Cognitive Neuropsychology • Interface between cognitive psychology and neuroscience at the systems level. – Systems level: • brain “regions” – anatomical (sulci/gyri) to functional (Broca’s area) – areas with consistent neural architecture » Brodmann’s Areas – areas with a “unified” cognitive function » e.g., Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 9/46, superior parts of 45 and 10 ) Brodmann’s areas Functional Regions vs. Brodmann’s Areas Major Sulci and Gyri Orientation and Navigation Cognitive Neuropsychology • Goals (for the field and the course): 1) Test cognitive theories of information processing Primary goal of all cognitive neuropsychologists 2) Map cognitive functions to neural regions Ascribe functions to individual regions • Localization Determine how multiple regions work together as a system • Connectionism Not a primary goal of ultra/radical cognitive neuropsychologists 3) Understand the consequences of brain damage and potential for rehabilitation Primary goal of clinical neuropsychology Cognitive Neuropsychology • Focus: behavior of patients with brain injury • Explain patterns of impaired/intact cognitive performance in brain-injured patients in terms of damage to one or more components of a theory or model of normal cognitive functioning • Draw conclusions about normal, intact cognitive processes from the patterns of impaired/intact capabilities seen in brain-injured patients. Case ‘AC’ • Case AC: 67-year old man, formerly employed as a clerical worker in New South Wales, Australia – Neurological profile: • Recent stroke in territory of left middle cerebral artery – What regions affected? • CT revealed older small lesions in both hemispheres – History of cardiovascular disease Case ‘AC’ – Behavioral profile: • Severe Reading deficit: – Could not match upper to lowercase letters (A: a or e?) – How do we know it is not a vision problem? » Could correctly match uppercase letters (A: A or E) – How do we know it is an acquired and not a developmental reading disability? » Formerly employed as a clerk! • Severe Writing deficit: – – – – Could not write anything to dictation except name & address Could not draw to dictation well (pictures) Could not write the lowercase letter to match an upper case example How do we know he doesn’t have a motor problem? » Could correctly copy exactly what was shown visually (when didn’t have to translate auditory to visual) Case ‘AC’ • Very poor knowledge about properties of animals • • • • • • • • • • • “The Oyster with Four Legs” (Sartori & Job, 1988) MC: How many legs does an oyster have? AC: A few MC: I see. What about an ant? AC: Some MC: A caterpillar? AC: No legs MC: What about a snake? AC: None MC: And a seagull? AC: Four legs? – Performance was at chance on further testing… – Suggests loss of information…perceptual or semantic? Case ‘AC’ – Problems with “animal” information? • Compared performance on animals with legs (e.g., bird, dog) to inanimate objects with legs (e.g., chair, sofa) • Performance is at chance; deficit is not animal specific – Problems with “leg-specific” information? • Question about “tails” yielded chance performance too • Deficit is not leg-specific – Problems with “parts” of an object? • No; Unable to make judgments about overall shapes of objects (round or not) or color (black or white) – Damage to information about “perceptual properties”? • Most likely: semantic knowledge of objects looks intact • Correctly classified animals as dangerous or not, where found, whether used as food, etc. – Damage to information about “visual perceptual properties”? • Yes! Could successfully retrieve information about auditory and olfactory features of objects Case ‘AC’: Case Closed! – AC has selectively damaged ability to use visual perceptual information about objects (storage) – Other abilities are mostly intact – What does this tell us about the normal object processing system? – Complex systems reveal their inner workings more clearly when they are malfunctioning – Relies upon assumption of modularity • When one part of cognitive system is damaged, others stay intact • Building box models… Methods: Considerations • What are the component processes required to perform a given cognitive test? • Issues in choosing patients • • • • • Single case vs. Group Studies Selection on the basis of lesion site or functional deficit Time of test: acute vs. chronic stages Site of injury: focal vs. diffuse Distance effects: disconnection and diaschisis • What is the appropriate control group? • How do you define “normal” performance? Cognitive Neuropsychology and Other Techniques • Strong theories of brain-behavior relationships require converging evidence from complementary techniques: • ERP, EEG, MEG (neuromonitoring) • fMRI (neuroimaging) • Virtual lesions (TMS) • How does this method complement functional neuroimaging? – Best at determining whether region is critical for process • With fMRI, regions other than those critical for process may become activated (e.g., due to feedback connections, strategies) – Can do extensive testing without time limit or methodological constraints • With TMS, “lesion” effects are brief, although “patients” can serve as their own controls Functional Neuroimaging of Patients • Detect regions of residual responsiveness in perilesional area • Detect abnormal responses in distant regions that result from diaschesis (clues about systems…) – Less activation than in normals: region has lost inputs – More activation than in normals: disinhibition (unmasking), compensation • Reveals alternative neuronal mechanisms that can support the same task (degeneracy) – Words can be read either by “whole word route” or by “letter to phoneme conversion” – Objects can be recognized by wholes or by parts • Reveals structural and functional plasticity/recovery