Running head: INTUITION 1 Hi Jayne, Excellent paper! Lots of comments and questions along the way, and some edits to facilitate your development with APA. Gary Galuzzo Intuition Jayne A. Sherman George Mason University INTUITION 2 Abstract Intuition is a way of knowing that has been studied by various researchers in recent years. From cognitive psychologists to neuroscientists, intuition has been a topic of discussion in how it affects our social, economic, professional, and interpersonal relationships. The science involved in our intuitive thoughts is fascinating. From brain research scientists can explain how the left brain is responsible for our conscious, analytic, and rational thoughts, while the right brain is responsible for our unconscious, automatic, and intuitive thoughts. Intuition can and does affect our decision making. In addition, intuition is not free from error. This paper will explore the definition of intuition, its historical roots, implications for research, and, finally, how intuition is a new way of knowing for this writer. INTUITION 3 Intuition as a way of knowing has been defined as immediate cognition, an instinctual feeling that something may or may or may not be right. Some people would argue that intuition is innate in everyone and can be a reliable guide. Others would argue that intuition is a gift and that not everyone has this ability to intuit feelings and emotions or hear that inner voice. Still others would assert that intuition is a powerful conscious influence that can either help us or hinder us in our everyday lives. According to the psychologist David Myers (2002), intuition can be both beneficial, and at times, detrimental. He writes regarding intuition, “Intuitions shape our fears, our first impression, and our relationships” (p. 371). In today’s culture there are numerous books on intuition. Just head to any bookstore or public library and you will find rows and rows of books dealing with this subject. What was once thought of as mystical and mysterious is considered a hot topic in the scientific community. There are a number of articles involving intuition and its methodology and implications in philosophy, psychology, sociology, education, neuroscience, as well as in the medical profession. The phenomenological idea of intuition involves a feeling or awareness that enables us to act in a particular way without being able to articulate in cognitive terms how or why. According to Myers (2002), “What you know, but don’t know you know, affects you more than you know” (p.51). Our intuitions affect us in a number of ways: our professions, relationships, social interactions, basically in our everyday lives. Intuition is a researchable phenomenon that is knowable and indeed not without conceptual problems. Intuition is a fascinating topic that is gaining much attention in the field of science. The roots of intuition as a way of knowing can be traced back through evolution. Myers explains about the amygdala, a pair of emotional control centers in the brain’s primitive core INTUITION 4 allowed our primitive ancestors to flee predators. He states, “The amygdala is a key part of our hard-wired alarm system that enabled our ancestors instinctively to avoid predators and disasters and to know whom to trust” (Myers, 2002, p. 38). Dr. Daniel Capon M.D. agrees that our ancestors could not have survived without intuition. He writes that intuition has always been an important part of human intelligence. “There is no way our human ancestors could have survived without intuition…old Pith could not possibly have survived predators or natural threats as melting of the ice age without intuitive decisions” (Capon, 1993, p. 41). This makes perfect sense when thinking about evolution and survival. Our ancestors must have had some intuitive feelings regarding predators and their ability to survive natural disasters and other dangers. The Greek philosophers, including Aristotle, believed intuition was not obtained through the senses but rather by intelligence. Descartes believed that intuition was a way of knowing through reason and deduction and not by way of the senses. The senses can fool us according to Descartes. Intuition was thought of as being synonymous with cognition unaided by experience. Descartes writes, “By intuition I mean, not the wavering assurance of the senses, or deceitful judgment of a misconstrued imagination, but a conception, formed by unclouded mental attention, so easy and distinct as to leave no room for doubt in regard to the thing we are understanding” (Descartes, 1657/2002, p. 124). Descartes believed that although the senses were necessary in practice they were not essential for knowledge. Following Descartes there were new explanations concerning intuition. The English philosopher John Locke believed that both perception and intellect were two separate intuitive faculties necessary for knowledge. Locke believed that intuitive knowledge was the irresistible and unquestionable perception of the agreement of any two ideas without the INTUITION 5 mediation of any other. He felt that intuitive thinking was the most perfectly certain of all degrees of human knowledge. Intuitive knowledge accounts for our self-evident truths and serves as the foundation for which all other knowledge is accepted (Locke, 2014). The German philosopher Immanuel Kant, widely considered a central figure of modern philosophy, thought sense perception to be the new paradigm for intuition. In his work Critique of Pure Reason, Kant suggests that much of what we consider to be reality is shaped by the perceiving mind. The mind, according to Kant, does not passively receive information provided by the senses. Rather, it actively shapes and makes sense of that information. Kant (1781/2011). The positivist Thomas Kuhn wrote of intuition and creating new paradigms. He believed that in order for new paradigms to be formed new experiences are added to previous ones and a new paradigm is conceived. Intuitions depend on experience gained from the old paradigm according to Kuhn. He writes of paradigms being derived from experiences or “flashes” of intuition. These “flashes” of intuition enable scientists to see a new solution to a previously unsolved puzzle. Kuhn also mentions how at other times such “illuminations” come during sleep (Kuhn, 1996 p. 122-123). He writes about the eventuality of a theory involving perception and the mind. One might wonder how Kuhn would have reacted to the research on intuition and if he felt a new paradigm was possible. The cognitive scientist Jerome Bruner believed that intuitive and analytical thinking should both be encouraged and rewarded. Bruner wrote of metacognition in learning and spoke of intuitive skills being under emphasized and made appeals for experts in all fields of research to make “intuitive” leaps regarding the educational process. Bruner writes, “A long time ago I proposed the concept of the “spiral curriculum,” the idea that in teaching a subject you begin with an “intuitive” account that is well within the reach of the student, and then circle back later INTUITION 6 to a more formal or highly structured account…the learner has mastered the topic” (Bruner, 1996, p. 119). Bruner continues to explain how children can demonstrate intuitively higher level thinking such as physics by simply understanding, for example, how a lever works on a playground seesaw. Today there are many theories of intuition. While researchers may not all agree as to how intuition happens, they do agree that it exists. At one time or another we have all had that unexplained instinctual feeling. Intuition can be viewed as either rational or conscious thought along with unconscious thought or a dual process or a single process not to be separated from other possible ways of knowing depending upon which research one reads. According to the research psychologist David Myers (2002), thinking, memory, perceptions and attitudes operate on two levels. Scientists refer to this as dual processing. The mind, according to Myers, has two components. There is the cognitive or conscious deliberate mind that is analytical and the unconscious mind that operates “off stage” and is automatic and intuitive. Myers states how neuroscientists have identified pathways in the brain that explain why feelings can sometimes occur before thinking. He explains that such a pathway travels from the eye via the thalamus, which is the brain’s sensory control center to the amygdala a pair of emotional control centers in the brain’s core. This eye to the? amygdala path bypassing the cortex enables emotional response before intellect intervenes. Myers writes, “The amygdala sends more neural projections up the cortex than it receives. This makes it easier for our feelings to hijack our thinking than for our thinking to rule our feelings” (p. 37). After the cortex interprets a threat, the thinking or conscious part of the brain takes over. There is another theory of intuition similar to that of the dual processing theory called the “two minds” theory. According to psychologist Johnathan Evans (2010), this theory consists of a INTUITION 7 number of characteristics from the dual processing account. However, Evans believes that each mind has access to different and distinct forms of knowledge together with corresponding learning and memory systems. He refers to these as Type 1 and Type 2 minds. Type 1 mind deals with intuitive and experiential learning consisting from evolutionary ancient mechanisms. Type 2 mind deals with working memory. Evans writes, “I argue also that intuition should be seen as the contrastive of reasoning, corresponding roughly to the distinction between Type 1 (intuitive) and Type 2 (reflective) processes in contemporary dual processes of thinking” (p. 316). Evans believes that intuition is quick and can reflect large amounts of information processing. Intuition can also provide accurate judgments when it is based upon relevant experiences. Yet another theory on intuition is referred to as the cognitive continuum theory. The dual processing theory examines the information process, the cognitive continuum theory deals with the outcome. This is called the “unidimensional” view which suggests that rational and intuitive thinking occur on opposite ends of a continuum. This theoretical perspective expects most information processing to occur as a mixture of both thinking styles referred to as zone of “quasirationality.” According to Marta Sinclair (2010), “Whereas dual system theories examine the process, the cognitive continuum theory deals with the outcome in terms of used style. In other words the former asks along which pathways the processed information travels, whereas the latter is interested in the resulting cognitive style” (p. 379). Sinclair states that it is very important to differentiate between intuiting as unconscious information processing and intuition as its consciously registered outcome. According to the psychologist Kenneth R. Hammond (2010), the concept of intuition needs to be reformulated. Hammond suggests the continuum would have pure analysis as the maximum at one end of a pole and pure intuition at a maximum at the opposite end of a pole. He INTUITION 8 writes, “the greater part of our cognitive activity is quasirational; it is the normal form of cognition-human judgment that is neither purely analytical nor purely intuitive it involves differing proportions of each, depending on which attributes are present in our normal activity” (p. 330). Hammond believes intuition is an in between process on the continuum rather than completely rational or experiential. This quasirational concept of intuition means information processing is to occur as a mixture of both rational and intuitive styles. Daniel Capon, M.D. writes of measuring intuition. He and several colleagues worked for three years to develop a measurement called the Intuition Quotient Test, or the IQ2. According to Capon the quotient refers to the proportion of general intelligence that intuition makes up. Capon (1993) states, “Although a trained scientist and great believer in rational thought, I am convinced that intuition is the older, wiser, and perhaps greater part of human intelligence” (p. 41). It would be interesting to know the outcome if Capon were able to measure the various types of intuitions such as moral, social, mathematical, psychic, etc. with his Quotient Test and if the measurements could predict intuitive behavior in individuals. Steven Hales believes that intuition has not always been thought of in a good way, but rather, “kept hidden from the light of day” (Hales, 2000, p. 135). Hales argues that intuition is a rational process rather than an empirical one. He writes that some philosophers are in denial when it comes to the role of intuition because they are in need of the empirical evidence. Hales continues to discuss intuition with regards to philosophy and concludes with, “appeal to rational intuition is epistemically justified only if a form of foundationalism is true” (p. 135). In his article Hales explores the many arguments concerning the faculty of intuition and its differing views among the positivists, logical positivists, and other philosophers. INTUITION 9 Regardless of the various theories of intuition, one thing is certain, intuition can affect us both positively and negatively. Intuition can inform our decision making in our personal and professional relationships. Intuition can affect our overall everyday lives, both good and bad. According to Nalini Ambady, (2010) people are intuitively able to predict certain outcomes based upon brief or “thin slices” of observations. “Thin slice judgments are impeded by tasks involving deliberation such as reasons analyses tasks. Thus, impressionistic evaluative thin slice judgments seem to be intuitive” (p. 271). Ambady believes that these brief judgments can indeed affect how we feel about people and situations. These “thin slice” observations can be beneficial to our decision making. The implications for researching intuition are many. From neuroscience, psychology, sociology, anthropology, philosophy, physics, education, medical, and even economical, research on intuition continues to inform our decision making. Our intuitions can and do affect our social interactions. Imagine the benefits in the medical profession if scientists discovered an actual intuition gene in the human brain. Since research does show that infants are naturally more intuitive than adults perhaps scientists could discover a way to help people retain that intuitiveness into adulthood (Myers, 2010, p. 18). One must consider the affects in human capabilities, in our professional and our personal lives if scientists could discover how to utilize our intuitions to our advantage. Educational researchers Connelly and Clandinin (as cited in Zembylas 2007, p. 355) studied intuition and student learning. They studied pedagogical content knowledge (PCK) of teachers, including their unconscious and reflective intuitions, and their effects on teaching and student learning. Their research demonstrated the positive effects on student learning when teachers intuitively knew how to execute lessons based upon experiences. If teachers could INTUITION 10 intuitively plan instruction more effectively and teach to their students’ strengths the outcomes for student learning would most likely be positive. Teachers’ intuitive expertise would be of great benefit in helping students learn difficult content. In his research, Anthony Rud examines how both reflective critical reason and intuition linked to expertise are needed in education. He suggests educators tap into both ways of knowing in teaching students (Rud, 1994, p. 65). Research in intuition and education could be used to increase teacher efficacy in planning and pedagogical methods and therefore help to increase student learning and possibly raise test scores. Other research has been conducted on intuitive learning and effective strategies for teaching and learning. For example, knowing how students learn will provide insight for the teacher’s instructional planning. According to the research by Richard M. Felder, a professor at North Carolina State University, intuitive learners often prefer discovering possibilities and relationships. His research demonstrates that intuitive students like innovation and dislike repetition. In addition, intuitive learners may be better at grasping new concepts as well. Felder provides insight into the concept of inquiry and student learning. He believes students are creative learners who learn best through discovery. Felder writes about the differences between sensory learners and intuitive learners and that everyone is a little of both a different times. He suggests differentiation of instruction consisting of various strategies and innovated activities that would promote intuitive thinking in students to improve student learning (Felder, 2000). Research in intuition and mathematics demonstrates a need for educational programs that emphasize conceptual development over procedural. Symbolic intuition or the intuitive understanding of mathematical symbols develops as a result of experience with formal and abstract school-based procedures. As stated by Talia Ben-Zeev (2002), of Brown University, “by INTUITION 11 examining the nature of intuitive mathematics we could help (a) improve our understanding of people’s formal- and informal reasoning skills and (b) create more effective instructional materials” (p.1). Perhaps this could help teachers to plan differentiated math instruction for students to help increase their understanding of mathematical concepts and improve their reasoning skills. Yale psychologist Robert Sternberg found that our intuition is a result of tacit or implicit knowledge learned by experience and without intention. Tacit knowledge is procedural. Sternberg studied sales people, military officers, teachers, executives, and company managers to discover their intuitive abilities in decision making. His findings demonstrated that success depends less on academic knowledge and more on intuitive or implicit knowledge (as cited in Myers, 2002, p. 56). Perhaps this helps to explain why some individuals just know “intuitively” how to run a business, play the stock market, or win athletic completions. Sternberg writes that some people just intuitively know how to do certain things with expertise and instinct. Imagine the impact on society if one could train people to tap into their business intuition more proficiently. Intuition has also been studied with counselors, nurses, police officers and clinicians. The need for quick insight without analytic thinking is often necessary in these professions. For instance, research on intuition and nursing demonstrates intuition and its emphasis on five features: rapid perception, lack of awareness of the processes engaged, presence of emotions, holistic understanding of the situation, and overall good quality of the proposed solutions (Gobet & Chassy, 2008). Research on intuition could provide evidence of the intuitive traits and necessary attributes to allow people to function even more proficiently in their jobs. INTUITION 12 There is even research on intuition as a woman’s way of knowing. Some men would argue that they are just as nurturing as and more sensitive than other men and women and therefore just as intuitive (Myers, 2013). But in general there does appear to be a gender gap with intuition and the sexes. Perhaps research could investigate intuition in men and women with respect to professional responsibilities, attitudes, and performance. Research might also indicate if there is a difference between heterosexuals and homosexuals with regards to intuition and if so how can we use this to our advantage in professional and interpersonal relationships. Research might even allow us to understand at what age we are the most intuitive and how can we tap into this intuitive knowledge to better ourselves and society. For instance, perhaps scientists could discover that different types of intuition are more prominent in certain individuals and are more suited for various subjects such as science, math, language and fine arts. Perhaps research on intuition could help find cures for certain disabilities. Experiments have been conducted with split brain surgery patients regarding the right and left brain functioning. According to this research the right brain is superior to the left brain in sensing or intuiting. The left brain is analytical and rational while the right brain is automatic, nonverbal, and intuitive (Myers, 2010). Perhaps more research on the unconscious automatic functioning intuitive right brain could lead to new discoveries and cures for diseases and illnesses from attention deficit to memory loss and Alzheimer’s. What needs to follow is research on how to use our intuitions more effectively in our everyday situations. We need to understand how to decipher between our gut level intuition and our conscious rational thinking. Obviously intuitions are not always accurate. Our intuitions can steer us wrong in our financial, professional, and personal relationships. I am certain many INTUITION 13 people can reflect on the times intuition has been wrong. Sometimes we do become over confident in our abilities and intuition fools us. Perhaps case studies involving the frequency in accuracy of intuitions would be interesting. One could argue Descartes was right in his discourse on how the senses fool us. Perhaps errs in intuition are merely perceptual tricks. The question remains, can research be conducted to help us understand our intuitions in making wiser decisions? If it is true that everyone is born with intuitive powers one must wonder if science can find a way to help us become more intuitive. Perhaps it is possible that scientists can isolate our intuitive right brains to strengthen our intuition in order to help us advance in our professional and personal lives. It appears then when all is said and done we may respect our intuitions, our gut feelings, however, in the end rational critical thinking will most likely take precedence. After all, how many of us are really ready to take a gamble with intuition if it will affect our loved ones, our professions, and our own well-being? With that being said I hope scientists are asking the next best question with regards to intuition and will continue to research this fascinating phenomenon. Intuition as a way of knowing proved to be very enlightening for me. I never knew there were so many different intuitions such as social, spiritual, moral, sports, and psychic to name a few. I learned the functions of the right and left hemispheres of the brain and how the right brain is responsible for our intuitive thoughts. I always thought intuition was just a feeling, an instinct but never really thought about the biological and cognitive aspects of it. I also never thought of intuition as a subject of scientific study or the implications for its study. In researching intuition I realized how the method of inquiry, as with Descartes, creates the need to know more about what fascinates us and drives scientists to ask that next best question. In the meantime we must INTUITION 14 remember to utilize our intuitions in such a way as to make good decisions in our lives, while at the same time not be fooled by it. For me, I need that certainty. I have never been one to fly by the seat of my pants. I like structure and routine, and the ability to know something is for real. I need procedures and reasons as to why I am doing something. I need certainty. I am the person who drives people crazy when I ask the question “why” over and over. I need proof. I prefer to read research and then reflect upon what I read and to analyze information. Even though I have had intuitive feelings on occasion, I would most likely use my rational analytical thinking as opposed to acting upon my gut feelings. I need hard evidence before I jump to any conclusions or make any important decisions. Yes, I am in need of the physical evidence over the instinctual more often than not. Perhaps I am more like Thomas Kuhn at school during data presentations when the Standards of Learning objectives are isolated and scrutinized. Yet, I am a fan of Jerome Bruner as I like his discourse on culture and education. Still…I can be somewhat Cartesian as I can be very rational at times as well. However, I can say this without ambiguity; I enjoyed studying intuition as a new way of knowing for me. I understand its implications for research and look forward to reading more on this way of knowing. However, being a little more cautious, I believe I will stick with my need for certainty as my way of knowing, at least for the time being. Nicely done, Jayne. You gave intuition a logical-positivist review and at the end confirmed that you remain one. As my final comment says, if you’ve widened your scope, then my goal is met. Who knows, you may find yourself relying on your own intuition as a teacher a bit more. Also, you did a really nice job with APA for a first-time paper. I made some model INTUITION edits below. You did so many things correctly, but the capitalization of titles, as noted below, needs some work. A 15 INTUITION 16 References Ambady, N. (2010). The perils of pondering: Intuition and thin slice judgments. Psychological Inquiry, 21(4), 271-278. Ben-Zeev T. (2002). Intuitive mathematics: Theoretical and educational implications. Retrieved from http//www.gseacademic.harvard.edu/~starjo/papers/intuition.pdf Bruner, J. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Capon, M. D., D. (1993). The anatomy of intuition. Psychology Today, 26(3), 41-46, 86-91. Connelly, M. F., & Clandinin J. D. (1985). Emotional Ecology: The Intersection of Emotional Knowledge and Pedagogical content Knowledge in Teaching. Teaching and Teaching Education, 23, 355-367. (as cited in Zembylas, 2007, p. 355). Descartes, R. (1657/1999). Discourse on Method and Related Writings. New York, NY: Penguin Books. Evans, J. B. (2010). Intuition and Reasoning: A Dual-Process Perspective. Psychology Press, 21, 313-326. Felder, R. M. (2005). Learning Style and Strategies. Retrieved from http://www4.ncsu.edu/unity/lockers/users/f/felder/public/RMF.html Gobet, F., & Chassy P. (2008). Towards an Alternative to Benner’s Theory of Expert Intuition in Nursing: A Discussion Paper. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 45, 129–139. Hales, S. (2012). The Faculty of Intuition, Analytic Philosophy, 53, 180-207. Hammond, K. R. (2010). Intuition, No...Quasirationality, Yes! Psychology Press, 21, 327-337. Kant, I. (1781/2011). The Critique of Pure Reason. ( J.M. D. Meiklejohn, trans.) Seattle, WA: Pacific Publishing Co. Kuhn, T. (1962). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions (3rd ed.). Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. INTUITION 17 Locke, T. (2014). Title? Retrieved from http://www.iep.utm.edu/locke/ John Locke Myers, D. (2002). Intuition Its Powers and Perils. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Myers, D. G. (2010). Intuition’s Powers and Perils, Psychological Inquiry, 21, 371-377. Rud, A. G. (1994). Intuition and the Socratic Method: Two Opposed Ways of Knowing? Studies in Philosophy and Education, 13, 65-75. Sinclair, M. (2010). Misconceptions about intuition. Psychological Inquiry, 28(21), 378-386. Sternberg, R. (2002) Intuition Its Powers and Perils, New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.