Peter de Villiers
(Smith College)
Frances Burns
(University of Massachusetts, Amherst and Vanderbilt University)
Supported by NIH grant N01-DC-8-2104
* web page: www.umass.edu/AAE
Jill de Villiers Smith College
Elizabeth Engen Rhode Island School for the Deaf
Debbie Topal Rhode Island School for the Deaf
Harry Seymour University of Massachusetts
Barbara Pearson University of Massachusetts
Tempe Champion University of South Florida
Essential for continuity of personal memory, encoding of experiences, and social and cultural connection.
A major prerequisite language skill for adequate reading and writing development (Snow et al,
1998)
A test of the productive application of syntactic and semantic skills in functional communicative contexts.
A primary early form of extended discourse/ taking a sustained turn = decontextualized language with more complex syntactic forms..
1. What makes for a “well-formed narrative”?
Thematic coherence on the macro-level of plot and episode organization.
Linguistic cohesion or connectivity at the microlevel of noun phrases and clauses and their interrelationships across the discourse.
Appropriate elaboration of the different points of view of the characters.
2. What specific features of these properties of a well-formed narrative can be easily scored and will translate directly into intervention?
Plot/Episode Structure = “the landscape of action”
(Bruner, 1986)
Setting/Introduction + Episode(s) + Resolution/Coda
Episode Structure:
Onset/Initiation -- introducing the problem, goal, or event that initiates and motivates the action in an episode of the story.
Unfolding/Elaboration/Action Attempts -- development of the action of the protagonists in terms of actions and attempts to solve the problem or reach the goal.
Consequences -- immediate effects of each of these actions.
Resolution -- the outcome of these endeavors.
Referential Cohesion -- introducing, maintaining reference to, and contrasting the characters (or objects) in the story (Karmiloff-Smith, 1981).
Temporal and Causal Connectivity -- clearly marking the time and causal relationships between events (Berman & Slobin, 1994).
Foregrounding and Backgrounding -- placing the unfolding plotline events (the foreground) in the context of attendant circumstances in which they take place (Perrera, 1986).
“the landscape of consciousness” (Bruner,
1986) -- talking about the mental states of the characters -- their emotional reactions, desires, and thoughts, and what they do and don’t know as events take place.
Referential Cohesion -- articles “a” and “the”, pronouns, names, adjectives, descriptive prepositional phrases, relative clauses.
Temporal and Causal Connectivity -- adverbs, adverbial phrases, adverbial clauses.
Foregrounding and Backgrounding -- adverbial clauses of time and place, often at the beginning of sentences.
Point of View / Evaluative Commentary -- mental state words and complement clauses.
Open-ended stories from a topic prompt
Familiar “scripted” events (e.g., a birthday party)
Story retelling
Picture or video sequences -- long or short
How well does the elicitation technique get the child to produce language that incorporates the narrative features we have outlined AND can be easily evaluated and scored for those features?
For a more complete evaluation use more than one type.
Case Study 1: Coherence and Cohesion in the Written English Narratives of Deaf
Students
Oral subjects:
63 eight to sixteen year olds, mean age 11;10.
Average hearing loss 95dB (range 70 to 120).
Hearing loss onset prior to 18 months.
Total Communication subjects:
56 eight to sixteen year olds, mean age 12;3.
14 with deaf parents (DoD), 42 with hearing parents (DoH)
Average hearing loss 99dB (range 70 to 120).
Hearing loss onset prior to 18 months.
One multi-episode narrative based on a wordless children’s story -- “The Pirate Story.”
This was a multi-episode story chosen because it had three clear episodes, each of which depicted an initiating event or problem, an action or attempt to deal with that event, and a resolution or consequence of the action sequence.
The story was presented twice in the form of 16 color slides. Then the students wrote the story from memory.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
QuickTime™ and a
Photo - JPEG decompressor are needed to see this picture.
Pirate Story sample -- Age = 9;6
Hearing Loss 98dB, Reading Grade 1.6
The man carrying the boat.
The man go the water.
I ride the boat. The man fighting to the boat. The man shot the gun.
The monster chseed the man.
The monster shot the gun.
The man take the monyey.
The man take bringing the boat.
The man to boat sount.
The man think. The man fixing the boat.
Pirate Story sample -- Age = 12;5
Hearing Loss 93dB, Reading Grade 3;4
The men and women carrying a big ship. They threw ship on the water. One ship is good, other is bad. The ship want to go to Skull and cross bones ship. The ship shoot connon Skull and cross bones ship. The skull and crossbones ship was under the water. The ship the winner. The monster want the ship, but he didn’t. The ship shoot arrow to moster. Monster was dead. Ship are going to look island.
The men climb down get golds. They put in ship. The golds was hevey in ship. The ship fell in the water. Everybody swimming off the water. Everybody sat the island. They cut the tree. They fell the tree. Everybody going to made a new ship. The everybody o.k.
The End.
Pirate Story sample -- Age = 13;3
Hearing Loss 93dB, Reading Grade 5;0
There is a group of men that made a ship. They dicide to find a treasure. That day they were on the boat and travel until there was another boat . They had a war. The other ship lost and the pirate ship won. The pirate ship went to find the treasure. The Monster that was in the water heard the war and blew fire to the ship. One of the man killed the Monster. They went to find the treasure and when they got to the sandy island with a trap door , the men went down and took the treasure and left. But it was too heavy and the ship sank. The men swim to the island and live and made the ship forever.
The End
Episode
Structure
SAT Grade
<2.0
SAT Grade
2.0 to 2.9
SAT Grade
3.0 to 4.9
SAT Grade
>5.0
All
Incomplete
Partially
Complete
(62.5)
62.5
(25)
18.8
(5)
30
(25)
10
(22.2)
Mostly
Complete
All
Complete
(12.5)
6.3
12.5
(45)
25
(25)
30
(22.2)
87.5
(55.6)
12.5
(5.9)
9.1
(94.1)
90.9
( ) = oral students
Two short narratives based on picture sequence scenarios.
These were designed to motivate the need to identify the characters in a contrastive way, to express temporal and causal relationships between events, and to refer to their mental states in explaining their actions.
They were written with the picture sequence in front of the students all the time.
Candy Stealing Story sample -- Age = 9;6
Hearing Loss 98dB, Reading Grade 1.6
He want to the candy
The girl gave to the a penny
The girl gave to the cookies
The girl don’t went the cookies
The girl dreaming police
The girl gave penny
The woman said thank you
Candy Stealing Story sample -- Age = 12;5
Hearing Loss 93dB, Reading Grade 3;4
Kerian Steal Candy Bag
Jane and Kerian went to the store. Jane like to buy some jelly bean in jar. Kerian saw candy in the shelf. Kerian want to steal some candy in the shelf . Then take candy and put in her purse. Kerian told Jane her, you want some candy , I steal candy bag in the shelf. Jane said no thank, because she learn in school . Kerian went to sleep. She dream about policeman take Kerian go to jail . Then went into the store.
Kerian pay for Mrs. Williams. She said I’m sorry I steal candy bag in the shelf. Mrs. Williams said, that o.k. you won’t go to jail. Kerian feel O.K.
Candy Stealing Story sample -- Age = 13;3
Hearing Loss 93dB, Reading Grade 5;0
There two girls in the store and one girl with a pocket purse. The girl was looking at the store lady and was stealing some candy on the counter, then they left.
Outside the girl ask the girl who was buying the candy , and she didn’t wanted it . That night the girl had a bad dream about going to jail and the police took her. The next morning she went to the store and paid for the candy, and then the store lady was happy and pat her on the head! The girl went home happily !!
Pronoun
Usage
SAT
Grade
<2.0
No
Contrast
Mostly
Incorrect
Mostly
Correct
All
Correct
(50)
56.3
(50)
31.3
12.5
SAT
Grade
2.0 to
2.9
(5)
40
(30)
30
(55)
25
(10)
5
SAT
Grade
3.0 to
4.9
SAT
Grade
>5.0
(27.8)
33.3
(50)
33.3
(22.2)
33.3
(17.6)
18.2
(82.3)
81.9
( ) = oral students
Reference
Specification
None
SAT
Grade
<2.0
SAT Grade
2.0 to 2.9
SAT Grade
3.0 to 4.9
SAT Grade
>5.0
Names/
“other”
Adjectives
Preposition
Phrase
Relative
Clause
(50)
6.3
(12.5)
43.8
(37.5)
50
(45)
30
(55)
40
5
25
( ) = oral students
(33.3)
33.3
(5.6)
22.2
(16.7)
0
(44.4)
44.4
(23.5)
9.1
(29.4)
9.1
(47.1)
81.8
Temporal
Cohesion
None
And/then
Adverbial
Phrase
Adverbial
Clause
Final
Adverbial
Clause
Initial
SAT Grade
<2.0
SAT Grade
2.0 to 2.9
SAT Grade
3.0 to 4.9
SAT Grade
>5.0
(37.5)
12.5
(50)
68.8
(12.5)
12.5
(35)
50
(50)
30
(5)
5
(27.8)
(44.4)
66.7
(5.6)
11.1
(17.6)
(11.8)
(10)
15
(22.2)
22.2
(70.6)
100.0
( ) = oral students
Partial Correlations between Reading
Comprehension level and Features of Written
Narrative (controlling for Age and Hearing Loss)
Group
Episode
Structure
Temporal
Cohesion
Pronoun
Usage
Reference
Specification
Oral
(df=56)
.55*** .61*** .67*** .54***
TC
(df=46)
.43** .55*** .58***
** p<.01 *** p<.001
.45**
Case Study 2: African-American
English and Mainstream American
English Children
Frances Burns
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
Only a few studies on narrative development have focused on children who speak a dialect other than mainstream American English (MAE).
Of these studies, an even smaller number have focused on the discourse skills of young children who speak African American English (AAE)
(Champion,1998; 2003).
Previous studies of young AAE speakers have focused on their overall narrative structures and the content of their stories (Champion 2003).
Michaels (1981), described the structure of African
American children’s narratives as complex but different from those of middle class European American children.
The narratives of the majority of African American children were seen as topic-associating (TA) rather than the topic-centered, linear style that dominates early schooling.
Topic associating refers to a narrative style in which the
“main topic is not explicitly stated but implied via a number of loosely connected episodes.”
Topic-centered refers to “a linear progression of information with explicit lexical temporal grounding and no significant shifts in temporal-spatial perspective .”
Topic-centered Topic-associating
Organized around a single topic or closely related topics.
Main characters and temporal/locational grounding remain constant and are lexically explicit.
Clear thematic progression with beginning, middle and end.
Organized around loosely linked topics with implied
(associative) connections.
Frequent shifts in key characters and temporal/locational grounding.
Does not adhere to a linear pattern of organization.
1. I live on lyme street
2. it’s a nice place
3. I got a- my auntie lives up there
4. I was gonna go to my- another school
5. this year I’m going to a different new school
6. so I might be happy there
7. but about my house
8. I just love being at my house
9. my cousins come over to play with me
10. an sleep over sometimes
11. sometimes I have slumber parties
12. great!
13. an den in the morning sometimes my mother takes us- my grandpa take us to the park
14. get us mcdonald’s or ummm all of that
15. sometimes he take us to the zoo
16. an see all the animals
17. it was fun at the zoo
18. I saw the animals, bears
19. it was great!
1. one day I was going over aunt’s house.
2. then me and my cousin Jenea, we wanted to go to the liberry.
3.then we got there and I was reading books.
4. and then I wanted to um go on computers.
5. so I signed up.
6. but then we…which.
7.uh then a magic show was um startin to come on.
8. then this guy, he was just, he didn’t know where his magic hat was.
9. so he made a hat with big balloons like clowns.
10. and then after he made a hat he made um the duck out of balloons.
11. um it was like that duck that’s on Michael Jordan.
12. he made that of balloons.
13. an then he, he had helpers.
14. but he didn’t pick me.
15. an then he, whoever go, whoever did the job he gave them a wand.
16. an then when the magic show was done we, they had snacks.
17. they had cracker fishes, cookies and juice.
18. then I wanted to go a computer.
19 but I forgot that I had to go on the computer.
20. then we leff.
Hyon and Sulzby (1994), looked at the narrative styles of 48 African American low-income urban kindergarteners.
58.3% of the narratives were topic centered.
33.3% were topic associating.
Champion (1998), found that African American children, ages 6-10, produced a variety of narrative structures including the “classic” narrative structure.
In fact 66% of the narratives were classified as classic or topic-centered .
As defined by Labov (1972), these narratives included an orientation, a complicating action, and a resolution, and then concluded with a coda.
Only 11% were classified as “performatives” or topic associating .
21 typically developing African American children from the
Northeast aged 5;9 to 11;6 (Mean age 8;2).
The participants were video-taped telling at least three open-ended stories to one adult African American listener.
The example topics (i.e., hurt, fieldtrip, a hero, vacation) were provided in order to prompt non-fictive narratives.
Fictitious narratives were discouraged because children may be tempted to tell fairytale or story book narratives that are limited in
AAE features and perhaps bias the children toward topic-centered narratives.
A total of sixty-six narratives were transcribed and analyzed for AAE dialect features, T-Units, and narrative style (topic centered vs topic associating).
The children were placed on a dialect continuum that ranged from low to high use of AAE on the basis of the frequency of appearance of several distinctive syntactic, semantic, and phonological features of AAE (Washington
& Craig, 1998) in their spontaneous speech.
Only 11% of the open-ended narratives were categorized as topic-associating, confirming
Champion’s (1998) findings.
There was no relationship between depth of AAE dialect and the likelihood that the children would produce topic-associating forms of narration.
There was a trend in the data for the younger children to produce more topic-associating narratives.
The younger children also produced a higher percentage of “series of events” stories. In these there were clear topics but no orientation, complicating action or resolution.
1.once upon a time I saw my friends at the beach.
2. it was ??? and Carmen and Carmen’s friend.
3. den we went an we ate there.
4. there were little pointers on the floor.
5. only in some ???
6. an I- den I we lef
7. den I rode my bike
8. den the end
9. oh, den I went in the house
10. den I got somin to drink
11. den I ate
12. den I went to bed
These results and those of Champion (1998) suggest that by the time African American children are aged 7 or 8, they have a range of narrative styles available to them.
By this age they predominantly produce the topiccentered, classic narrative (Labov, 1972).
This may result from code switching into the style that they are exposed to in school.
Younger African American children produce more of the topic-associating and “series of event” types of stories.
More research is needed to explore whether the topicassociating narrative style is dominant in still younger children, less than age 6.
78 AAE (n=53) and MAE (n=25) speaking children.
Ages 4 to 6. No difference between the AAE and MAE groups in age distribution or mean age.
One picture sequence narrative from the Diagnostic
Evaluation of Language Variation-Criterion Referenced
(DELV-CR), San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
Corporation (2003)
Assessing Reference Contrasting, Temporal Links,
Mental State References, and Theory of Mind .
Narratives were audio-taped and transcribed.
SEE THE DELV-CR FOR STIMULUS PICTURES
I want my train. I ’ m gonna hide the train from him. I ’ m gonna play out of the toy box. I ’ m gonna find that train.
Bring that train. (C: 4;2, MAE)
He was looking for the choo choo train because the other boy was playin ’ . And then … and then he said, “ I want that choo choo train back ” , and umm … he put it in his toy box. And then he came back to find it and he looked under the bed and it wasn ’ t there. (SC: 4;9, MAE)
The big boy came into the little boy ’ s room and took away the little boy ’ s train. Then he hid it under the boy ’ s bed where he couldn ’ t get it. Then the little boy … when he left … he got out his train and put it in the toy box while the big boy was eating. Then the big boy thought about the train and he went under the bed to go see it but it wasn ’ t there.
(A: 6;4, MAE)
The little brother was trying to get his toy from the big brother. And the big brother hiding his toy under the bed. When he is eating his sandwich, the little boy go and get it and put it inside of his toy box.
When his big brother walk in, he think about the train and he look under his bed for it. (J: 6;3, AAE)
For reference contrasting , the children were given 1 point if they contrasted the two boys in some way in their story
(e.g., “the big brother” vs “the little boy”)
For temporal links the children were given a score based on the most sophisticated type of temporal expression they used: 0 = no time links expressed, 1 = only sequencers like “then” or “and then” used, 2 = adverbial clauses of time used (e.g., “while” or “after”).
For mental state references in describing the thought balloon picture in the eliciting sequence, the children received 1 point if they referred to the intention or desire of the boy (“He wants his train.”), but 2 points if they referred to his cognitive state (“He is thinking about his train.”)
For their answers to the final question about why the boy was looking under the bed for the train, the children again received 1 point for an answer in terms of his motivation for looking (“to find his train.”), but 2 points for a theory of mind explanation (“because he thinks his train is there.”)
So the total score on the narrative was 7 points.
For reference contrasting there was a significant age effect (p=.018), but no effect of dialect, and no interaction between age and dialect.
For temporal expressions there was a significant age effect (p=.003), but no effect of dialect, and no interaction between age and dialect.
For mental state references in Picture 5 descriptions, there was a significant age effect
(p=.015), but no effect of dialect, and no interaction between age and dialect.
For theory of mind explanations there was a significant age effect (p=.003), but no effect of dialect, and no interaction between age and dialect.
5
4
3
2
1
7
6
0
Narrative Score
AAE
MAE
4 5
Age
6
Narratives elicited by the train story sequence.
On-line scoring by the clinicians administering the test (a reliability check showed 87.5% agreement with audio-taped and transcribed narratives)
1014 four to nine year olds from all around the
USA.
60% of them speakers of AAE, 40% speakers of
MAE.
30% of each group (roughly equally spread across the ages) were diagnosed as being language-impaired and were receiving intervention services.
AAE and MAE groups were matched for parent education level (average level = high school).
No differences were found between the dialect groups on any of the separate measures: reference contrasting, temporal expressions, mental state references, or theory of mind.
BUT there were strong developmental growth effects for each of the measures.
AND there were clear differences between the typically-developing children and the languageimpaired children on each of the measures.
Development of reference contrast in narratives
(contrasting the two main characters) in typically developing MAE and AAE speaking children.
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
1
0.8
4.5
Reference Contrast in Spoken Narrative
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
AAE
MAE
Development of reference contrast in narratives
(contrasting the two main characters) in typically developing and language impaired children.
1
0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
4.5
Reference Contrast in Spoken Narrative
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
Impaired
Typical
Development of the expression of temporal links between events in the narratives of typically developing MAE and
AAE speaking children.
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
4.5
Temporal Links in Spoken Narrative
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
AAE
MAE
Development of the expression of temporal links between events in the narratives of typically developing and language impaired children.
Temporal Links in Spoken Narrative
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
4.5
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
Impaired
Typical
Development of mental state references to describe the “thought balloon” picture (typically developing
MAE versus AAE speaking children)
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
4.5
Mental State Descriptions of Picture 5
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
AAE
MAE
Development of mental state references to describe the “thought balloon” picture (typically developing versus language impaired children).
Mental State Descriptions of Picture 5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
4.5
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
Impaired
Typical
Development of “theory of mind” explanations for the character’s mistaken action in the picture narrative
(typically developing MAE versus AAE speaking children).
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
4.5
Mental State Explanations of Action
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
AAE
MAE
Development of “theory of mind” explanations for the character’s mistaken action in the picture narrative
(typically developing versus language impaired children).
Mental State Explanations of Action
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
4.5
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
Impaired
Typical
Development of combined narrative skills in MAE and AAE speaking children aged 4 to 12.
Spoken Narrative Score
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
4.5
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
AAE
MAE
Overall narrative scores in typically developing and language impaired children aged 4 though 12.
Spoken Narrative Score
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
4.5
5.5
6.5
Age
8 10 12
Impaired
Typical
The last two studies demonstrate that these picture sequences produce a dialect neutral assessment of important features of narrative cohesion and point of view.
The materials can be used for diagnosis of language impairment in both Mainstream
American English speaking children and
African American English speaking children.