Lecture 14: Selection Experiments & Experimental Evolution Irschick, D. J., and D. Reznick. 2009. Field experiments, introductions, and experimental evolution: a review and practical guide. Pages 173-193 in Experimental Evolution: Concepts, Methods, and Applications of Selection Experiments. T. Garland, Jr. and M. R. Rose, eds. University of California Press, Berkeley, California. 1 4 Ways to Study Physiological Evolution 1. Phylogenetic Comparisons of Species (or populations) Shows what has happened in past evolution 2. Biology of Natural Populations: extent of individual variation (repeatability) heritability and genetic correlations natural and sexual selection field manipulations and introductions Shows present evolution in action 3. Selection Experiments Shows, experimentally, what might happen during future evolution 4. Compare Real Organisms with Theoretical Models Shows how close selection can get to producing optimal solutions 2 Selection Experiments: 1. The earliest form of “genetic engineering” 2. An experimental way to study “evolution in action” 3. A way to produce “useful” organisms 4. The most direct and convincing test of whether a trait shows any additive genetic variance in the population (narrow-sense heritability) 5. A modern corollary to the August Krogh Principle. If a suitable model does not exist, then create one! Bennett, A. F. 2003. Experimental evolution and the Krogh Principle: generating biological novelty for functional and genetic analyses. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 76:1-11. 3 Selection Experiments: 6. A way to probe the interrelations among traits (correlated responses indicate genetic correlations) 7. A way to test hypotheses about trade-offs and constraints 8. A way to help find the genes that underlie phenotypic variation. Crossing a selected population with a non-selected or oppositelyselected population facilitates genetic mapping. 4 Selection Experiments: 9. A powerful way to demonstrate mechanism, i.e., how organisms work: a. Select on an organismal trait b. Observe correlated response in lowerlevel trait that you hypothesize causes the organismal difference c. Test that hypothesis by performing a second experiment, selecting on the lower-level trait d. Does the organismal trait change as predicted? 5 Hypothetical Example 1 a. Select for long life span in mice b. Observe correlated increase in anti-oxidant enzyme activities c. Select for high anti-oxidant enzyme activities (e.g., biopsy individuals to score their phenotype and then choose breeders) d. Does life span increase as predicted? 6 Hypothetical Example 2 a. Select for high maximal O2 consumption b. Observe increase in blood [hemoglobin] c. Select for high blood [hemoglobin] d. Does VO2max increase as predicted? 7 Recent Physiological Perspectives Gibbs, A. G. 1999. Laboratory selection for the comparative physiologist. Journal of Experimental Biology 202:2709-2718. Harshman, L. G. , and A. A. Hoffmann. 2000. Laboratory selection experiments using Drosophila: what do they really tell us? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 15:32-36. Bennett, A. F. 2003. Experimental evolution and the Krogh Principle: generating biological novelty for functional and genetic analyses. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 76:1-11. Garland, T., Jr. 2003. Selection experiments: an under-utilized tool in biomechanics and organismal biology. Pages 23-56 in V. L. Bels, J.-P. Gasc, A. Casinos, eds. Vertebrate biomechanics and evolution. BIOS Scientific Publishers, Oxford, U.K. Bradley, T. J., and D. G. Folk. 2004. Analyses of physiological evolutionary response. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 77:1-9. Swallow, J. G., and T. Garland, Jr. 2005. Selection experiments as a tool in evolutionary and comparative physiology: insights into complex traits - An introduction to the symposium. Integrative and Comparative Biology 45:387-390. Swallow, J. G., J. P. Hayes, P. Koteja, and T. Garland, Jr. 2009. Selection experiments and experimental evolution of performance and physiology. Pages 301-351 in Experimental Evolution: Concepts, Methods, and Applications of Selection Experiments, T. Garland, Jr., and M. R. Rose, eds. Univ. of Calif. Press, Berkeley. Feder, M. E., T. Garland, Jr., J. H. Marden, and A. J. Zera. 2010. Locomotion in response to shifting climate zones: not so fast. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 72:167-190. 8 Types of "Selection Experiments" Domestication: The process may vary widely, e.g., dogs, cats, cattle, horses, corn. At some point, it involves some unintentional selection (e.g., ability of organisms to reproduce in altered conditions). Domestication often also involve some intentional selection for particular characteristics, such as tameness or coloration. Further selection may occur for particular traits (e.g., milk yield in cows) or to differentiate breeds based on various traits. 9 Domestication followed by Intentional Selection: 10 Modern European radiation Parker et al. 2004 Science 304: 1160-4 11 Mass-adjusted Log(MEI) Aggressive breeds have higher daily metabolizable energy intake (MEI) r = 0.83 2.90 N=9 2.85 No mention of selection on food consumption… P = 0.02 2.80 Changed as a correlated response to selection on aggressiveness? 2.75 2.70 What physiological or neurobiological mechanism might cause that? 2.65 2.60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 Aggressiveness score Loadings: Aggression to dogs and territorial defence Careau et al. Am Nat 2010 12 Pasi, B. M., and D. R. Carrier. 2003. Functional trade-offs in the limb muscles of dogs selected for running vs. fighting. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16:324-332. Kemp, T. J., K. N. Bachus, J. A. Nairn, and D. R. Carrier. 2005. Functional trade-offs in the limb bones of dogs selected for running versus fighting. J. Exp. Biol. 208:3475-3482. 13 Domestication: (http://ngm.nationalgeographic.com/2011/03/taming-wild-animals/ratliff-text/2) Siberia - Dmitri K. Belyaev developed colonies of silver foxes, river otters, minks, and rats, starting in 1959. 14 Domestication: Siberia - Dmitri K. Belyaev developed colonies of silver foxes, river otters, minks, and rats, starting in 1959. Frank Albert, a graduate student at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology in Germany, is studying two colonies of tame and hyperaggressive Siberian rats to determine the genetics behind their differences. A handful of genes could be responsible. http://www.nytimes.com/imagepages/2006/07/25/science/25rats2_ready.html http://ngm.nationalgeographic.com/2011/03/taming-wild-animals/musi-photography 15 Artificial Selection: Captive populations in which individuals in each generation are measured for a phenotypic trait (or combination of traits). Some top or bottom fraction of individuals is then chosen as the breeders to produce the next generation. This is called "truncation selection" or "mass selection." One variation is taking the highest-scoring (or lowest-scoring) male and female from within each family. Within-family selection increases the effective population size (Ne), reduces rate of inbreeding, and helps to eliminate confounding influences of some maternal effects. But, it also reduces the possible intensity of selection as compared with "mass selection," which involves choosing breeders without regard to their family membership. 16 The longestrunning vertebrate artificial selection experiment: Body Mass (g) Mice Male mice at 42 days of age 67 grams 100 gens. 30 grams Generation Bunger, L., A. Laidlaw, G. Bulfield, E. J. Eisen, J. F. Medrano, G. E. Bradford, F. Pirchner, U. Renne, W. Schlote, and W. G. Hill. 2001. Inbred lines of mice derived from long-term growth selected lines: unique resources for mapping growth genes. Mammalian Genome 12:678-686. 17 Laboratory Natural Selection: Individual phenotypes are not measured each generation, nor are breeders specifically chosen by the investigator. Rather, a freely breeding population is exposed to altered environmental conditions, such as different temperatures or salinities, or to altered husbandry conditions, which could favor changes in demographic schedules. Assuming that additive genetic variance exists for relevant traits, the population will adapt to the new conditions. Most common with non-vertebrates, including Drosophila, bacteria, and viruses, but have also been employed with vertebrates: Barnett and Dickson housed mouse colonies at room temperature or around 0o Celsius. 18 Barnett, S. A., and R. G. Dickson. 1984b. Milk production and consumption and growth of young of wild mice after ten generations in a cold environment. Journal of Physiology 346:409-417. In only 10 generations, "Eskimo mice" evolved to be larger and to have more body fat for their body size. 19 Intentional Field Introductions & Manipulations: David Reznick's guppies in Trinidad Reznick, D. N., F. H. Shaw, F. H. Rodd, and R. G. Shaw. 1997. Evaluation of the rate of evolution in natural populations of guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Science 275:1934-1937. Plus comment on page 1880. Anolis lizards introduced to Caribbean islands (Tom Schoener, Jonathan Losos) Losos, J. B., K. I. Warheit, and T. W. Schoener. 1997. Adaptive differentiation following experimental island colonization in Anolis lizards. Nature 387:70-73. Losos, J. B., D. A. Creer, D. Glossip, R. Goellner, A. Hampton, G. Roberts, N. Haskell, P. Taylor, and J. Ettling. 2000. Evolutionary implications of phenotypic plasticity in the hindlimb of the lizard Anolis sagrei. Evolution 54:301-305. Losos, J. B., T. W. Schoener, and D. A. Spiller. 2004. Predator-induced behaviour shifts and natural selection in fieldexperimental lizard populations. Nature 432:505-508. 20 "Accidental" Introductions & Manipulations: Drosophila introduced to North America Huey, R. B., G. W. Gilchrist, M. L. Carlson, D. Berrigan, and L. Serra. 2000. Rapid evolution of a geographic cline in size in an introduced fly. Science 287:308-309. Calboli, F. C. F., G. W. Gilchrist, and L. Partridge. 2003. Different cell size and cell number contribution in two newly established and one ancient body size cline of Drosophila subobscura. Evolution 57:566-573. House Sparrows Parkin, D. T., and S. R. Cole. 1985. Genetic differentiation and rates of evolution in some introduced populations of the House Sparrow, Passer domesticus in Australia and New Zealand. Heredity 54:15-23. Adaptations of fishes to ponds heated by nuclear power plants Smith, M. H., M. W. Smith, S. L. Scott, E. H. Liu, and J. C. Jones. 1983. Rapid evolution in a post-thermal environment. Copeia 1983:193-197. Adaptations of plants to living on mine tailings Macnair, M. R. 1987. Heavy metal tolerance in plants: A model evolutionary system. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 2:354359. Adaptations of rodents to poisons Smith, P., M. G. Townsend, and R. H. Smith. 1991. A cost of resistance in the brown rat? Reduced growth rate in warfarinresistant lines. Functional Ecology 5:441-447. 21 Examples of Selection Experiments 22 Falconer, D. S. 1992. Early selection experiments. Annu. Rev. Genet. 26:1-14. All the experiments described so far were done by geneticists. Tryon, in contrast, was a psychologist and he was not primarily concerned with the process of selection itself. ... The objectives of applying selection were to find out how the learning ability was inherited, to produce divergent strains, and to identify the behavioral and physiological traits associated with the maze learning. … The experiment was started in 1926 and the first publication was in 1929. Selection was made in both directions for the number of errors made in running the maze, and was carried on for 21 generations. Unfortunately inbreeding was practiced, as in most experiments at that time. ... The responses continued for about seven generations, after which there was hardly any overlap between the distributions of the two lines. … The experiment provided very convincing evidence that heredity was one of the factors contributing to the differences between individual rats in their ability to learn a maze. Crosses between the selected lines showed that the inheritance was polygenic. Subsequent … studies … showed that the differences were not in general learning ability, but were rather specific; for example, the maze-dull rats were more easily distracted by the noises made by the mechanical maze used for the selection (11). 23 Falconer, D. S. 1992. Early selection experiments. Annu. Rev. Genet. 26:1-14. All the experiments described so far were done by geneticists. Tryon, in contrast, was a psychologist and he was not primarily concerned with the process of selection itself. ... The objectives of applying selection were to find out how the learning ability was inherited, to produce divergent strains, and to identify the behavioral and physiological traits associated with the maze learning. … The experiment was started in 1926 and the first publication was in 1929. Selection was made in both directions for the number of errors made in running the maze, and was carried on for 21 generations. Unfortunately inbreeding was practiced, as in most experiments at that time. ... The responses continued for about seven generations, after which there was hardly any overlap between the distributions of the two lines. … The experiment provided very convincing evidence that heredity was one of the factors contributing to the differences between individual rats in their ability to learn a maze. Crosses between the selected lines showed that the inheritance was polygenic. Subsequent … studies … showed that the differences were not in general learning ability, but were rather specific; for example, the maze-dull rats were more easily distracted by the noises made by the mechanical maze used for the selection (11). 24 Falconer, D. S. 1992. Early selection experiments. Annu. Rev. Genet. 26:1-14. All the experiments described so far were done by geneticists. Tryon, in contrast, was a psychologist and he was not primarily concerned with the process of selection itself. ... The objectives of applying selection were to find out how the learning ability was inherited, to produce divergent strains, and to identify the behavioral and physiological traits associated with the maze learning. … The experiment was started in 1926 and the first publication was in 1929. Selection was made in both directions for the number of errors made in running the maze, and was carried on for 21 generations. Unfortunately inbreeding was practiced, as in most experiments at that time. ... The responses continued for about seven generations, after which there was hardly any overlap between the distributions of the two lines. … The experiment provided very convincing evidence that heredity was one of the factors contributing to the differences between individual rats in their ability to learn a maze. Crosses between the selected lines showed that the inheritance was polygenic. Subsequent … studies … showed that the differences were not in general learning ability, but were rather specific; for example, the maze-dull rats were more easily distracted by the noises made by the mechanical maze used for the selection (11). 25 Rats Tryon, R. C. 1929. The genetics of learning ability in rats. Univ. Calif. Publ. Psychol. 4:71-89. 26 Rats Ridley, 1996, p. 45 Hunt, H. R., C. A. Hoppert, and S. Rosen. 1955. Genetic factors in experimental rat caries. Pages 66-81 in R. F. Sognnaes, ed. Advances in experimental caries research. American Association for the Advancement of Science, Washington, D.C. 27 Selection for Ethanol Sleep Time in Laboratory Mice Human alcoholism involves both liking of alcohol and physical effects of (e.g., tolerance to) alcohol. This experiment targeted the latter only. 167 min 41.7 min Plomin, R., J. C. DeFries, and G. E. McClearn. 1990. Behavioral genetics: A primer. 2nd ed. W. H. Freeman, New York. 455 pp. 28 Selection for Ethanol Sleep Time in Laboratory Mice Note complete separation of shortand long-selected lines 33.3 min 200 min Plomin, R., J. C. DeFries, and G. E. McClearn. 1990. Behavioral genetics: A primer. 2nd ed. W. H. Freeman, New York. 455 pp. 29 The Importance of Replication A study of genetic differences between any two lines will likely find many that have nothing to do with the phenotypic difference of interest. 30 The Importance of Replication Line differences in the trait under selection may be caused by: 1. the selective breeding 2. founder effects 3. subsequent genetic drift 4. unique mutations 5. different adaptive responses 31 The Importance of Replication Line differences in other traits (correlated responses) may be caused by: 1. the selective breeding pleiotropic genetic effects genetic linkage 2. founder effects 3. subsequent genetic drift 4. unique mutations 5. different adaptive responses 32 Selection on Open-field Activity in Mice Video camera Method developed by C. S. Hall in 1930s to measure levels of fear and “emotional reactivity” in rodents 33 DeFries, J. C., J. R. Wilson, and G. E. McClearn. 1970. Open-field behavior in mice: selection response and situational generality. Behavior Genetics 1:195-211. 34 "The foundation population for the selection experiment consisted of 40 F3 litters which were descendants from an original cross of two inbred strains of mice (BALB/cJ and C57BL/6J)." DeFries, J. C., J. R. Wilson, and G. E. McClearn. 1970. Open-field behavior in mice: selection response and situational generality. Behavior Genetics 1:195-211. 35 91 cm square arena; # of photobeams crossed in 3 minutes, summed over 2 days The direct response to selection. Note consistency of response between replicates. Mice 600 beam breaks is at most ~ 91 m Total movement is ~91 m, at an average (although movement is actually periodic) velocity of 0.51 m/s Important features of experimental design: replication up, down, and control lines 36 A correlated response to selection. Note somewhat lower consistency of correlated response between replicates. 37 Coat color also changes! An example of pleiotropy, one of the main causes of genetic correlations. Again, note consistency of replicates. 38 "Two inbred strains of mice (BALB/cJ and C57BL/6J) which differ widely in open-field behavior were crossed …" Abstract. In segregating F2, F3, and F4 generations, albino mice had lower activity and higher defecation scores than pigmented animals when tested in a brightly lighted open field. These differences persisted when members of an F5 generation were tested under white light, but largely disappeared under red light. Thus it was concluded that there is a major gene effect on the quantitative traits of open-field activity and defecation which is mediated by the visual system and that albino mice are more photophobic than pigmented mice under conditions of bright illumination. DeFries, J. C., J. P. Hegmann, and M. W. Weir. 1966. Open-field behavior in mice: evidence for a major gene effect mediated by the visual system. Science 154:1577-1589. 39 Selection for Thermoregulatory Nesting Lynch, C. B. 1980. Response to divergent selection for nesting behavior in Mus musculus. Genetics 96:757-765. The base population was a genetically heterogeneous stock of lab mice (Mus musculus) originally derived from an 8-way cross among inbred strains. May be considered the first rodent selection experiment in "evolutionary physiology." 40 Selection for Thermoregulatory Nesting Lynch, C. B. 1994. Evolutionary inferences from genetic analyses of cold adaptation in laboratory and wild populations of the house mouse. Pages 278-301 in C. R. B. Boake, ed. Quantitative genetic studies of behavioral evolution. Univ. Chicago Press. 41 Selection for Thermoregulatory Nesting The overall realized heritability pooled across lines and replicates was 0.18 + 0.02 (0.15 + 0.03 for high nesting scores and 0.23 + 0.04 for low nesting scores), or 0.28 + 0.05 when adjusted for within-family selection. r = h2 s h2 = r/s 42 Selection for Thermoregulatory Nesting Cotton Used in 4 Days (g) Lynch, C. B. 1994. Evolutionary inferences from genetic analyses of cold adaptation in laboratory and wild populations of the house mouse. Pages 278-301 in C. R. B. Boake, ed. Quantitative genetic studies of behavioral evolution. Univ. Chicago Press. 60 50 High 40 30 20 Control 10 Low 0 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 Generation 35 40 45 Cause of limit in low lines is obvious: you cannot go below zero. 43 What Caused the Selection Limit in the High Lines? 44 Why a Selection Limit? Quantitative-Genetic Answers: Exhausted Additive Genetic Variance Genetic Correlations with Other Traits Counterposing Natural Selection Traditional, Interesting, Black Box 45 Why a Selection Limit? Abstract: To test the hypothesis that large, well-built, nests are an important component of fitness, we kept 12 mating pairs of two highselected, two control, and two low-selected lines, selected for thermoregulatory nest-building behavior, at 22 and 4 degrees C with access to 10 g of cotton to build a nest, for a period of 180 days. Measurements included number of lifters born per family, number of young per litter born and surviving up to 40 days of age, nest type built by the parents, and weight gain of the young from weaning (20 days of age) to 40 days of age. In all lines the production and survival of offspring was substantially decreased at 4 degrees C compared to 22 degrees C, but the high-selected lines produced more and better-quality offspring, surviving up to 40 days of age at both temperatures compared to the control and low-selected lines. This indicates that thermoregulatory nest-building behavior and evolutionary fitness are closely associated. So, we do not seem to have Counterposing Natural Selection. Bult, A., and C. B. Lynch. 1997. Nesting and fitness: lifetime reproductive success in house mice bidirectionally selected for thermoregulatory nest-building behavior. Behavior Genetics 27:231-240. 46 Why a Selection Limit? Functional Answers: Stopped here 19 Feb. 2015 Motivation to build nest at a maximum Not enough time or space to build larger Not enough time or space to eat Energetic cost is too high Hyperthermia (literally overheating) or possibly negative feedback to the thermoregulatory behavior of nest-building Not mutually exclusive, May elucidate mechanisms of evolutionary “constraints” 47 Selection for Treadmill Endurance in Laboratory Rats "The starting population was 96 male and 96 female genetically heterogeneous rats (N:NIH stock) obtained from a colony maintained at the National Institutes of Health. Each rat in the founder population was of different parentage, so selection was not among brothers and sisters, which broadens the genetic variance." Koch, L. G., and S. L. Britton. 2001. Artificial selection for intrinsic aerobic endurance running capacity in rats. Physiological Genomics 5:45-52. 48 Selection for Treadmill Endurance in Laboratory Rats Koch, L. G., and S. L. Britton. 2001. Artificial selection for intrinsic aerobic endurance running capacity in rats. Physiological Genomics 5:45-52. 49 Selection for Treadmill Endurance in Laboratory Rats Females Distance Run (m) Males 50 Selection for Treadmill Endurance in Laboratory Rats Fig. 6. Change in body weight for females (A) and males (B) at each generation of selection for the low (solid bars) and high (open bars) lines. For both sexes, the low line became heavier and the high line became lighter as a function of selection for running capacity across the six generations. At generation 6 (G6), the body weights of the low and high females differed by 34 g (20%, P , 0.001) and the body weights of the low and high males differed by 40 g (16%, P , 0.001). *P , 0.05, significant weight change different from generation 1 (G1) values within each line as assessed by the Dunnett test. Values are means 6 1 SE. 51 Remember that narrow-sense heritability indicates whether a trait tends to "run in families" and can be estimated as the slope of the regression of offspring mean on midparent mean … 52 Mean of Offspring - Trait A Least-squares linear regression to estimate heritability 3 2 N = 50 1 0 -1 Y = 0.80 X - 0.03 2 R = 0.56 -2 -3 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 Mean of Parents - Trait A 53 Similarly, a genetic correlation indicates whether two traits tend to "run in families" and can be estimated as the slope of the regression of offspring mean for one trait on midparent mean for the other trait (or vice versa) … 54 Mean of Offspring - Trait B This plot indicates that that Traits A and B tend to go together in families, and so are positively genetically correlated. 3 2 1 Therefore, selection to increase one would also cause an increase in the other. 0 -1 -2 -3 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 Hypothetical example: Trait A might be body mass and Trait B might be tail length. Mean of Parents - Trait A 55 A correlated response to selection indicates the presence of a genetic correlation. In rats, treadmill endurance capacity and body mass are negatively genetically correlated. In mice, voluntary wheel running and body mass are negatively genetically correlated. (next lecture on Garland mice) 56 Continued Selection for Treadmill Endurance in Lab Rats Wisløff, U., S. M. Najjar, Ø. Ellingsen, P. M. Haram, S. Swoap, Q. Al-Share, M. Fernstrom, K. Rezaei, S. J. Lee, L. G. Koch, S. L. Britton. 2005. Cardiovascular risk factors emerge after artificial selection for low aerobic capacity. Science 307:418-420. 57 Selection for Treadmill Endurance: Various Correlated Responses Lack of control line means we don’t know if HCR are better than average rats or LCR are worse than average, or both. Wisløff, U., S. M. Najjar, Ø. Ellingsen, P. M. Haram, S. Swoap, Q. Al-Share, M. Fernstrom, K. Rezaei, S. J. Lee, L. G. Koch, S. L. Britton. 2005. Cardiovascular risk factors emerge after artificial selection for low aerobic capacity. Science 307:418-420. 58 Selection for Treadmill Endurance in Laboratory Rats The amount of voluntary exercise has also diverged. HCR LCR Exercise behavior is genetically correlated with exercise ability (performance capacity). Why? Waters, R. P., K. J. Renner, R. B. Pringle, C. H. Summers, S. L. Britton, L. G. Koch, and J. G. Swallow. 2008. Selection for aerobic capacity affects corticosterone, monoamines and wheel-running activity. Physiology & Behavior 93:1044-1054. 59 Selection for Training Response (Plasticity) in Rats Adaptational response to aerobic exercise was artificially selected for across one generation in a founder population of 20 female and 20 male genetically heterogeneous rats (N:NIH). Selection for low and high response was based on the change in treadmill running capacity, assessed by meters (m) run to exhaustion before and after 24 days of modest treadmill running. Troxell, M. L., S. L. Britton, and L. G. Koch. 2003. Selected contribution: Variation and heritability for the adaptational response to exercise in genetically heterogeneous rats. Journal of Applied Physiology 94:1674-1681. 60 The bank vole, Clethrionomys glareolus Did not get to this in Winter 2014 Sadowska, E. T., K. Baliga-Klimczyk, K. M. Chrzascik, and P. Koteja. 2008. Laboratory model of adaptive radiation: a selection experiment in the bank vole. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 81:627-640. 61 can be reared in lab 62 Goal of Koteja's Selection Experiment Mimic an "adaptive radiation" from a generalized omnivorous rodent and develop: Herbivores (e.g., Microtus voles) Predators (e.g., Onychomys) Highly-Aerobic Runners (e.g., chipmunks) 63 Design of Koteja's Selection Experiment 16 lines have been established: 4 selected for ability to maintain body mass on a low-quality diet 4 selected for high predatory aggression on crickets 4 selected for high aerobic capacity measured during swimming 4 unselected, control lines ~3,500-4,000 animals/generation 64 The ability to cope with a low-quality of food: changes of body mass during a 4-day trial standard food low-quality food (dried grass) 65 65 The ability to grow on a herbivorous diet: body mass change in a test with low-quality diet Generations 0-3: Sadowska et al. 2008, Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 66 Predatory behavior: proportion of "predatory" individuals Generations 0-3: Sadowska et al. 2008, Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 67 The aerobic capacity: VO2max during swimming relaxed selection Generations 0-3: Sadowska et al. 2008, Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 68 Cumulative effects of the selection: differences between lines in SD units Generations 0-4: Swallow, Hayes, Koteja and Garland 2009, in: Experimental Evolution:... 69 Extra Slides Follow Koteja_Sel_Exp_Voles_UC R_26_April_2012_v3.pptx 70 Falconer, D.S. (1992) Early selection experiments. Annu. Rev. Genet. 26: 1-14. R. Tryon All the experiments described so far were done by geneticists. Tryon, in contrast, was a psychologist and he was not primarily concerned with the process of selection itself. He studied the ability of rats to learn a maze. The objectives of applying selection were to find out how the learning ability was inherited, to produce divergent strains, and to identify the behavioral and physiological traits associated with the maze learning. (Tryon’s papers are hard to obtain but for accounts of his experiment, with references, see refs. 11, 20, 24, 29). The experiment was started in 1926 and the first publication was in 1929. Selection was made in both directions for the number of errors made in running the maze, and was carried on for 21 generations. Unfortunately inbreeding was practiced, as in most experiments at that time. The selection in both directions was undeniably successful. The responses continued for about seven generations, after which there was hardly any overlap between the distributions of the two lines. The mean number of errors was reduced by 33% in the "maze-bright" rats and increased by 37% in the "maze-dull" rats. (See refs. 24, 29 for graphs of the responses and ref. 11 for graphs of the distributions.) The experiment provided very convincing evidence that heredity was one of the factors contributing to the differences between individual rats in their ability to learn a maze. Crosses between the selected lines showed that the inheritance was polygenic. Subsequent behavioral studies of the lines showed that the differences were not in general learning ability, but were rather specific; for example, the maze-dull rats were more easily distracted by the noises made by the mechanical maze used for the selection (11). 71 Bult, A., and C. B. Lynch. 1996. Multiple selection responses in house mice bidirectionally selected for thermoregulatory nest-building behavior: crosses of replicate lines. Behavior Genetics 26:439-446. Bult, A., and C. B. Lynch. 1997. Nesting and fitness: lifetime reproductive success in house mice bidirectionally selected for thermoregulatory nest-building behavior. Behavior Genetics 27:231240. Bult, A., and C.B. Lynch. 2000. Breaking through artificial selection limits of an adaptive behavior in mice and the consequences for correlated responses. Behavior Genetics 30:193-206. Bult, A., L. Hiestand, E. A. Van der Zee, and C. B. Lynch. 1993. Circadian rhythms differ between selected mouse lines: a model to study the role of vasopressin neurons in the suprachiasmatic nuclei. Brain Research Bulletin 32:623-627. Bult, A., E. A. Van der Zee, J. C. Compaan, and C. B. Lynch. 1992. Differences in the number of arginine-vasopressin-immunoreactive neurons exist in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of house mice selected for differences in nest building behavior. Brain Research 578:335-338. Kolbe, J. J., M. Leal, T. W. Schoener, D. A. Spiller, and J. B. Losos. 2012. Founder effects persist despite adaptive differentiation: a field experiment with lizards. Science. 72 Artificial Selection on Longevity in Mice Nagai, J., C. Y. Yin, and M. P. Sabour. 1995. Lines of mice selected for reproductive longevity. Growth, Development & Aging 59:79-91. Mice were pair-mated, and their offspring from parities 5-9 were used as breeders for the next generation. Age at Last Parturition (days) Young Born Alive Lifespan (days) Selection line 1 297 77 378 Selection line 2 299 83 437 Control line 191 46 347 At gener. 16: This is covered in 105 Evolution 73 This is covered in 105 Evolution At generation 89, selection in the low line was discontinued because of poor viability and an oil concentration so low that it cannot be measured. At this point, the populations had changed from 4.7% oil to 19.3% in the high and 1.1% in the low line. Note physical lower limit of zero % oil Ridley, 1996, p. 238 74 This is covered in 105 Evolution r = h2 s only works for initial generations Ridley, 1996, pp. 236-239 75 2004. Genetics 168:2141-2155. 76 Remember that narrow-sense heritability can be estimated as the slope of the regression of offspring on midparent mean … Correlation (bivariate) - relationship between two traits or variables: Can be positive or negative Ranges from -1 to +1 Usually assumed to be linear for purposes of statistical testing Pearson product-moment correlation assumes bivariate normality; denoted as r or R Spearman rank correlation is a "nonparametric" alternative By itself, correlation does not indicate causation! 77 SCOR1PCT = 100 * (Exam1 + Exam2 + Critique1 + Critique2)/160 Descriptive Statistics EXAM1 EXAM1PCT EXAM2 EXAM2PCT SCOR1PCT Valid N (lis twis e) N Statis tic 41 41 41 41 40 40 Minimum Statis tic 36.00 60.00 34.00 56.67 68.13 Maximum Statis tic 58.50 97.50 57.00 95.00 97.19 Mean Statis tic Std. Error 49.2073 .9703 82.0122 1.6171 46.9146 .9229 78.1911 1.5382 84.6484 1.0471 Std. Deviation Statis tic 6.21287 10.35478 5.90963 9.84939 6.62239 Skewness Statis tic Std. Error -.681 .369 -.681 .369 -.199 .369 -.199 .369 -.458 .374 78