Summarising the Data

advertisement
Meta-analysis:
summarising data for two
arm trials and other simple
outcome studies
Steff Lewis
statistician
When can/should you do
a meta-analysis?
• When more than one study has estimated an
effect
• When there are no differences in the study
characteristics that are likely to substantially
affect outcome
• When the outcome has been measured in
similar ways
• When the data are available (take care with
interpretation when only some data are
available)
Types of data
• Dichotomous/ binary data
• Counts of infrequent events
• Short ordinal scales
• Long ordinal scales
• Continuous data
• Censored data
What to collect
• Need the total number of patients in
each treatment group
Plus:
• Binary data
– The number of patients who had the
relevant outcome in each treatment group
• Continuous data
– The mean and standard deviation of the
effect for each treatment group
•
Then enter data into RevMan / MIX
(easy to use and free)
http://www.mix-for-meta-analysis.info/
http://www.cc-ims.net/RevMan/
Or R (harder to use and free)
Or Stata (harder to use and costs)
Etc etc....
Summary statistic for each
study
• Calculate a single summary
statistic to represent the effect
found in each study
• For binary data
– Risk ratio with rarer event as
outcome
• For continuous data
– Difference between means
Meta-analysis
Averaging studies
• Starting with the summary statistic for
each study, how should we combine
these?
• A simple average gives each study
equal weight
• This seems intuitively wrong
• Some studies are more likely to give an
answer closer to the ‘true’ effect than
others
Weighting studies
• More weight to the studies which give
us more information
– More participants
– More events
– Lower variance
• Weight is closely related to the width of
the study confidence interval: wider
confidence interval = less weight
Displaying results graphically
• RevMan (the Cochrane Collaboration’s
free meta-analysis software) and MIX
produce forest plots (as do R and Stata
and some other packages)
Review :
Comparison:
Outcome:
Corticosteroids for acute traumatic brain injury
01 Any steroid administered in any dose against no steroid
01 Death at end of follow up period
Study
or sub-category
Alexander 1972
Brackman 1983
CRASH 2004
Chacon 1987
Cooper 1979
Dearden 1986
Faupel 1976
Gaab 1994
Giannotta 1984
Grumme 1995
Hemesniemi 1979
Pitts 1980
Ransohoff 1972
Saul 1981
Stubbs 1989
Zagara 1987
Zarete 1995
Steroid
n/N
16/55
44/81
1052/4985
1/5
26/49
33/68
16/67
19/133
34/72
38/175
35/81
114/201
9/17
8/50
13/98
4/12
0/30
6179
Total (95% CI)
Total events: 1462 (Steroid), 1194 (Control)
Test for heterogeneity: Chi² = 26.46, df = 15 (P = 0.03), I² = 43.3%
Test for overall effect: Z = 3.27 (P = 0.001)
Control
n/N
RR (fixed)
95% CI
RR (fixed)
95% CI
22/55
47/80
893/4979
0/5
13/27
21/62
16/28
21/136
7/16
49/195
36/83
38/74
13/18
9/50
5/54
4/12
0/30
0.73 [0.43, 1.23
0.92 [0.70, 1.21
1.18 [1.09, 1.27
3.00 [0.15, 59.8
1.10 [0.69, 1.77
1.43 [0.94, 2.19
0.42 [0.24, 0.71
0.93 [0.52, 1.64
1.08 [0.59, 1.98
0.86 [0.60, 1.25
1.00 [0.70, 1.41
1.10 [0.86, 1.42
0.73 [0.43, 1.25
0.89 [0.37, 2.12
1.43 [0.54, 3.80
1.00 [0.32, 3.10
Not estimable
5904
1.12 [1.05, 1.20
0.1
0.2
0.5
Steroid better
1
2
5
Steroid w orse
10
Heterogeneity
What is heterogeneity?
•
Heterogeneity is variation between the
studies’ results
Causes of heterogeneity
Differences between studies with respect
to:
• Patients: diagnosis, in- and exclusion
criteria, etc.
• Interventions: type, dose, duration,
etc.
• Outcomes: type, scale, cut-off points,
duration of follow-up, etc.
• Quality and methodology:
randomised or not, allocation
concealment, blinding, etc.
How to deal with heterogeneity
1. Do not pool at all
2. Ignore heterogeneity: use fixed effect
model
3. Allow for heterogeneity: use random
effects model
4. Explore heterogeneity: meta-regression
(tricky)
How to assess heterogeneity from a
forest plot
Statistical measures of heterogeneity
• The Chi2 test measures the
amount of variation in a set of
trials, and tells us if it is more than
would be expected by chance
Estimates with 95% confidence intervals
Study
Liggins 1972
Block 1977
Morrison 1978
Trials from
Cochrane logo:
Corticosteroids for
preterm birth
(neonatal death)
Taeusch 1979
Papageorgiou 1979
Heterogeneity test
Schutte 1979
Collaborative Group 1981
0.61
Pooled
0.05 0.25 1
( 0.46 , 0.81 )
4
Odds ratio
Corticosteroids better
Corticosteroids worse
Q = 11.2 (6 d.f.)
p = 0.08
Estimates with 95% confidence intervals
Study
Corticosteroids for
preterm birth
(neonatal death)
Liggins 1972
Block 1977
Morrison 1978
Taeusch 1979
Papageorgiou 1979
Heterogeneity test
Schutte 1979
Q = 11.2 (6 d.f.)
Collaborative Group 1981
p = 0.08
Heterogeneity test
Q = 44.7 (27 d.f.)
p = 0.02
0.05 0.25 1
4
Odds ratio
0.05 0.25 1
4
Odds ratio
I squared quantifies
heterogeneity
Q  df
I  100 
Q
2
where Q = heterogeneity c2 statistic
I2 can be interpreted as the proportion of
total variability explained by
heterogeneity, rather than chance
• Roughly, I2 values of 25%, 50%,
and 75% could be interpreted as
indicating low, moderate, and high
heterogeneity
• For more info see: Higgins JPT et
al. Measuring inconsistency in
meta-analyses. BMJ
2003;327:557-60.
Fixed and random effects
Fixed effect
Philosophy behind fixed effect model:
• there is one real value for the treatment
effect
• all trials estimate this one value
Problems with ignoring heterogeneity:
• confidence intervals too narrow
Random effects
Philosophy behind random effects
model:
• there are many possible real values for
the treatment effect (depending on dose,
duration, etc etc).
• each trial estimates its own real value
Example
Could we just add the data from all
the trials together?
• One approach to combining trials would
be to add all the treatment groups
together, add all the control groups
together, and compare the totals
• This is wrong for several reasons, and it
can give the wrong answer
If we add up the columns we get 34.3%
vs 32.5% , a RR of 1.06, a higher chance
of death in the steroids group
From a meta-analysis, we get
RR=0.96 , a lower chance of
death in the steroids group
Problems with simple addition of
studies
• breaks the power of randomisation
• imbalances within trials introduce bias
*
The Pitts trial contributes 17% (201/1194) of all the data to the
experimental column, but 8% (74/925) to the control column.
Therefore it contributes more information to the average chance
of death in the experimental column than it does to the control
column.
There is a high chance of death in this trial, so the chance of
death for the expt column is higher than the control column.
Interpretation
Interpretation - “Evidence of
absence” vs “Absence of evidence”
• If the confidence interval crosses the
line of no effect, this does not mean that
there is no difference between the
treatments
• It means we have found no
statistically significant difference in the
effects of the two interventions
In the example below, as more data is included,
the overall odds ratio remains the same but the
confidence interval decreases.
It is not true that there is ‘no difference’ shown
in the first rows of the plot – there just isn’t
enough data to show a statistically significant
result.
Review :
Comparison:
Outcome:
Steff
01 Absence of evidence and Evidence of absence
01 Increasing the amount of data...
Study
or sub-category
Treatment
n/N
Control
n/N
1 study
2 studies
3 studies
4 studies
5 studies
10/100
20/200
30/300
40/400
50/500
15/100
30/200
45/300
60/400
75/500
OR (fixed)
95% CI
OR (fixed)
95% CI
0.63
0.63
0.63
0.63
0.63
0.1
0.2
0.5
Favours treatment
1
2
5
Favours control
10
[0.27,
[0.34,
[0.38,
[0.41,
[0.43,
1.48]
1.15]
1.03]
0.96]
0.92]
Interpretation - Weighing up benefit
and harm
•
When interpreting results, don’t just
emphasise the positive results.
•
A treatment might cure acne instantly,
but kill one person in 10,000 (very
important as acne is not life
threatening).
Interpretation - Quality
•
Rubbish studies = unbelievable results
•
If all the trials in a meta-analysis were
of very low quality, then you should be
less certain of your conclusions.
•
Instead of “Treatment X cures
depression”, try “There is some
evidence that Treatment X cures
depression, but the data should be
interpreted with caution.”
Summary
• Choose an appropriate effect measure
•
Collect data from trials and do a metaanalysis if appropriate
•
Interpret the results carefully
–
–
–
–
Evidence of absence vs absence of
evidence
Benefit and harm
Quality
Heterogeneity
Sources of statistics help and advice
Cochrane Handbook for Systematic
Reviews of Interventions
http://www.cochrane.org/resources/handbook/index.htm
The Cochrane distance learning material
http://www.cochrane-net.org/openlearning/
The Cochrane RevMan user guide.
http://www.cc-ims.net/RevMan/documentation.htm
Download