #2 on the DELV (con’t) • • • • • II. b. Complex Variables 2 variables in the same sentence: “who bought what?” requires reference to all the members in the 2 sets in an ordered relation: Person 1 bought Thing 1 Person 2 bought Thing 2 • Does the child get variable properties right? Core Syntactic CONCEPT #2 on the DELV (con’t) Does the child know how to answer Double WH-questions: Who ate what? How did she play what? • Requires “set” answers to BOTH questions – (he and she, chocolate and vanilla) • Not just listed, but PAIRED. Ex. HE ate CHOCOLATE, and SHE ate VANILLA. Testing Complex WH-Question Comprehension We test this: 1) Can children answer both parts of a double-WH? 2) Can children answer questions whose site of origin is far away (long distance)? and 3) Can children appropriately block meanings that the grammar doesn’t allow, i.e.when there is a barrier? Wh-Question Comprehension: Testing Procedure • The child is told a brief story about a pictured event. • They are then asked the key test question about some aspect of the event. • The pictured events and stories support several possible interpretations of the question. Typical Answers to double WH questions • PAIRED, EXHAUSTIVE responses – Ex. She played the piano with her hands and the drums with her feet. • SINGLETONS (Incorrect) – One element: “piano” “with her feet” – Both objects, no instruments: “piano and drums” – One pair: “the piano with her hands.” • OTHER – “She played a lot.” “She was playing.” Double WH Response Types by Age and Language Status (N = 1014, 708 Typically Developing, 306 Language Impaired) Fig. 1b. Double WH Response Types 1 Not Paired 0.8 OTHER-LI 0.6 Paired Exhaustive 0.4 Not Paired-LI 0.2 Paired Exhaustive-LI 0 4 5 6 Age Yrs 7 8 9 Typical Answers to “False Clause” questions • LONG DISTANCE (LD) TWO CLAUSE responses – Ex. She said she bought paper towels. • ONE CLAUSE responses (Incorrect) – Ex. (She bought) a birthday cake. • OTHER – “a surprise” “a bag” “I don’t know.” LD False Clause Response Types by Age and Language Status (N = 1014) Fig. 3 WH "False Clause" Response Types by Age and Language Status 1 clauseTD 1 LD 2 clauses-TD 0.8 0.6 OTHER-LI 0.4 0.2 1-clauseTD 0 4 5 6 Age Yrs 7 8 9 LD 2 clauses-LI Long distance movement barriers • We also tested children on long distance movement, and respect for a variety of barrier effects: wh-islands: • How did the girl ask how to ride? • Who did the girl ask what to bring? relative clauses: • How did the boy who sneezed drink the milk? empty operators: • Where did the boy buy the lemonade to splash on his face? Typical Answers toWH-barriers questions • SHORT DISTANCE responses – (How did she learn…?) By watching TV.. • MEDIAL ANSWERS (Incorrect) – (…what to bake?) “a cake” • LONG DISTANCE responses (Incorrect) – (How…..bake?) “With a pudding mix,” “With a spoon” • OTHER – Ex. “She didn’t know how.” WH Barrier Response Types by Age and Language Status (N = 1014) WH "Barriers" Response Types by Age and Language Status 1 0.8 Short Distance-TD Short Distance-LI Medial-TD Medial-LI OTHER-li 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 4 5 6 Age Yrs 7 8 9 Summary of barrier effects • All the barriers were obeyed well even in LI but the rate of errors was higher in LI children. • The most prevalent error was answering the medial, an error type that persists in LI. • No children answer the “who” complementizer in the relative clause, despite superficial equivalence. The Echo-Exhaustive distinction Echo questions differ from ‘real” wh questions in several ways: What did the children eat? The children ate what? Differences • Echo questions ask for the missing constituent, real wh for an exhaustive answer • Echo questions can be substitute for a part of a constituent, real wh cannot: • The boy said he bought a big blue what? • * What did the boy say he bought a big blue t? Previous tests • Mari Takahashi (1991) tested whether 3 year olds respected this distinction and got nice contrasting results: more exhaustive for real wh, more constituents for echo questions. • A student pilot study reported in de Villiers and Roeper 1995 found intonation insignificant for distinguishing the two. Echo/Exhaustive Distinction by age 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 disaae dissae tryaae Ages 11 -1 2 78 6 5 trysae 4 Percentage of newscore "2" Respect ec/ex distinction Wh-Question Asking Elicitation Procedure • The child is shown a picture with something missing from it. • They have to ask the right question to find out what the event is about. • The missing elements of the pictures include objects, people, locations, tools, and causes of emotions -- so what, who, where, how, and why questions are motivated. • Different levels of prompting are given for each trial if the child does not spontaneously ask an appropriate question -- varying from the semantic domain of the question to ask, to the specific wh-word to begin the question with. • If the child asks an appropriate question they are shown the complete picture. Wh-Question production in MAE and AAE speaking children following all prompts. Wh-Question Production 9 8 Average Score /9 7 6 5 AAE MAE 4 3 2 1 0 4 5 6 7 Age 8 9 Wh-Question production in typically developing and language impaired children following all prompts. Wh-Question Production 9 8 Average Score /9 7 6 5 IMPAIRED TYPICAL 4 3 2 1 0 4 5 6 7 Age 8 9 Production of Double Wh-Questions by Typically-developing and Language-impaired Children following all prompts Why is semantics a challenge? • Bias of acquired vocabulary tests: too culturally dependent? • Want to look at process: CAN the child learn a new word easily? • For older children, lexical organization/retrieval may be more significant than size of vocabulary. Three Semantics tests • Novel verb learning/fast mapping - gets at the process of learning a new word • Verb contrasts • Preposition contrasts - these get at lexical organization and contrasts Syntactic Bootstrapping and Fast Mapping of Word Meanings from Context • Children acquire a verb’s meaning in part through the argument frames in which it appears. This phenomenon of fast mapping of meanings from context is often called syntactic bootstrapping. • We test how much children can learn from intransitives, transitives, datives, and complement argument frames. • Nonsense verbs were used in these frames to describe strange actions in ambiguous contexts. The child then answered questions about the verb and its subjects and/or objects. Procedure • The child saw a picture that contained at least two events. S/he heard a sentence about it containing either a REAL or a NOVEL verb. • The child had to answer a set of questions about the picture that are designed to test which action s/he has associated with the verb. Argument structures • Intransitive: one argument E.g. the dog is barking • Transitive: two arguments E.g. The boy poured the drink • Dative: three arguments E.g. The mailman handed the letter to the boy • Complement: three arguments E.g. The policeman asked the woman to stop the car Question types • • • • • • • • ING e.g Which one is zanning? (agent) ER e.g. Which one is the zanner? (agent) Got-ED e.g. Which one got zanned? (patient) ABLE e.g. Which one is zannable? (patient) Subj-comp e.g. Which one did she sug (e) to send the ball? Obj-comp e.g. Which one did she sug the man to send (e)? Verb Contrasts • How do children organize their lexicon for easy retrieval of e.g. contrasts or opposites? • Waxman & Hatch studied noun organization in 3 year olds e.g. plantflowerrose • We decided to focus on verbs because a) less culturally biased b) maybe disordered in SLI (Rice) • Tried to elicit different verbs from the children for the same picture depending on the prompt, to tap versatility and organization of verb lexicon. Procedure • The child sees a picture (of a man crawling out of a building) and is told e.g. “The man is not WALKING, he’s…?” (crawling) • THEN, for the same picture: “The man is not ENTERING the building, he’s…?” (going out) Preposition contrasts • Finally, we decided to tap preposition contrasts in the same way, to see if children could handle the different semantic and grammatical forms they take. • The format is the same as for verb contrasts, i.e. we prompt for two different prepositions per picture. Prompts to picture • “She’s not looking at the radio, she’s listening . . .” (to the radio) • “She’s not lifting the chair, she’s sitting . . .” (under the chair) Development of MAE and AAE speaking children on the Semantics Domain Score DELV-CRT Semantics Subdomain 40 35 Average Score 30 25 AAE MAE 20 15 10 5 0 4 5 6 7 Age 8 9 Development of typically developing and language impaired children on the Semantics Domain Score DELV-CRT Semantics Subdomain 40 35 Average Score 30 25 IMPAIRED TYPICAL 20 15 10 5 0 4 5 6 7 Age 8 9 Double Questions Among the questions we elicited were double wh-questions such as: • Who is eating what? • Or • Which person is eating which food? Singleton answers by age Decline in singletons, subtest 2wh 1-6 Percentage singletons 30 25 disaae 20 dissae 15 tryaae 10 trysae 5 0 4 4.6 5 5.6 6 Ages 6.6 7-8 9- 1110 12 II. General Background: Quantification 1. Problems with quantification (Roeper and DeVilliers (1991) a. A boy saw every fish. He raised his eyebrow. => fish, not boy • How do child ren und erstand Quanti fiers? Every, some, only, all, most, many These words are easily confusing: Some child ren kno w every present each one received from every pa rent? Hidden Ambiguity Some boy had every toy = every toy was possessed by a different boy = one boy had all the toys Mathematics: Does every boy have three shovels? => boy-shovel boy-shovel boy-shovel Answer: "no", but ask a 7yr old and many will say "yes" Do the boys have three shovels? ambiguous: 1. each one has three shovels 2. they have three shovels altogether Cont rol-No, no rmal vs. diso rdered Summ ary: d evelopm ent p attern is th e same, with th e normals outp erforming th e diso rdereds th e whol e way. NB: th e graph sp ans 0 to mo re than 1, so th e differe nces between th e two lin es is not as insigni ficant as it might s eem. E.g. at age 4, th e differe nce is clos e to 20 %. Control-no, Normal vs. Disordered 1.2 1 % Correct 0.8 Normal 0.6 Disordered 0.4 0.2 0 4 5 6 7 8 Age 9 10 11 12 No Quantifier Hypothesis: WhSingletons and Control-No Failure Results on 2 control-no que stions Normal Disorder Overall 0 errors 540 (60.7%) 200 (49.3%) 740 (57.1%) 1 error 128 (14.4) 74 (18.2) 202 (15.6) 2 errors 221 (24.9) 132 (32.5) 353 (27.3) Total 889 406 1295 349/ 889 normal children => 1 or 2 errors: 128 made 1 error, 221 made 2 errors. Out of , 206 /406 disordered children=:> 1 or 2 errors Correlation of Wh-singletons and Control-No Wh-exhaustivity errors: 0 1 2+ 11.2 23.3 22.4 3.9 12.9 17.6 Quantifier errors: 0 1 2 84.9 63.9 59.8 - 35% children who show one Q error, have 1or 2 wh- errors - 40% children who show two Q, errors, have 1or 2 wh-errors Significance • Results are highly significant (statistically): • Chi-squared analysis result: p < 0.001 • (i.e. probability that the 2 effects are independent and only appear to be related by accident is less than 0.1%. Wh without “every”? • Singleton => wh-without "every" • Control-no => no comprehension of every • Conclusion: Children must learn: => whcontains hidden "every" • LI children: fail to recognize this factor Spreading total 100 80 disaae dissae tryaae trysae 60 40 20 0 4 4.7 5 5.7 Ages 6 6.7 7-12 Spreading incidence • Quantifier Spreading: Prominent until a late age for all children • • • • LI children definitely show Q-spreading, but many Normals do as well until a late age. Spreading exists for all, some, every, most And may disappear differentially Key Features of the Pragmatics Assessment Procedures on the DELV-CR • They test the interaction of syntactic and semantic forms with specific pragmatic functions -- assessment of pragmatic skills cannot be divorced from the forms that are needed for those functions of language. • They sample a range of simpler to more complex syntactic forms that serve the same communicative functions. • They assess pragmatic skills that are important for early school success and literacy development. • The materials are all picture-based so they require minimal technology and can be administered and scored by a single clinician interacting with the child. Wh-Question Asking Elicitation Procedure • The child is shown a picture with something missing from it. • They have to ask the right question to find out what the event is about. • The missing elements of the pictures include objects, people, locations, tools, and causes of emotions -- so what, who, where, how, and why questions are motivated. • Different levels of prompting are given for each trial if the child does not spontaneously ask an appropriate question -- varying from the semantic domain of the question to ask, to the specific wh-word to begin the question with. • If the child asks an appropriate question they are shown the complete picture. who where what how what who where why who eats what 5 year old TryAAE WHAT IS THE NURSE FEEDING? WHERE DID SHE GO SWIMMING? WHAT IS THE GIRL MAD ABOUT? HOW IS THE GIRL FIXIN' THAT? WHAT IS THE WOMAN EATING? WHO IS RIDING THE BIKE? WHERE IS THAT BOY GOING? WHAT HAPPENED? WHAT IS THEY EATING? 5 year old DISAAE NR SHE MAKING A POOL. WHAT THE GIRL SHE'S FIXING HIS BIKE WHAT SOME MEAT WHAT A BOY THE BOY IS RUNNING TO THE ICE CREAM WHAT? NR who where what how what who where why who eats what 6 year old TryAAE WHO IS THE NURSE FEEDING? WHERE DID THE GIRL SWIM? WHAT IS THE GIRL MAD FOR? WHAT IS THE GIRL FIXING? WHAT IS THE GIRL EATING? WHO IS RIDING THE BIKE? WHERE IS THE BOY RUNNING? WHY IS THE BOY CRYING? WHAT ARE THE PEOPLE EATING? 6 year old DISAAE WHO IS THAT FEEDING HIM? SHE JUMPED IN THE WATER. SHE MAD AT THE TABLE. SHE IS FIXIN THE TOY. WHO'S EATIN? A BOY RIDIN ON THE BIKE. WHO'S RUNNING? HE DROPPED HIS ICE CREAM. WHO'S EATIN? who where what how what who where why who eats what 8 year-old TRYAAE WHO IS THE NURSE FEEDING? WHERE DID THE GIRL GO SWIMMING? WHY IS THE GIRL MAD? HOW IS THE GIRL FIXING THE TOY? WHAT IS THE WOMAN EATING? WHO IS RIDING THE BIKE? WHAT IS THE BOY RUNNING TO? WHY IS THE BOY CRYING? WHO IS EATING WHAT FOOD? 8 year old DISAAE WHO IS SHE FEEDING? WHAT SOMETHING SHE SWIM IN? WHO IS SHE MAD AT? WHAT'S SHE HOLDING ON HER HAND? WHAT HER MOM EATING FROM HER TWO FINGERS AND HER SOMETHING RIDING A BICYCLE. WHERE IS HIS HOUSE? WAS HE CRYING? HOW WAS THEY WAS EATING? Narratives Narratives have three important components: • Coherence = use of required story grammar components • Cohesion = a. use of linguistic devices to establish, maintain, and specify referents (e.g., articles and pronouns, or referent characterizing expressions) b. expression of causal and temporal links between events in the story. • Adopting different perspectives on the events -- “inside” versus “outside” view -- “landscape of action” versus “landscape of consciousness” (Bruner, 1986). This depends on having a “theory of mind”. Narrative Samples from the DELV-CR • I want my train. I’m gonna hide the train from him. I’m gonna play out of the toy box. I’m gonna find that train. Bring that train. (C: 4;2) • He was looking for the choo choo train because the other boy was playin’. And then… and then he said, “I want that choo choo train back”, and umm… he put it in his toy box. And then he came back to find it and he looked under the bed and it wasn’t there. (SC: 4;9) More Narrative Samples from the DELV-CR • The big boy came into the little boy’s room and took away the little boy’s train. Then he hid it under the boy’s bed where he couldn’t get it. Then the little boy… when he left… he got out his train and put it in the toy box while the big boy was eating. Then the big boy thought about the train and he went under the bed to go see it but it wasn’t there. (A: 6;4) • The little brother was trying to get his toy from the big brother. And the big brother hiding his toy under the bed. When he is eating his sandwich, the little boy go and get it and put it inside of his toy box. When his big brother walk in, he think about the train and he look under his bed for it. (J: 6;3) Development of reference contrast in narratives (contrasting the two main characters) in typically developing MAE and AAE speaking children. Reference Contrast in Spoken Narrative Proportion of Group 1 0.8 0.6 AAE MAE 0.4 0.2 0 4.5 5.5 6.5 8 Age 10 12 Development of reference contrast in narratives (contrasting the two main characters) in typically developing and language impaired children. Reference Contrast in Spoken Narrative Proportion of the Group 1 0.8 0.6 Impaired Typical 0.4 0.2 0 4.5 5.5 6.5 8 Age 10 12 Development of the expression of temporal links between events in the narratives of typically developing MAE and AAE speaking children. Temporal Links in Spoken Narrative Average Score /2 2 1.5 AAE MAE 1 0.5 0 4.5 5.5 6.5 8 Age 10 12 Development of the expression of temporal links between events in the narratives of typically developing and language impaired children. Temporal Links in Spoken Narrative Average Score /2 2 1.5 Impaired Typical 1 0.5 0 4.5 5.5 6.5 8 Age 10 12 Development of “theory of mind” explanations for the character’s mistaken action in the picture narrative (typically developing MAE versus AAE speaking children). Mental State Explanations of Action Average Score /2 2 1.5 AAE MAE 1 0.5 0 4.5 5.5 6.5 8 Age 10 12 Development of “theory of mind” explanations for the character’s mistaken action in the picture narrative (typically developing versus language impaired children). Mental State Explanations of Action Average Score /2 2 1.5 Impaired Typical 1 0.5 0 4.5 5.5 6.5 8 Age 10 12 Communicative Role Taking and Understanding Speech Acts • Children not only need to produce different kind of speech acts at appropriate times (e.g., asking for information, requesting action, rejecting or denying, prohibiting etc.); they also need to understand the circumstances and force of those utterances in other people. • The children were shown pictures in which a person was communicating to another about some object or event that was clearly depicted. They were asked what the characters were telling (reporting an observed event), asking, or saying (prohibiting an action), depending on the scenario. Development of appropriate speech act production in a communicative role taking context (MAE versus AAE speaking children) Communicative Role Taking 4 Average Score /4 3 AAE MAE 2 1 0 4 5 6 7 Age 8 9 Development of appropriate speech act production in a communicative role taking context (typically developing versus language impaired children). Communicative Role Taking 4 Average Score /4 3 IMPAIRED TYPICAL 2 1 0 4 5 6 7 Age 8 9 Phonology Structure – – – – 25 target phonemic Clusters Cluster targets only--two and three elements Initial and medial positions of words only Phonotactic Properties Phonology Format – Sentence repetition – Cartoon illustrations – Carrier Phrase--”I see…” Fig.Phonology 3 Phonology Subtest Subtest by by AgeAge andand Dialect Dialect 30 Fig. 3 Phonology Subtest by Age and Dialect 30 25 Number Correct Number Correct 25 20 15 20 AAE 15 MAE AAE aae 10 MAE sae 5 10 0 4 5 6 5 7 8 9 Age Years 0 4 55 66 77 Age AgeYears Years 88 99 Phonology Subscore by Age and Articulation Status Number C orrect (of 25) 30 25 20 Disart 15 TDart 10 5 0 4 5 6 7 Age Years 8 9 Typical Answers to “False Clause” questions • LONG DISTANCE (LD) TWO CLAUSE responses – Ex. She said she bought paper towels. • ONE CLAUSE responses (Incorrect) – Ex. (She bought) a birthday cake. • OTHER – “a surprise” “a bag” “I don’t know.” WH-False Clause Example Responses from field testing CHILD A (12663) CHILD B (18221) A cake Paper towels 1 clause answer 0 points 2-clause answer (long distance) 1 point Item Type 3 Barrier to Long Distance Movement Note: Children’s ability to give LD answers (without embedded false clause) was tested in piloting and then in the DSLT Tryout testing. 90% of the children ages 4-6 and 95% of the children 7-10 gave at least one Long Distance answer, so for reasons of time, simple Long Distance items do not appear on the DELV. Typical Answers toWH-barriers questions • SHORT DISTANCE responses – (How did she learn…?) By watching TV.. • MEDIAL ANSWERS (Incorrect) – (…what to bake?) “a cake” • LONG DISTANCE responses (Incorrect) – (How…..bake?) “With a pudding mix,” “With a spoon” • OTHER – Ex. “She didn’t know how.” WH-barrier Example Responses 2 Who did she ask what to buy? CHILD A (12663) CHILD B (18221) bologna The grocery store lady Medial Short Distance 0 points 1 point Other WH Example Responses CHILD A (12663) 2 correct barriers, 2 barrier violations 1 other CHILD B (18221) 4 correct barriers 1 medial 2 points (of 5) 4 points (of 5) Total:4 of 14 Total: 12 of 14 Who are these children? CHILD A (12663) 5 years old White Female From South Parents w/ HS education Mainstream English speaker Not receiving speech or language services CHILD B (18221) 4 years old African American boy From “north Central” US Parents w/ HS education “Some difference” from MAE” Not receiving speech or language services Profiles of semantic problems Purpose: discuss some individual response patterns to show the tasks in detail, the kinds of responses to expect as a function of age of the child, and possible disordered status. 1)Fast mapping task • The child saw a picture that contained at least two events. S/he heard a sentence about it containing either a REAL or a NOVEL verb. • The child had to answer a set of questions about the picture that are designed to test which action s/he has associated with the verb. Six AAE-speaking children Which one was the zann er? Which one go t zann ed? Which one was zann able? Which one was zann ing? Which one was the sugg er? Which one d id the g irl sug to send th e ball ? Which one d id the g irl sug the man to send? Which one was sugg ing? Total 4 yr- old 4 yr-old 6 yr o ld 6-yr-old 8 yr-old 8-yr-old TRYAA E DISAAE TRYAA E DISAAE TRYAA E DISAAE 0 0 1 1 1 0 1 0 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 6 1 0 0 1 3 1 1 0 0 6 0 0 0 0 3 0 1 1 1 6 0 0 0 0 2 Summary • This is a demanding task over this age range, but younger children can do some questions easily. In general, transitive is easier than dative, and both are easier than complements. • Children with a language disorder have a hard time fastmapping a new word from the grammar, and make many errors. • The task reveals a problem some children may have picking up new words casually from conversation or text: a process increasingly necessary with schooling. They may need more support and repetition than normally developing children. 2) Verb Contrasts • How do children organize their lexicon for easy retrieval of e.g. contrasts or opposites? • Tried to elicit different verbs from the children for the same picture depending on the prompt, to tap versatility and organization of verb lexicon. Examples • The child sees a picture (of a man crawling out of a building) and is told e.g. “The man is not WALKING, he’s…?” (crawling) • THEN, for the same picture: “The man is not ENTERING the building, he’s…?” (going out) The child is shown a picture of a woman buttoning her coat as she walks out the door. • The child is told, “She’s not taking off her coat, she’s…?” (putting it on) And then: “She not undressing, she’s…? (dressing) Six AAE-speaking children DisAAE 4 SLIP ON THE STEP TryAAE score score STAY R IGHT THERE 0 0 SLIDING DOWN THE STEPS COMING OUT THE BUILDING 0 1 4 PUTTING IT IN THERE 0 PUT HE COAT ON 1 0 DRESSING 1 0 0 CRAWL ING ENTERING OUT THE BUILDING 1 0 0 1 PUTTING ON HER COAT LEAVING OUT THE DOOR 1 0 1 0 CRAWL IN' LEAVING 1 1 0 0 PUTTING ON HER COAT DRESSING 1 1 PUTTIN IT IN THE COAT (P) GOING TO WORK 6 SITTING DOWN TRYING TO DO THAT 6 TRYING TO GO TO WORK FIXING HER COAT 8 CRAWL ING HIS HANDS PUTTING STUFF IN 8 ON IT THE COAT ZIPPING THE COAT Summary • Normally developing children have flexibility in describing a scene, and can find the right “level” to describe it at given the prompt. • Language-disorded children have less flexibility and also don’t hit the right contrast so easily. They use more “all -purpose” or “vague” verbs. • This suggests the verb lexicon is an area of concern for children with language difficulties: both incomplete, and poorly organized into contrasts. Prompts to picture • “She’s not looking at the radio, she’s listening . . .” (to the radio) • “She’s not lifting the chair, she’s sitting . . .” (under the chair) Six AAE-speaking children Disaae 4 TO THE RADIO UNDER THE CHAIR A FORK DOWN ON FLOOR GET THE CAT IN THE NIGHT TIME 6 TO THE RADIO IN THE CHAIR THE FORK THE SIDE THE TV GET THE CAT UP THERE 8 THE RADIO DOWN THE CHAIR FORK NEXT T O IT UP THE CAT IN THE NIGHT Tryaae 1 1 0 0 0 1 TO THE RADIO UNDER THE CHAIR WITH A FORK ON THE FLOOR TO GET THE CAT AT DARK 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 TO THE RADIO UNDER THE CHAIR WITH A FORK IN FRONT OF THE TV HE TRYING TO GET HIS CAT IN THE NIGHT 1 1 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 1 TO THE RADIO UNDER THE CHAIR WITH A FORK ON SIDE OF THE TV BY HIMSELF IN THE NIGHT 1 1 1 0 1 1 Summary • As with the verb contrasts, languagedisordered children show difficulty finding the right preposition. Sometimes they omit one, sometimes they use an odd form e.g. “down the chair” instead of “under the chair”. • Their lexicon of prepositions may be poorly organized too. Overall Summary • The three AAE-speaking children who are normally developing reveal similar strengths across these semantic tasks.They all pass the DELV. • The three AAE-speaking children identified as possibly disordered show marked problems across the semantic tasks. They all fail the DELV. • It is possible that existing tests that look at MAE morphology and at acquired vocabulary normed on MAE would pick out all six as disordered. Communicative Role Taking and Understanding Speech Acts • Children not only need to produce different kind of speech acts at appropriate times (e.g., asking for information, requesting action, rejecting or denying, prohibiting etc.); they also need to understand the circumstances and force of those utterances in other people. • The children were shown pictures in which a person was communicating to another about some object or event that was clearly depicted. They were asked what the characters were telling (reporting an observed event), asking, or saying (prohibiting an action), depending on the scenario. Four-year-old AAE Children TYPICAL AAE TELL ASK ASK SAY HIS BIG SISTER FELLED OFF HER BIKE CAN I PLAY BASEBA LL? CAN I HAVE A PI ECE OF CAKE? YOU DON'T FEED THE DOG. THAT'S HIS OWN FOOD IMPAIRED AAE TELL ASK ASK SAY HER BLEEDIN' HIM CARRY SOME THING HER SAY LOOK AT THE CAKE HE FEEDIN THE DOG Six-year-old AAE Children TYPICAL AAE TELL THAT HIS SISTER GOT HURT ASK CAN HE GO OUTSIDE AND PLAY WITH HIS BROTHER ASK CAN I GET A PIECE OF THAT CAKE ? SAY DON'T GIVE THE DOG NONE OF YOUR FOOD IMPAIRED AAE TELL SHE GOT AN OW IE, A SORE ASK I GOT A BAT AND A GLOVE ASK SHE CAN EAT CAKE YET SAY NO IMPAIRED AAE? TELL THE GI RL SHE HAD FELL AND SHE WAS BLEEDIN' ON HER KNEE ASK CAN HE COME OUTSIDE? ASK CAN SHE HAVE SO ME CAKE? SAY NO Narrative • Uniquely specifying referents -- telling my listener(s) who and what I am referring to. • Linking meaning across referents and events -expression of temporal relationships. • Marking point of view -- appreciating different perspectives on events -- having a “theory of mind”. Four-year-old AAE Children TYPICAL AAE REF TIME PICT5 ToM YES SEQUENCER HE DREA MED THAT HIS TRAIN WAS UNDER THE BED BECAUSE HE WANTED IT. IMPAIRED AAE REF TIME PICT5 ToM NONE NONE THE BOY TAKE THAT FOR HIM. CAUSE HE G OT FIND THE TRAIN Six-year-old AAE Children TYPICAL AAE REF YES TIME ADVERB IAL CLAUSE PICT5 THE BIG BROTHER IS THINKING ABO UT THE T RAIN AND HE GOING BACK TO HIS ROOM ToM HE THINK IT'S THERE IMPAIRED AAE REF NONE TIME SEQUENCER PICT5 THE BOY CAN'T FIND THE CHOO CHOO TRAIN ToM BECAUSE HE CAN'T FIND IT IMPAIRED AAE? REF YES TIME ADVERB IAL CLAUSE PICT5 HE THINK THE T RAINS UNDER THE BED. ToM HIS BIG BROTHER IS LOOKING FOR HIS TRAIN… HE THINK IT'S UNDER THE BED, BUT ITS IN THE TOYB OX. Conclusions • We have shown that the assessment of complex aspects of children’s syntactic development between the ages of 4 and 9 can be carried out in a dialect neutral fashion. • These materials and procedures capture the development of several aspects of language that are vital for success in early schooling and the transition to literacy. • They provide the clinician with a substantial profile of the child language strengths and weaknesses, not just a diagnostic categorization. • As such they provide a much richer evaluation of language variation and its sources that has direct implications for areas and methods of intervention.