Running Head: MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS MINDFULNESS AND RUMINATION: MEDIATORS OF CHANGE IN DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS? A PRELIMINARY INVESTIGATION OF A UNIVERSAL MINDFULNESS INTERVENTION FOR ADOLESCENTS Trainee Name Nicola Motton, College of Life and Environmental Science, University of Exeter Supervisor Willem Kuyken, College of Life and Environmental Science, University of Exeter Nominated journal Behaviour Research and Therapy Word count 7,075 (13,313 with appendices) This work is submitted in partial fulfilment of a doctoral degree in clinical psychology. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 2 Table of Contents Abstract …………………………………………………………………………………. 3 Introduction ………………………………………………………………………………. 4 Method …………………………………………………………………………………. 10 Statistical Analyses ………………………………………………………………………. 15 Results ………………………………………………………………………….………… 17 Discussion ……………………………………………………………………...………… 24 References …………………………………………………………………….......……… 32 Appendix A: Expanded Results ………………………………………………..………… 41 Appendix B: Journal’s Author Guidelines …………………………………….…………. 43 Appendix C: Ethical Approval letter …………………………………………..…………. 61 Appendix D: Questionnaires …………………………………………………...………… 62 Appendix E: Consent Forms and Debriefing Statement …………………………………. 65 Dissemination Statement …………………………………………………………………. 68 MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 3 Abstract Mindfulness-based interventions reduce depressive symptoms and rumination, and enhance mindfulness in adults; this non-randomised controlled feasibility study aimed to determine whether these conclusions apply to young people, and whether mindfulness and rumination mediate the effect on depressive symptoms. Participants aged 12-16 received a nine-week universal mindfulness intervention in schools delivered by trained teachers (intervention group, N = 256) or their regular school curriculum (control group, N = 266). Intervention schools were matched to control schools on key variables (publicly-funded versus private, mainstream versus special needs). Young people who received the intervention reported fewer depressive symptoms post-intervention relative to controls, which was maintained at three-month follow-up. Mindfulness and rumination were unchanged immediately after the intervention, however by follow-up, intervention participants were significantly more mindful and less likely to ruminate than controls. The extent to which young people practiced mindfulness was negatively correlated with depressive symptoms at post-intervention and follow-up, positively correlated with mindfulness at post-intervention and follow-up, and positively correlated with rumination at follow-up. This universal mindfulness intervention shows promise for reducing depressive symptoms, reducing rumination and increasing mindfulness in young people, however further research is warranted, particularly regarding the mechanisms of change. Keywords: Depressive symptoms, mindfulness, rumination, adolescence, prevention. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 4 Mindfulness and rumination: Mediators of change in depressive symptoms? A preliminary investigation of a universal mindfulness intervention for adolescents The World Health Organisation (WHO) has ranked unipolar major depression as one of the most burdensome diseases in terms of both the number of years lost to disease-related disability, and premature mortality (Murray & Lopez, 1996). It is expected to become the most burdensome disease in developed countries by 2030 (Mathers & Loncar, 2006). Depression can be not only disabling (Judd et al., 2000a), but also often runs a chronic course (Judd, 1997; Judd, 2000b); around 75% of individuals will relapse within five years of their first episode (Kennard, Emslie, Mayes, & Hughes, 2006). Importantly, early age of onset is associated with more chronic and recurrent depression in adulthood (Coryell et al., 2009), greater intention to act on suicidal ideation when depressed (Thompson, 2008) as well as a higher likelihood of attempted suicide in later life (Williams et al., 2012; Zisook et al., 2007). Research on the course of major depression has suggested it is preferable to consider depressive symptoms on a continuum with depression (Lewinsohn, Solomon, Seeley, & Zeiss, 2000), as depressive symptoms are both predictive of later depression (Pine, Cohen, Cohen, & Brook, 1999; Shankman et al., 2009) and associated with distress and functional impairment in their own right (Aalto-Setälä, Marttunen, Tuulio-Henriksson, Poikolainen, & Lönnqvist, 2002). In fact, the extent of depressive symptoms is associated in a linear fashion with psychosocial impairment and the probability of developing depression, leading some researchers to conclude that low-grade depressive symptoms should be as much a target for intervention as depressive symptoms that are deemed to fall above the threshold for depression (Lewinsohn et al., 2000). Longitudinal studies find depressive symptoms peak markedly in mid-adolescence (Ge, Lorenz, Conger, & Elder, 1994; Saluja et al., MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 5 2004). It is imperative therefore, that interventions are developed to reduce depressive symptoms in this age group, in order to improve adolescent wellbeing and functioning, and prevent adverse outcomes later in life. Interest in preventative interventions for depression has grown in the last few years, with promising findings to date (Cuijpers, van Straten, Smit, Mihalopoulos, & Beekman, 2008), however a recent large scale RCT (N = 5030) of classroom-based Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) found no reduction in depressive symptoms in at-risk adolescents compared to an attention control group or usual school provision (Stallard et al., 2012). This finding calls for alternative means of prevention to be considered. Any successful preventative intervention must target factors known to predict and maintain depressive symptoms. Rumination, defined as “behaviours and thoughts that focus one’s attention on one’s depressive symptoms and on the implications of these symptoms” (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991, p. 569), has been strongly implicated in the onset and maintenance of depression; rumination has been found in prospective studies to predict higher levels of depressive symptoms following a stressful event, even after accounting for baseline levels of depressive symptoms (Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991), contribute to the maintenance and exacerbation of depressive symptoms in adolescents (Burwell & Shirk, 2007), and predict the onset of new depressive episodes in adolescents (Broderick & Korteland, 2004), as well as adults (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000). Given the substantial body of research that strongly implicates rumination in the development and maintenance of depressive symptoms and depression, preventative interventions should aim to reduce rumination. If an intervention were to reduce adolescents’ tendency to ruminate, it could be expected to reduce depressive symptoms and the likelihood of later depression. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 6 Mindfulness may be one such method of overcoming rumination, as a means to reducing depressive symptoms. Mindfulness has been described as “the awareness that emerges through paying attention on purpose, in the present moment, and non-judgementally to the unfolding of experience moment by moment” (Kabat-Zinn, 2003, p. 145). The two most established mindfulness programmes are the eight-week Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy programme (MBCT; Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2013) designed to prevent depressive relapse for individuals who have experienced three or more episodes of depression, and the eight-week Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction course (MBSR; Kabat-Zinn, 1990), designed to aid the management of stress, pain and physical ill health in the general population. Both interventions reduce depressive symptoms in adults; MBCT is effective in preventing the recurrence of depressive episodes in adults with chronic depression (Kuyken et al., 2008; Teasdale et al., 2000), indeed, the evidence is sufficiently robust for NICE (2009) to recommend MBCT as an intervention for preventing relapse in adults who experience recurrent depression. In addition to the research on MBCT, studies of MBSR suggest it can reduce depressive symptoms in nonclinical samples of adults (Deyo, Wilson, Ong, & Koopman, 2009; Labelle, Campbell, & Carlson, 2010). Thus research with adults supports the premise that mindfulness interventions can be used to reduce depressive symptoms in clinical and non-clinical adult samples. Considerably less research has been conducted examining the application of mindfulness interventions for young people, however initial research suggests that, at least in the form of MBSR, mindfulness does indeed reduce depressive symptoms in adolescents presenting with mental health problems (Biegel, Brown, Shapiro, & Schubert, 2009). The literature on the application of mindfulness interventions for children and adolescents is growing, however no studies to date have examined MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 7 the mechanisms of change for mindfulness interventions in this population (Harnett & Dawe, 2012). Understanding the mechanisms through which mindfulness interventions produce change in depressive symptoms is essential, as for interventions to be maximally effective, the aspects of the intervention that bring about change must be well understood in order that they may be enhanced (Kazdin, 2007). The current study examines the hypothesis that a mindfulness intervention for young people may reduce depressive symptoms as a result of reducing rumination. In this context, reduction in depressive symptoms is the outcome, and reducing rumination as a result of learning mindfulness, the hypothesised mechanism through which mindfulness interventions may exert their effect. The concept of mechanisms of change is closely related to the concept of mediation; a mediator is a variable that accounts statistically for the effect of an intervention on a given outcome. In order for mediation to be established, there must be significant relationships between the intervention and the outcome (depressive symptoms), the intervention and the process variables through which the intervention is expected to exert its effect (mindfulness and rumination), and the process variables and the respective outcome (depressive symptoms). Furthermore, the process variables must account uniquely for some of the variance of the intervention on depressive symptoms, after the effect of the intervention itself has been accounted for. A temporal sequence of events is a pre-requisite to establish mediation, as mediation presumes causal links between process and outcome. That is the intervention effects change in process variables (reduced rumination and increased mindfulness), leading to a change in outcome (reduced depressive symptoms). Therefore in order to establish mediation, change in process variables must occur prior to change in outcome, otherwise it cannot be concluded that the outcome changed as a result of change in process MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 8 variables. Research on preventative interventions must measure both outcome and process variables, in order to establish how interventions work, so that effectiveness can be maximised. Initial evidence with adults supports the notion that mindfulness interventions reduce depressive symptoms by reducing rumination (e.g. Labelle et al., 2010). Certainly for MBCT it has been theorised that mindfulness prevents depressive relapse by allowing individuals to become more aware of depressive thoughts and feelings, and to observe them from a neutral stance, without engaging in the ruminative processes that perpetuate low mood and lead to depression (Teasdale et al., 2000). Indeed, research supports this theoretical supposition; the effect of MBCT on depressive symptoms is mediated through enhancing mindfulness (Kuyken et al., 2010). Research on MBSR concurs with this finding, MBSR has been found not just to reduce depressive symptoms in adults, but to reduce rumination and enhance mindfulness with adults who are predominantly not depressed (Deyo et al., 2009). Furthermore, reductions in rumination have been found to mediate the effect of MBSR on depressive symptoms in adults (Labelle et al., 2010). Research to date suggests mindfulness interventions may be effective at reducing depressive symptoms in adults (Deyo et al., 2009; Kuyken et al., 2008; Teasdale et al., 2000), and potentially with adolescents with mental health problems (Biegel et al., 2009). Some evidence suggests this may occur as a result of reducing rumination and enhancing mindfulness (Deyo et al., 2009; Kuyken et al., 2010; Labelle et al., 2010). However it is yet to be determined whether the reduction in depressive symptoms following mindfulness interventions can be replicated in other populations of adolescents (such as non-clinical populations) and whether the mechanisms of change hypothesised here are valid for adolescents, that is, whether depressive MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 9 symptoms are in fact reduced as a result of improved ability to react to mental events in a more adaptive manner: mindfully rather than ruminatively. This non-randomised controlled feasibility study (Craig et al., 2008) investigates these questions by examining the effect of a universal school-based mindfulness intervention specifically designed for adolescents on depressive symptoms, mindfulness, and rumination. Universal programmes target the general population irrespective of their level of symptoms or risk of developing a disorder, whereas selective interventions target high risk individuals who are asymptomatic, and indicated interventions target individuals with subthreshold symptoms (Cuijpers et al., 2008). Universal interventions can be advantageous to selective or indicated programmes as they are low cost and non-stigmatising (Stallard et al., 2012), and delivering these within a school setting is a pragmatic and powerful means of affecting the wellbeing of young people (Weare & Nind, 2011). The Mindfulness in Schools Programme (MiSP) is a universal mindfulness intervention, which aims to enhance the wellbeing and mental health of young people. This nine-week complex intervention (Craig et al., 2008) was designed specifically for adolescents, and delivered during regular school lessons by trained secondary school teachers. Given the sharp rise in depressive symptoms in mid-adolescence (Ge et al., 1994; Saluja et al., 2004), the effects of the intervention are examined on a group of adolescents in the 12-16 year age range. This study asks three questions: Does participation in this universal school-based mindfulness programme reduce depressive symptoms in young people, relative to young people in matched control groups who receive usual school provision? Do young people who take part in the programme learn to respond more adaptively, that is does their tendency to respond in a mindful way increase, and their tendency to respond in a ruminative way decrease, compared to MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 10 their usual school provision counterparts? And for young people who participate in the programme: Are changes in depressive symptoms mediated by improvements in their ability to be mindful, and reductions in their tendency to ruminate? Correspondingly there are three hypotheses for these questions. Firstly, it is predicted that participation in the mindfulness programme will reduce young people’s depressive symptoms relative to controls, and that this difference will be apparent following the intervention and at three-month follow-up, during the stressful summer exam period. Secondly, it is predicted that participation in the mindfulness programme will increase young people’s tendency to be mindful and reduce their tendency to ruminate relative to controls, and that this difference will be apparent following the intervention and at three-month follow-up. Thirdly, it is predicted that for young people who participate in the mindfulness programme, reductions in depressive symptoms will be mediated by increases in their tendency to be mindful and reductions in their tendency to ruminate. Finally, an exploratory approach is adopted to understand the relationships between the amounts of mindfulness practice young people engaged in and changes in outcome (depressive symptoms) and process variables (mindfulness and rumination). Method Design and Procedure This study was embedded in a larger trial that examined the feasibility and efficacy of the MiS (Mindfulness in Schools) programme on a range of mental health and wellbeing outcomes. Readers interested in knowing more about the wider project are referred to the manuscript for further information (Kuyken et al., 2013). The current study employed a non-randomised controlled parallel group (mindfulness intervention versus matched control group) design. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 11 Outcome (presence and frequency of depressive symptoms) and process measures (mindfulness and rumination) were measured pre- and post- intervention, and at follow-up (three months after baseline). The MiS programme consisted of nine weekly hour-long sessions delivered during regular school lessons, usually in place of religious studies or personal, social, health and economic (PSHE) education lessons. The programme was delivered by schoolteachers; the programme developers themselves, and teachers who had been trained and deemed competent by them. Participants in control schools received their regular school curriculum, including elements that related to social or emotional wellbeing. Schools that had teachers who were trained and deemed competent at delivering the programme were selected for the intervention arm. Control schools were selected due to having teachers who were interested, but not trained in the programme, to control for the effect of teacher interest. Intervention and control schools were matched on several key variables: feepaying or public-funded status, school year group, and the school’s academic record (based on published Office for Standards in Education (OFSTED) findings). Head Teachers were approached and offered the opportunity for their school to be included in the study. Parents received a letter from the Head Teacher informing them their child was taking part in a research study and allowing them to retract their child from the study. Participants who chose not to take part were provided with alternative activities in place of the mindfulness intervention, and did not complete the measures. The study received ethical approval from the University of Exeter Psychology Department Ethics Committee (Ref. 2011/527, December 2011; Appendix C). MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 12 The MiS Programme The MiS programme material is drawn from both mindfulness-based stress reduction (Kabat-Zinn, 1990) and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (Segal et al., 2013), and has been developed over the last 4 years, in response to feedback from the 200 teachers who have received training in the programme and the 2,000 young people who have taken part. The programme aims to teach young people about the benefits of mindfulness and to increase their capacity for being mindful through regular in-session and out of session practice. The 9-week programme employs a number of elements considered important to the effectiveness of school-based wellbeing programmes (Durlak & DuPre, 2008; Weare & Nind, 2011), such as shorter practices, age-appropriate teaching material, an explicit and experimental teaching style, and strong fidelity to the programme manual. Young people were provided with a programme handbook and access to guided mindfulness practices with CD or MP3 audio files. Participants The intervention was universal, thus all consenting participants took part in the study; no participants were excluded on any grounds. Measures Data collection occurred at three time points; pre-intervention data were collected at the start of the second school term (January 2012), post-intervention at the end of the second school term (March 2012) and follow-up data in the final term (May 2012). Follow-up occurred during end of year exams, thus providing a test of the intervention’s effects during a period of academic MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 13 pressure. Data were collected via online or paper based questionnaires, whichever was most convenient for the teachers at their school. Outcome measure: Depressive symptoms The presence and severity of depressive symptoms were measured using the 8-item version of the Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (8-item CES-D; Steffick, 2000). The 8-item version is a shortened version of the original twenty-item scale (Radloff, 1977), which is a widely used measure of the frequency and severity of depressive symptoms in the general population that has good psychometric properties (Fendrich, Weissman, & Warner, 1990; Radloff, 1991). The 8-item scale requires respondents to rate how much of the time over the past week they felt or behaved in certain ways on a 4-point Likert scale from “none or almost none of the time” to “all or almost all of the time”. A sample item is “ How much of the time during the past week have you felt depressed?” Total score is achieved by summing all items (range 8-32). Though not as widely used, the shortened version has demonstrated acceptable internal consistency (α = .81 male sample, α = .85 female sample), and validity, indicated by Confirmatory Factor Analysis, which despite a significant χ² value, found a score of over .90 for the Tucker Lewis Index and Comparative Fit Index, and a Root Mean Square Error smaller than .80, indicative of good validity (Bracke, Levecque, & Van de Velde, 2008). The measure was deemed appropriate for the age group, having been recently used in a large-scale European study of over 43,000 young people aged 15 and above (Bracke et al., 2008), and piloted with a group of young people whose feedback supported the acceptability of the measure. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 14 Process measures Mindfulness Propensity for mindfulness was measured using the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R; Feldman, Hayes, Kumar, Greeson, & Laurencceau, 2007). This 12 item abbreviated version of the original 18-item scale (Kumar, 2005) pertains to four facets of mindfulness: attention, present-focus, awareness, and acceptance. Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale from “rarely or not at all” to “almost always”. A sample item is “I am able to accept the thoughts and feelings I have”. The scale has demonstrated acceptable internal consistency in two samples (sample 1 α = .74; Sample 2 α = .77), and convergent and discriminant validity with measures of related constructs (Feldman et al., 2007). The measure was used in a recent study of young people aged 11-17 (Hennelly, 2011), and feedback from piloting with young people supported its acceptability with this age group. Rumination For a brief measure of rumination, the rumination subscale of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ; Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007) was used. The full 36 item questionnaire measures a total of nine different cognitive coping strategies, has been validated for use with individuals aged 12 and above, has good convergent and discriminant validity (poorer emotion regulation scores correlate positively with depression and anxiety) and good reliability (α ranged from .75 to .87; Garnefiski & Kraaij, 2007). The four items of the rumination subscale were rated on a 5-point Likert scale from “never or almost never” to MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 15 “always or almost always”. A sample item is “I dwell upon the feelings the situation has evoked in me”. Practice The extent to which intervention participants had practiced using mindfulness was assessed at three-month follow-up with the question: “During the .b course you were taught a range of mindfulness practices. Since the end of the course how often have you used these practices?”. Frequency of practice was rated using a 6-point scale (never to almost every day). Statistical Analyses Descriptive statistics are reported for baseline data in the control and intervention arms to enable comparison. Baseline data were examined using independent t-tests and Chi-square to determine any pre-existing imbalances in demographic, outcome or process variables, to indicate which variables to use as covariates in later analyses. In relation to the first research question; whether participation in the mindfulness programme reduced depressive symptoms relative to controls at post-intervention and at threemonth follow-up, between-groups analyses in the form of an ANCOVA was run, with depressive symptoms at post-intervention and three-month follow-up as the dependent variables, and condition (intervention or control) as the independent variable. Given that participants in this study were not randomised to condition, gender, age, ethnicity and baseline level of depressive symptoms were used as covariates, to ensure that observed differences could be attributed to the intervention rather than to any baseline imbalances in demographic variables or pre-existing individual differences in symptomology. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 16 In relation to the second research question; whether adolescents who took part in the programme learnt to respond more adaptively, that is did their tendency to respond in a mindful way increase, and their tendency to respond in a ruminative way decrease, compared to their usual school provision counterparts, a further ANCOVA was run, to provide a between-groups measure of mindfulness and rumination in the intervention group compared to controls at postintervention and at follow-up. Mindfulness and rumination at post-intervention and three-month follow-up were the dependent variables, and condition (intervention or control) the independent variable. In order to account for lack of randomisation to condition, gender, age, ethnicity and baseline mindfulness and rumination were used as covariates. This allowed the observed effects to be attributed to the intervention and not imbalances in gender ratio between conditions, or preexisting differences in mindfulness or rumination. In relation to the final research question, that is whether change in depressive symptoms was mediated by improvements in young people’s ability to be mindful and reductions in their tendency to ruminate, within-groups meditational analyses would be conducted using Baron and Kenny’s (1986) approach. Firstly, in order to establish whether the mindfulness intervention is related to depressive symptoms, a simple regression would be conducted with depressive symptoms at follow-up as the dependent variable, and mindfulness intervention as the predictor. Assuming the mindfulness intervention does predict depressive symptoms at follow-up, step two would entail determining whether the mindfulness intervention predicts change in mindfulness and rumination post-intervention. A simple regression using change scores (from baseline to post-intervention) for mindfulness and rumination as dependent variables, and mindfulness intervention as the predictor would be conducted. If step one and two were completed successfully, this would demonstrate that the intervention correlates with both outcome MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 17 (depressive symptoms) and process variables (mindfulness and rumination). Finally it must be determined whether the change scores in mindfulness and rumination (pre to post-intervention) are correlated with depressive symptoms at follow-up. This third step would use multiple regression, with depressive symptoms at follow-up as the dependent variable, and mindfulness intervention and changes scores for mindfulness and rumination as predictors. This would enable a test of whether change in mindfulness and rumination between baseline and post-intervention accounted for some of the variance in depressive symptoms post-intervention, after the effect of the intervention was included in the equation. If the impact of the mindfulness intervention on depressive symptoms at follow-up was fully mediated by change in mindfulness and rumination, the regression coefficient for the mindfulness intervention as a predictor of depressive symptoms at follow-up would be zero. Sensitivity analyses were conducted to ensure the pattern of findings were robust and not explained by other factors. Sensitivity analyses included analyses that did not include covariates. The results conclude with exploratory within-groups one-tailed Pearson’s correlations of the outcome (depressive symptoms), process variables (mindfulness and rumination) and practice. All data were analysed using SPSS version 19.0. Results Five hundred and twenty two young people participated in the study; 256 in the intervention schools, and 266 in the control schools. Young people attended one of twelve schools; six of which were private schools, and six of which were publicly funded, with one selective grammar school in both the intervention and control group. The schools published MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 18 exam results evidenced varied levels of academic attainment across the sample, and a small cohort of young people in special needs schools was included in each condition. Ninety-six percent of participants attended six or more sessions of the nine-session intervention and attrition from the programme was lower than 1%. Seven trained teachers, of whom 6 were male, delivered the intervention. Teachers had an average of 12 years of general teaching experience, and an average of 1.8 years experience delivering the mindfulness intervention. The two course developers had the longest experience of delivering the intervention, having taught if for 3 years since developing the programme. Most of the schools (5/6 experimental schools) delivered the programme as a universal intervention; therefore these young people took part in the programme during the regular school timetable. However one class (N = 26) participated in the programme voluntarily during lunch break. The characteristics of participants in each group at baseline are reported in Table 1. The intervention arm contained a higher percentage of female participants than the control arm (χ² (1) = 15.00, p = <.001), and the sample as a whole contained fewer females than males in both the control and intervention arm (approximately two thirds of the sample were male). There were no significant differences between the intervention and control group on age, ethnicity, depressive symptoms and rumination, however baseline levels of mindfulness were significantly higher in the control group than the intervention group t(424) = 2.26, p = .024. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 19 Table 1 Baseline characteristics of participants in the MiS intervention and control group. Variable MiS Intervention Control (n=256) (n=259) Female, % 36.5 19.2 Age in years at baseline, mean (SD) 14.9 (1.5) 14.7 (1.4) White, % 75.6 75.1 Asian, % 9.6 9.8 Chinese, % 1.6 3.5 Black, % 4.4 3.5 Mixed, % 4.8 5.2 Other, % 4.0 2.9 Ethnicity Depressive symptoms (CES-D), M (SD) 15.1 (4.0) 14.95 (3.7) Mindfulness (CAMS-R), M (SD) 30.5 (5.3) 31.6 (4.8) Rumination (Rumination CERQ subscale), M (SD) 10.1 (3.6) 10.1 (3.7) Note. MiS = Mindfulness in schools; CES-D = Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; CAMS-R = Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised; CERQ = Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. a Sample sizes range from 228 to 256 in the intervention arm and 169 to 259 in the control arm. Between-groups comparisons of the effect of the MiS programme on depressive symptoms The primary hypothesis for this study was that young people who participated in the mindfulness programme would experience a reduction in depressive symptoms following the intervention relative to controls, and that this would be maintained at follow-up. This prediction was supported; intervention participants experienced significantly fewer depressive symptoms post-intervention compared to control participants, even after controlling for gender, age, ethnicity, and depressive symptoms at baseline F(1, 375) = 15.78. This reduction was maintained MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 20 at three-month follow-up; intervention participants continued to show lower levels of depressive symptoms at follow-up relative to control participants, after controlling for gender, age, ethnicity and baseline depressive symptoms F(1, 378) = 10.22. Table 2 displays unadjusted and adjusted means including confidence intervals and p values for the intervention and control group at postintervention and follow-up. Table 2 Mean differences in outcome and process variables between study arms at post-intervention and three-month follow-up. Mean (SD) for trial arms Outcomes Intervention Control (I) (C) Adjusted Unadjusted Mean Mean 95 % difference difference Confidence (I – C) (I – C) Interval P value Post-intervention Depressive symptoms (CES-D) 14.3 (3.5) 15.4 (4.0) -1.1 -1.45 -2.18 to -0.74 <.001* Mindfulness (CAMS-R) 31.0 (5.5) 31.2 (5.2) -0.2 0.48 -0.74 to 1.35 .565 Rumination (CERQ subscale) 11.0 (3.4) 10.4 (3.6) 0.6 .357 -0.38 to 1.09 .339 Depressive symptoms (CES-D) 14.6 (3.7) 15.6 (4.6) -1 -1.25 -2.03 to -0.48 .002* Mindfulness (CAMS-R) 31.3 (5.4) 31.5 (5.8) -0.2 1.02 0.05 to 1.99 .039* Rumination (CERQ subscale) 10.1 (3.6) 10.9 (3.6) -0.8 -0.94 -1.64 to -0.24 .008* 3 month follow-up Note. CES-D = Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; CAMS-R = Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised; CERQ = Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. a * = p < .05. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS b 21 At post intervention, the sample sizes range from 215 to 234 in the intervention arm and 140 to 147 in the control arm. c At 3 month follow-up, the sample sizes for range from 218 to 237 in the intervention arm and 146 to 149 in the control arm. Between-groups comparisons of the effect of the MiS programme on mindfulness and rumination The second hypothesis for this study was that young people would learn to respond more adaptively, that is their tendency to use mindfulness would be enhanced, and their tendency to ruminate reduced following the programme, and this effect would be maintained at follow-up. In between-groups analyses of mindfulness, contrary to predictions, an ANCOVA found no main effect of experimental group on levels of mindfulness post-intervention, after accounting for gender, age, ethnicity and baseline levels of mindfulness, F(1, 372) = .33. That is, levels of mindfulness were not altered immediately following the intervention. However at threemonth follow-up, after adjusting for the same covariates, there was a main effect of experimental condition on mindfulness, F(1, 380) = 4.72. The adjusted mean difference reported in table 2 suggests participants who received the intervention reported a significantly greater propensity to be mindful at three-month follow-up compared to control participants. In between-groups analyses of rumination, also contrary to predictions, an ANCOVA found no main effect of experimental group on levels of rumination post-intervention after accounting for gender, age, ethnicity and baseline levels of rumination, F(1, 349) = .916. This suggests that like mindfulness, rumination was not altered immediately after the intervention. However at three-month follow-up, after adjusting for the same covariates, there was a MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 22 significant main effect of experimental condition on rumination F(1, 358) = 7.04. The adjusted mean difference shown in table 2 suggests participants who received the intervention reported a significantly reduced tendency to ruminate at follow-up compared to controls. In summary, after controlling for covariates, neither process variable (mindfulness or rumination) appeared to have been changed immediately following the intervention, however as predicted, by follow-up intervention participants were more mindful, and ruminated less. Mindfulness and rumination: Mediators of change in depressive symptoms? The conditions for mediation were not met in this study, as depressive symptoms reduced post-intervention, which occurred prior to changes in mindfulness and rumination (process variables) at follow-up. Therefore it was not possible to carry out the planned withingroups mediation analyses (Kazdin, 2007), as the temporal sequence of events could not be established. Sensitivity Analyses Analyses excluding covariates found the same pattern as the main findings, aside from mindfulness, where not including covariates led to a non-significant finding at follow-up; that is levels of mindfulness were not significantly different at follow-up without any covariates (see Appendix A for details). Exploratory analyses of outcome, process variables, and practice Intervention participants reported levels of general mindfulness practice since the end of the intervention to be 3.93 (SD = 0.95), on a six-point scale (from 1 = never to 6 = almost every MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 23 day). A score of three corresponds to practicing mindfulness two to three times a week, and a score of four about once a week, suggesting that on average, participants who received the intervention practiced about once a week. These descriptive statistics suggest that the mindfulness exercises were used reasonably regularly although not very frequently by participants after the intervention came to an end. To better understand the relationships that variables had with one another and particularly to get a sense of how practice affected depressive symptoms and process variables (mindfulness and rumination), within-groups one-tailed Pearson’s correlations were carried out (see Table 3). Cohen (1988, 1992) suggested that r = .10 represents a small correlation, r = .30 represents a medium correlation, and r = .50 represents a large correlation. Table 3 shows within-groups correlations between practice and depressive symptoms post-intervention and at follow-up for depressive symptoms, mindfulness, and rumination. Postintervention, depressive symptoms were negatively correlated with practice. Practice was positively correlated with mindfulness post-intervention, but not significantly correlated with rumination post-intervention. At three-month follow-up, practice was negatively correlated with depressive symptoms, and positively correlated with mindfulness, and counter to what theory would predict, rumination. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 24 Table 3 Within-groups Pearson’s correlations between practice, depressive symptoms, mindfulness and rumination for intervention participants at post-intervention and follow-up. Mindfulness Practice Post-intervention Depressive symptoms Mindfulness Rumination r = -.192** r = .227 *** r = 0.104 Follow-up Depressive symptoms Mindfulness Rumination r = -.180** r = .223 *** r = 0.181** Note. ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 Discussion This non-randomised controlled feasibility study sought to determine whether a universal intervention designed to teach mindfulness to young people would reduce the extent to which they experienced depressive symptoms immediately after the intervention and at follow-up, during the stressful exam period, compared to young people in matched groups who did not receive the intervention. Further, it aimed to identify what factors might contribute to such a reduction, and predicted that the intervention would increase young people’s propensity to be mindful, and reduce their tendency to ruminate. It was predicted that change in depressive symptoms post-intervention would be mediated by increased mindfulness and reduced rumination. Finally, it explored relationships between the extent to which young people practiced MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 25 mindfulness and outcome (depressive symptoms) and process variables (mindfulness and rumination). In relationship to the primary outcome for the study, depressive symptoms, the main hypothesis was supported. As predicted, participants reported experiencing fewer depressive symptoms following the intervention than participants who did not receive the intervention in all analyses. Despite the three-month follow-up occurring during a period hypothesised to be highly stressful (the school exam period), the reduction in depressive symptoms in intervention participants was maintained. This suggests that even during an academically challenging time, participants still experienced benefit from the intervention. These findings resonate with a recent study of MBSR that reduced depressive symptoms in adolescents presenting with mental health problems (Biegel et al., 2009), however the present study differed in that it measured the impact of a universal mindfulness intervention. This study therefore contributes to the existing research that mindfulness can reduce depressive symptoms in a clinical population of young people, with the finding that mindfulness can reduce depressive symptoms in a non-clinical population of young people. The findings are in contrast to those of recent large-scale study of a universal CBT intervention that did not produce change in depressive symptoms in at-risk adolescents, if anything, intervention participants experienced a slight increase in depressive symptoms, which the authors attributed to greater awareness of depressive symptoms (Stallard et al., 2012). In this study, it could also be expected that mindfulness practice led to a greater awareness of depressive symptoms, yet a reduction in depressive symptoms still occurred, suggesting this intervention offered benefits over and above heightened awareness of existing symptoms. One possible explanation for the difference in findings between these studies is that mindfulness may target the mechanisms that maintain sub-threshold depressive symptoms more effectively than CBT. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 26 Although CBT is an established and effective therapy for the treatment of major depression (NICE, 2009), remarkably little is known as to how it effects change (Kazdin, 2007). It is not implausible that CBT and mindfulness work through different mechanisms. Although CBT and mindfulness programmes for depressive symptoms share the same aim, they endeavour to achieve it through very different means: CBT emphasises changing the content of thoughts, whereas mindfulness emphasises changing the relationship with thoughts. It could be the case that changing the relationship with thoughts may be more effective than changing the content of thoughts in reducing sub-threshold depressive symptoms. Further research that utilises designs that enable investigation of mechanisms of change is required to establish what accounts for effectiveness in both mindfulness and CBT interventions. The second hypothesis for this study, that the universal mindfulness intervention would enhance young people’s capacity for mindfulness and reduce their tendency to ruminate, was supported to some extent. Contrary to what was predicted, no changes were observed in mindfulness or rumination immediately following the intervention. However by follow-up, young people who had received the intervention reported significantly lower levels of rumination than control participants, as predicted. As a result of changes in mindfulness and rumination occurring at follow-up, after the observed change in depressive symptoms post-intervention, it was not possible to compute the meditational analyses (Kazdin, 2007). Therefore it was not possible to test the third hypothesis; that change in depressive symptoms occurred as a result of increasing young people’s propensity for mindfulness and reducing their tendency to ruminate. The reported increase in mindfulness, and reduction in rumination is consistent with research on MBSR with predominantly non-depressed adults (Deyo et al., 2009), suggesting mindfulness interventions may well benefit young people in the same way as adults; by enhancing MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 27 mindfulness and reducing rumination. The delayed effect on rumination parallels the findings of a study of MBSR with a non-clinical sample of adults (Robins et al., 2012), which reported significant reductions in rumination at two-month follow-up but not post-intervention, which the authors attributed to the effect of practice. There may be a number of explanations for the lack of change in mindfulness and rumination at post-intervention, and subsequent change at follow-up. One possibility is that the intervention achieved a reduction in depressive symptoms via a different mechanism (or mechanisms) than those that were hypothesised (mindfulness and rumination). That is, depressive symptoms may have reduced post-intervention due to an unmeasured third variable, such as social support from teachers. This is a possibility, however it is perhaps unlikely that a factor such as social support would have maintained a reduction in depressive symptoms threemonths after the intervention, particularly during a stressful time. One possibility is that this gain was maintained as a result of the intervention impacting on a factor known to affect depressive symptoms, namely, rumination (Burwell & Shirk, 2007; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991). The findings bear this out as not only was the reduction in depressive symptoms maintained, but by follow-up, levels of mindfulness had increased and rumination had reduced in participants who received the intervention, suggesting that the intervention had exerted some effect on the proposed mechanisms. However the data in this study are not sufficient to conclude what factors accounted for the reduction in and maintenance of levels of depressive symptoms. Future research is needed to address the relationship between the observed differences in levels of mindfulness and rumination, and depressive symptoms. In order to better understand the mechanisms of change, future studies should collect data at multiple time points to ensure change in mediating variables is detected prior to change in depressive symptoms. In addition, it MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 28 is recommended that a range of measures are used in the evaluation of complex interventions (Craig et al., 2008), in order that a range of possible mediators can be accounted for, such as social support from teachers. A stronger design would include an active control condition (e.g. psychoeducation sessions), in order that non-specific effects of receiving an intervention could be controlled for, to determine the effects of the intervention that are uniquely attributable to mindfulness. Exploratory correlational findings suggest avenues for future research; notably, the significant positive association between the extent to which young people practiced mindfulness, and their levels of depressive symptoms both after the intervention and at follow-up. Potentially, this could suggest that the more young people practiced mindfulness, the greater the reduction they experienced in depressive symptoms, however as it is not possible to imply causality with correlational data, it is also possible that young people who were experiencing more depressive symptoms practiced mindfulness less. There was also a significant positive association between practice and mindfulness at both post-intervention and follow-up. Theoretically it could be expected that the more young people practiced mindfulness, the greater the improvement in their ability to be mindful, although due to the correlational nature of the data, causality cannot be implied. The findings for rumination were less clear; no significant association was found between practice and rumination immediately after the intervention, and a positive association was found between mindfulness and rumination at follow-up. This finding is anomalous to what theory would predict, as increased practice would be expected to reduce rumination. Further research is needed to clarify the relationship between mindfulness practice and rumination. Despite the inability from these data to imply causality, it seems likely that practice enhanced the extent to which young people benefited from the programme in terms of reducing MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 29 depressive symptoms and increasing mindfulness, as changing habitual ways of responding to depressogenic thoughts and feelings is likely to require considerable effort and repetition of new ways of responding, thus effects may not be immediate and would likely depend on practice. Indeed, recent studies of mindfulness programmes suggest practice is important to outcome in young people (Biegel et al., 2009; Huppert & Johnson, 2010), and adults (Ramel, Goldin, Carmona, & McQuaid, 2004). Additionally, studies of MBSR with adults have reported that intervention effects increase over time, in terms of enhanced mindfulness and reduced rumination, speaking to the importance of continued implementation of mindfulness to outcome (Shapiro, Oman, Thoresen, Plante, & Flinders, 2008; Robins et al., 2012). Future studies should measure practice at several time-points to establish whether practice does indeed predict better outcome, and to determine what level of practice is required to achieve change. Comparison of participants randomised to groups that promote either regular or infrequent practice, would enable researchers to draw causal conclusions about the effects of practice on outcome, and control for third variables that might otherwise explain the association between practice and better outcome. Strengths, limitations and future directions The present feasibility study had a number of strengths. It examined a universal mindfulness intervention in a real world context delivered by those that would provide the intervention in practice: teachers who were either the developers of the intervention or had been trained by them. The sample was large, diverse and matched on key variables, including individuals from private and state funded schools drawn from an age group at an important developmental window for developing depressive symptoms (12-16 years). Follow-up occurred MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 30 at a stressful time, the summer exam period, theoretically providing a good test of the intervention’s effectiveness. As this was a feasibility study, groups were matched, but not randomised to condition. Although this was accounted for in analyses by controlling for covariates, the non-randomised design limits the extent to which the observed effects can be attributed to the intervention and not individual differences occurring between participants or condition. An example of this is the greater proportion of females in the sample as a whole and in the intervention group compared to controls. Randomisation would ensure gender distribution was even between conditions. A second limitation is the relatively limited range of self-report measures. Future evaluation studies should measure a wider range of variables, to include variables that may influence outcome such as history of depressive episodes and anti-depressant use, other potential outcomes of the intervention such as improved psychosocial functioning, and other potential mechanisms of change, such as social support. This would enable firmer conclusions to be drawn as to the effects of the intervention, and the mechanisms through which these effects occur. Selfreport data would be enriched by inclusion of other means of data collection; teacher or parent observations, diagnostic interviews by assessors blind to condition, and physiological measures (e.g. in response to mood induction tasks). Following this feasibility study, evaluation trials are now required that employ randomised controlled designs with large and diverse samples, and a wide range of psychometrically robust self-report measures, complimented by additional means of measurement. Measures taken at multiple time-points will allow the teasing apart of the mechanisms of change, which is essential to allow future interventions to be tailored to maximise effectiveness. Longer follow-up periods are needed to assess whether effects are maintained over MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 31 time, and to answer the question of whether reducing depressive symptoms with an intervention such as this does in fact reduce the risk of developing depression later in life. Conclusions This non-randomised controlled feasibility study found as predicted that a universal mindfulness intervention specifically designed for young people and delivered by trained teachers in schools resulted in lower levels of depressive symptoms in intervention participants compared to matched controls after the intervention, an effect that was maintained three months later during an academically stressful time. It also found evidence that by follow-up the intervention had increased participants’ tendency to be mindful and reduced their tendency to ruminate compared to controls, suggesting the intervention impacted on the mechanisms predicted to reduce depressive symptoms. A randomised controlled study is now required that is designed to determine the mechanisms that account for change in depressive symptoms; with longer follow-up periods, a wider range of measured variables and a range of measurement methods. Further research will allow effective preventative interventions to be implemented that reduce depressive symptoms in young people and offset the associated distress, disability and risk of later depression. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 32 References Aalto-Setälä, T., Marttunen, M., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Poikolainen, K., & Lönnqvist, J. (2002). Depressive symptoms in adolescence as predictors of early adulthood depressive disorders in maladjustment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1235-1237. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. Biegel, G.M, Brown, K.W., Shapiro, S.L, & Schubert, C. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for the treatment of adolescent psychiatric outpatients: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 855-866. Bracke, P., Levecque, K., & Van de Velde, S. (2008). The psychometric properties of the CES-D 8 depression inventory and the estimation of cross-national differences in the true prevalence of depression. Unpublished manuscript. Broderick, P. C., & Korteland, C. (2004). A prospective study of rumination and depression in early adolescence. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9, 1359-1045. Burwell, R. A., & Shirk, S. R. (2007). Subtypes of rumination in adolescence: Associations between brooding, reflection, depressive symptoms, and coping. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 56-65. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 33 Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences (2nd ed.). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159. Coryell, W., Solomon, D., Leon, A., Fiedorowicz, J. G., Schettler, P., Judd, L., & Keller, M. (2009). Does major depressive disorder change with age? Psychological Medicine, 39, 1689-1695. Craig, P., Dieppe, P., Macintryre, S., Mitchie, S., Nazareth, I., Petticrew, M. (2008). Developing and evaluating complex interventions: The new Medical Research Council guidance. BMJ, 337, 979-983. Last accessed 3rd May 2013. Doi: 10.1136/bmj.a1655. Cuijpers, P., van Straten, A., Smit, F., Mihalopoulos, C., & Beekman, A. (2008). Preventing the onset of depressive disorders: A meta-analytic review of psychological interventions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1272-1280. Deyo, M., Wilson, K. A., Ong, J., & Koopman, C. (2009). Mindfulness and rumination: Does mindfulness training lead to reductions in the ruminative thinking associated with depression? EXPLORE: The Journal of Science and Healing, 5, 265-271. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 34 Durlak, J. A., & DuPre, E. P. (2008). Implementation matters: A review of research on the influence of implementation on program outcomes and the factors affecting implementation. American Journal of Community Psychology, 41, 327-350. Feldman, G., Hayes, A, Kumar, S., Greeson, J. & Laurencceau, J. (2007). Mindfulness and emotional regulation: the development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). Journal of Psychopathological Behavioural Assessment, 29, 177-190. Fendrich, M., Weissman, M. M., & Warner, V. (1990). Screening for Depressive Disorder in Children and Adolescents - Validating the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale for Children. American Journal of Epidemiology, 131, 538-551. Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007). The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; Psychometric features and prospective relationships with depression and anxiety in adults. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 141-149. Ge, X., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H. (1994). Trajectories of stressful life events and depressive symptoms during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30, 467-483. Harnett, P. H., & Dawe, S. (2012). Review: The contribution of mindfulness-based therapies for children and families and proposed conceptual integration. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 17, 195-208. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 35 Hennelly, S. (2011). The immediate and sustained effects of the .b mindfulness programme on adolescents’ social and emotional well-being and academic functioning. (Unpublished Masters Dissertation). Oxford Brookes University, Oxford. Huppert, F. A., & Johnson, D. M. (2010). A controlled trial of mindfulness training in schools: The importance of practice for an impact on well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 264-274. Judd, L. L. (1997). The clinical course of unipolar major depressive disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 989-991. Judd, L. L., Akiskal, H. S., Zeller, P. J., Paulus, M., Leon, A. C., Maser, J. D., . . . Keller, M. B. (2000a). Psychosocial disability during the long-term course of unipolar major depressive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 375-380. Judd, L. L., Paulus, M. J., Schettler, P. J., Akiskal, H. S., Endicott, J., Leon, A. C., . . . Keller, M. B. (2000b). Does incomplete recovery from first lifetime major depressive episode herald a chronic course of illness? American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1501-1504. Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 1-27. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 36 Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: How to cope with stress, pain and illness using mindfulness meditation. New York: Delacorte. Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology; Science and Practice, 10, 144-156. Kennard, B. D., Emslie, G. J., Mayes, T. L., & Hughes, J. L. (2006). Relapse and recurrence in paediatric depression. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 15, 1057-1079. Kumar, S. M. (2005). Grieving mindfully: A compassionate and spiritual guide to coping with loss. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. Kuyken, W., Weare, K., Ukoumunne, O., Lewis, R., Motton, N., Burnett, R., . . . Huppert, F. (2013). Effectiveness of the .b Mindfulness in Schools Program: A non-randomized controlled feasibility study. Manuscript accepted for publication. Kuyken, W., Byford, S., Taylor, R. S., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., . . . Mullan, E. (2008). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to prevent relapse in recurrent depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 966-978. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 37 Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Taylor, R. S., Byford, S., . . . Dalgleish, T. (2010). How does mindfulness-based cognitive therapy work? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 1-8. Labelle, L. E., Campbell, T. S., & Carlson, L. E. (2010). Mindfulness-based stress reduction in oncology: Evaluating mindfulness and rumination as mediators of change in depressive symptoms. Mindfulness, 1, 28-40. Lewinsohn, P. M., Solomon, A., Seeley, J. R., Zeiss, A. (2000). Clinical implications of “subthreshold” depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 345-351. Mathers, C. D., & Loncar, D. (2006). Projections of global mortality and burden of disease from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Med, 3: e442. Last accessed on 3rd May 2013. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0030442. Murray, C. J. L., & Lopez, A. D. (Eds.). (1996). The global burden of disease. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. NICE (2009). Depression: A NICE guideline on the treatment and management of depression in adults (updated edition). Retrieved from http://guidance.nice.org.uk/CG90/Guidance on 26th April 2013. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 38 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrom, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115-121. Pine, D. S., Cohen, E., Cohen, P., & Brook, J. (1999). Adolescent depressive symptoms as predictors of adult depression: Moodiness or mood disorder? American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 133-135. Radloff, L. S. (1991). The use of the center for epidemiological studies depression scale in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 149-166. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. Ramel, W., Goldin, P. R., Carmona, P. E., & McQuaid, J. R. (2004). The effects of mindfulness meditation on cognitive processes and affect in patients with past depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 433-455. Saluja, G., Iachan, R., Scheidt, P. C., Overpeck, M. D., Sun, W., & Giedd, J. N. (2004). Prevalence of and risk factors for depressive symptoms among young adolescents. Archives of Paediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 158, 760-765. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 39 Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2013). Mindfulness-based Cognitive Therapy for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse. New York: The Guildford Press. Shankman, S. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Klein, D. N., Small, J. W., Seeley, J. R., & Altman, S. E. (2009). Subthreshold conditions as precursors for full syndrome disorders: A 15-year longitudinal study of multiple diagnostic classes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1485-1494. Shapiro, S. L., Oman, D., Thoresen, C. E., Plante, T. G., & Flinders, T. (2008). Cultivating mindfulness: Effects on well-being. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, 840-862. Stallard, P., Sayal, K., Phillips, R., Taylor, J. A., Spears, M., Anderson, R., . . . Montgomery, A. A. (2012). Classroom-based cognitive behavioural therapy in reducing symptoms of depression in high risk adolescents: Pragmatic cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ, 345, e6058. Last accessed 3rd May 2013. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e6058. Steffick, D. E. (2000). Documentation of affective functioning measures in the Health and Retirement Study (Tech. Rep. No. HRS/AHEAD). Ann Arbor, MI: Survey Research Center, University of Michigan. Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615-623. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 40 Thompson, A. H. (2008). Younger onset of depression is associated with greater suicidal intent. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43, 538-544. Weare, K., & Nind, M. (2011). Mental health promotion and problem prevention in schools: what does the evidence say? Health Promotion International, 26, 129-169. Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Duggan, D. S., Shah, D., Brennan, K., . . . Russell, I. T. (2012). Pre-adult onset and patterns of suicidality in patients with a history of recurrent depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 138, 173-179. Zisook, S., Lesser, I., Stewart, J. W., Wisniewski, S. R., Balasubramani, G. K., Fava, M., . . . Rush, A. J. (2007). Effect of age at onset on the course of major depressive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1539-1546. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 41 Appendix A: Extended results Sensitivity Analyses Analyses without covariates To investigate whether controlling for covariates in the main analysis accounted for the pattern of findings, independent groups t-tests were run to test the main hypotheses, that is whether depressive symptoms, and rumination decreased, and mindfulness increased postintervention and whether these changes were maintained at follow-up in intervention participants compared to controls. As was found in the main analyses, an independent groups t-test revealed that participants who had received the mindfulness intervention experienced significantly fewer depressive symptoms than control participants t(445) = 3.28, p = .001, a difference that was maintained at follow-up t(461) = .37, p = .012. In line with the main findings, independent groups t-tests found no significant differences in levels of mindfulness post-intervention t(442) = 0.56, p = .58, and unlike the main findings, no significant difference in mindfulness was found at follow-up t(463) = 2.51, p = .71. The lack of change in mindfulness at follow-up when not controlling for covariates could well be a result of the finding that control participants scored higher at baseline on mindfulness, thus intervention participants may well have increased their level of mindfulness, but compared to a sample of young people who were already more mindful, this difference may not have been marked enough to reach significance. In relation to rumination the results of the main findings were confirmed by independent groups t-tests which found no significant differences post-intervention in levels of rumination between intervention and control groups t(429) = -1.68, p = .09, but by follow-up MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 42 found intervention participants did ruminate significantly less than control participants t(455) = 2.24, p = .025. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 43 Appendix B: Instructions for Authors For Behaviour Research and Therapy Accessed online 2nd April 2013: http://www.elsevier.com/journals/behaviour-research-andtherapy/0005-7967/guide-for-authors Introduction Behaviour Research and Therapy encompasses all of what is commonly referred to as cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT). The focus is on the following: theoretical and experimental analyses of psychopathological processes with direct implications for prevention and treatment; the development and evaluation of empirically-supported interventions; predictors, moderators and mechanisms of behaviour change; and dissemination and implementation of evidence-based treatments to general clinical practice. In addition to traditional clinical disorders, the scope of the journal also includes behavioural medicine. The journal will not consider manuscripts dealing primarily with measurement, psychometric analyses, and personality assessment. The Editor and Associate Editors will make an initial determination of whether or not submissions fall within the scope of the journal and/or are of sufficient merit and importance to warrant full review. Contact details Any questions regarding your submission should be addressed to the Editor in Chief: Professor G. T. Wilson Psychological Clinic at Gordon Road Rutgers The State University of New Jersey 41C Gordon Road Piscataway New Jersey 088548067 USA Email: brat@rci.rutgers.edu Before you Begin Ethics in Publishing For information on Ethics in publishing and Ethical guidelines for journal publication see MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 44 http://www.elsevier.com/publishingethics and http://www.elsevier.com/ethicalguidelines. Conflict of Interest All authors are requested to disclose any actual or potential conflict of interest including any financial, personal or other relationships with other people or organizations within three years of beginning the submitted work that could inappropriately influence, or be perceived to influence, their work. See also http://www.elsevier.com/conflictsofinterest. Submission Declaration Submission of an article implies that the work described has not been published previously (except in the form of an abstract or as part of a published lecture or academic thesis or as an electronic preprint, see http://www.elsevier.com/postingpolicy), that it is not under consideration for publication elsewhere, that its publication is approved by all authors and tacitly or explicitly by the responsible authorities where the work was carried out, and that, if accepted, it will not be published elsewhere including electronically in the same form, in English or in any other language, without the written consent of the copyright-holder. Changes to Authorship This policy concerns the addition, deletion, or rearrangement of author names in the authorship of accepted manuscripts: Before the accepted manuscript is published in an online issue: Requests to add or remove an author, or to rearrange the author names, must be sent to the Journal Manager from the corresponding author of the accepted manuscript and must include: (a) the reason the name should be added or removed, or the author names rearranged and (b) written MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 45 confirmation (e-mail, fax, letter) from all authors that they agree with the addition, removal or rearrangement. In the case of addition or removal of authors, this includes confirmation from the author being added or removed. Requests that are not sent by the corresponding author will be forwarded by the Journal Manager to the corresponding author, who must follow the procedure as described above. Note that: (1) Journal Managers will inform the Journal Editors of any such requests and (2) publication of the accepted manuscript in an online issue is suspended until authorship has been agreed. After the accepted manuscript is published in an online issue: Any requests to add, delete, or rearrange author names in an article published in an online issue will follow the same policies as noted above and result in a corrigendum. Copyright Upon acceptance of an article, authors will be asked to complete a 'Journal Publishing Agreement' (for more information on this and copyright see http://www.elsevier.com/copyright). Acceptance of the agreement will ensure the widest possible dissemination of information. An email will be sent to the corresponding author confirming receipt of the manuscript together with a 'Journal Publishing Agreement' form or a link to the online version of this agreement. Subscribers may reproduce tables of contents or prepare lists of articles including abstracts for internal circulation within their institutions. Permission of the Publisher is required for resale or distribution outside the institution and for all other derivative works, including compilations and translations (please consult http://www.elsevier.com/permissions). If excerpts from other copyrighted works are included, the author(s) must obtain written permission from the copyright owners and credit the source(s) in the article. Elsevier has preprinted forms for use by authors in these cases: please consult http://www.elsevier.com/permissions. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 46 Retained Author Rights As an author you (or your employer or institution) retain certain rights; for details you are referred to: http://www.elsevier.com/authorsrights. Role of the Funding Source You are requested to identify who provided financial support for the conduct of the research and/or preparation of the article and to briefly describe the role of the sponsor(s), if any, in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the article for publication. If the funding source(s) had no such involvement then this should be stated. Please see http://www.elsevier.com/funding. Funding Body Agreements and Policies Elsevier has established agreements and developed policies to allow authors whose articles appear in journals published by Elsevier, to comply with potential manuscript archiving requirements as specified as conditions of their grant awards. To learn more about existing agreements and policies please visit http://www.elsevier.com/fundingbodies. Open Access This journal offers authors a choice in publishing their research: Open Access; articles are freely available to both subscribers and the wider public with permitted reuse. An Open Access publication fee is payable by authors or their research funder. Subscription; articles are made available to subscribers as well as developing countries and patient groups through our MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 47 access programs (http://www.elsevier.com/access). No Open Access publication fee. Your publication choice will have no effect on the peer review process or acceptance of your submission. All articles published Open Access will be immediately and permanently free for everyone to read and download. Permitted reuse is defined by your choice of one of the following Creative Commons user licenses: Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY): lets others distribute and copy the article, to create extracts, abstracts, and other revised versions, adaptations or derivative works of or from an article (such as a translation), to include in a collective work (such as an anthology), to text or data mine the article, even for commercial purposes, as long as they credit the author(s), do not represent the author as endorsing their adaptation of the article, and do not modify the article in such a way as to damage the author's honor or reputation. Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike (CC-BY-NCSA): for non-commercial purposes, lets others distribute and copy the article, to create extracts, abstracts and other revised versions, adaptations or derivative works of or from an article (such as a translation), to include in a collective work (such as an anthology), to text and data mine the article, as long as they credit the author(s), do not represent the author as endorsing their adaptation of the article, do not modify the article in such a way as to damage the author's honor or reputation, and license their new adaptations or creations under identical terms (CC-BY-NCSA). Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs (CC-BY-NC-ND): for noncommercial purposes, lets others distribute and copy the article, and to include in a collective work (such as an anthology), as long as they credit the author(s) and provided they do not alter or modify the article. To provide Open Access, this journal has a publication fee which needs to be met by the authors or their research funders for each article published Open Access. The publication fee MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 48 for this journal is $3000, excluding taxes. Learn more about Elsevier's pricing policy: http://www.elsevier.com/openaccesspricing. Language (Usage and Editing Services) Please write your text in good English (American or British usage is accepted, but not a mixture of these). Authors who feel their English language manuscript may require editing to eliminate possible grammatical or spelling errors and to conform to correct scientific English may wish to use the English Language Editing service available from Elsevier's WebShop http://webshop.elsevier.com/languageediting/ or visit our customer support site http://support.elsevier.com for more information. Submission Submission to this journal proceeds totally online and you will be guided stepwise through the creation and uploading of your files. The system automatically converts source files to a single PDF file of the article, which is used in the peer-review process. Please note that even though manuscript source files are converted to PDF files at submission for the review process, these source files are needed for further processing after acceptance. All correspondence, including notification of the Editor's decision and requests for revision, takes place by e-mail removing the need for a paper trail. Submit your article Please submit your article via http://ees.elsevier.com/brat/ MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 49 Preparation Article structure Subdivision - Unnumbered sections Divide your article into clearly defined sections. Each subsection is given a brief heading. Each heading should appear on its own separate line. Subsections should be used as much as possible when cross-referencing text: refer to the subsection by heading as opposed to simply 'the text'. Appendices If there is more than one appendix, they should be identified as A, B, etc. Formulae and equations in appendices should be given separate numbering: Eq. (A.1), Eq. (A.2), etc.; in a subsequent appendix, Eq. (B.1) and so on. Similarly for tables and figures: Table A.1; Fig. A.1, etc. Essential title page information Title Concise and informative. Titles are often used in information-retrieval systems. Avoid abbreviations and formulae where possible. Author names and affiliations Where the family name may be ambiguous (e.g., a double name), please indicate this clearly. Present the authors' affiliation addresses (where the actual work was done) below the names. Indicate all affiliations with a lower-case superscript letter immediately after the author's name and in front of the appropriate address. Provide the full postal address of each affiliation, including the country name and, if available, the e-mail address of each author. Corresponding author MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 50 Clearly indicate who will handle correspondence at all stages of refereeing and publication, also post-publication. Ensure that phone numbers (with country and area code) are provided in addition to the e-mail address and the complete postal address. Contact details must be kept up to date by the corresponding author. Present/permanent address If an author has moved since the work described in the article was done, or was visiting at the time, a 'Present address' (or 'Permanent address') may be indicated as a footnote to that author's name. The address at which the author actually did the work must be retained as the main, affiliation address. Superscript Arabic numerals are used for such footnotes. Abstract A concise and factual abstract is required with a maximum length of 200 words. The abstract should state briefly the purpose of the research, the principal results and major conclusions. An abstract is often presented separately from the article, so it must be able to stand alone. For this reason, References should be avoided, but if essential, then cite the author(s) and year(s). Also, non-standard or uncommon abbreviations should be avoided, but if essential they must be defined at their first mention in the abstract itself. Graphical abstract A Graphical abstract is optional and should summarize the contents of the article in a concise, pictorial form designed to capture the attention of a wide readership online. Authors must provide images that clearly represent the work described in the article. Graphical abstracts should be submitted as a separate file in the online submission system. Image size: Please provide an image with a minimum of 531 × 1328 pixels (h × w) or proportionally more. The image should be readable at a size of 5 × 13 cm using a regular screen resolution of 96 dpi. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 51 Preferred file types: TIFF, EPS, PDF or MS Office files. See http://www.elsevier.com/graphicalabstracts for examples. Authors can make use of Elsevier's Illustration and Enhancement service to ensure the best presentation of their images also in accordance with all technical requirements: Illustration Service. Highlights Highlights are mandatory for this journal. They consist of a short collection of bullet points that convey the core findings of the article and should be submitted in a separate file in the online submission system. Please use 'Highlights' in the file name and include 3 to 5 bullet points (maximum 85 characters, including spaces, per bullet point). See http://www.elsevier.com/highlights for examples. Keywords Immediately after the abstract, provide a maximum of 6 keywords, to be chosen from the APA list of index descriptors. These keywords will be used for indexing purposes. Abbreviations Define abbreviations that are not standard in this field in a footnote to be placed on the first page of the article. Such abbreviations that are unavoidable in the abstract must be defined at their first mention there, as well as in the footnote. Ensure consistency of abbreviations throughout the article. Acknowledgements Collate acknowledgements in a separate section at the end of the article before the references and do not, therefore, include them on the title page, as a footnote to the title or otherwise. List here those individuals who provided help during the research (e.g., providing MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 52 language help, writing assistance or proof reading the article, etc.). Shorter communications This option is designed to allow publication of research reports that are not suitable for publication as regular articles. Shorter Communications are appropriate for articles with a specialized focus or of particular didactic value. Manuscripts should be between 3000-5000 words, and must not exceed the upper word limit. This limit includes the abstract, text, and references, but not the title page, tables and figures. Artwork Electronic artwork General points Make sure you use uniform lettering and sizing of your original artwork. Embed the used fonts if the application provides that option. Aim to use the following fonts in your illustrations: Arial, Courier, Times New Roman, Symbol, or use fonts that look similar. Number the illustrations according to their sequence in the text. Use a logical naming convention for your artwork files. Provide captions to illustrations separately. Size the illustrations close to the desired dimensions of the printed version. Submit each illustration as a separate file. A detailed guide on electronic artwork is available on our website: http://www.elsevier.com/artworkinstructions You are urged to visit this site; some excerpts from the detailed information are given here. Formats If your electronic artwork is created in a Microsoft Office application (Word, PowerPoint, Excel) then please supply 'as is' in the native document format. Regardless of the application MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 53 used other than Microsoft Office, when your electronic artwork is finalized, please 'Save as' or convert the images to one of the following formats (note the resolution requirements for line drawings, halftones, and line/halftone combinations given below): EPS (or PDF): Vector drawings, embed all used fonts. TIFF (or JPEG): Color or grayscale photographs (halftones), keep to a minimum of 300 dpi. TIFF (or JPEG): Bitmapped (pure black & white pixels) line drawings, keep to a minimum of 1000 dpi. TIFF (or JPEG): Combinations bitmapped line/halftone (color or grayscale), keep to a minimum of 500 dpi. Please do not: Supply files that are optimized for screen use (e.g., GIF, BMP, PICT, WPG); these typically have a low number of pixels and limited set of colors; Supply files that are too low in resolution; Submit graphics that are disproportionately large for the content. Tables Number tables consecutively in accordance with their appearance in the text. Place footnotes to tables below the table body and indicate them with superscript lowercase letters. Avoid vertical rules. Be sparing in the use of tables and ensure that the data presented in tables do not duplicate results described elsewhere in the article. References Citation in text Please ensure that every reference cited in the text is also present in the reference list (and vice versa). Any references cited in the abstract must be given in full. Unpublished results and personal communications are not recommended in the reference list, but may be mentioned in the text. If these references are included in the reference list they should follow the standard reference style of the journal and should include a substitution of the publication date with either MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 54 'Unpublished results' or 'Personal communication'. Citation of a reference as 'in press' implies that the item has been accepted for publication. Web references As a minimum, the full URL should be given and the date when the reference was last accessed. Any further information, if known (DOI, author names, dates, reference to a source publication, etc.), should also be given. Web references can be listed separately (e.g., after the reference list) under a different heading if desired, or can be included in the reference list. Reference management software This journal has standard templates available in key reference management packages EndNote (http://www.endnote.com/support/enstyles.asp) and Reference Manager (http://refman.com/support/rmstyles.asp). Using plug-ins to wordprocessing packages, authors only need to select the appropriate journal template when preparing their article and the list of references and citations to these will be formatted according to the journal style which is described below. Reference style Text Citations in the text should follow the referencing style used by the American Psychological Association. You are referred to the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, Sixth Edition, ISBN 978-1-4338-0561-5, copies of which may be ordered from http://books.apa.org/books.cfm?id=4200067 or APA Order Dept., P.O.B. 2710, Hyattsville, MD 20784, USA or APA, 3 Henrietta Street, London, WC3E 8LU, UK. List. References should be arranged first alphabetically and then further sorted chronologically if necessary. More than one reference from the same author(s) in the same year MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 55 must be identified by the letters 'a', 'b', 'c', etc., placed after the year of publication. Examples Reference to a journal publication: Van der Geer, J., Hanraads, J. A. J., & Lupton, R. A. (2010). The art of writing a scientific article. Journal of Scientific Communications, 163, 51–59. Reference to a book: Strunk, W., Jr., & White, E. B. (2000). The elements of style. (4th ed.). New York: Longman, (Chapter 4). Reference to a chapter in an edited book: Mettam, G. R., & Adams, L. B. (2009). How to prepare an electronic version of your article. In B. S. Jones, & R. Z. Smith (Eds.), Introduction to the electronic age (pp. 281–304). New York: E-Publishing Inc. Video data Elsevier accepts video material and animation sequences to support and enhance your scientific research. Authors who have video or animation files that they wish to submit with their article are strongly encouraged to include links to these within the body of the article. This can be done in the same way as a figure or table by referring to the video or animation content and noting in the body text where it should be placed. All submitted files should be properly labeled so that they directly relate to the video file's content. In order to ensure that your video or animation material is directly usable, please provide the files in one of our recommended file formats with a preferred maximum size of 50 MB. Video and animation files supplied will be published online in the electronic version of your article in Elsevier Web products, including ScienceDirect: http://www.sciencedirect.com. Please supply 'stills' with your files: you can choose any frame from the video or animation or make a separate image. These will be used instead of standard icons and will personalize the link to your video data. For more detailed instructions please visit our video instruction pages at http://www.elsevier.com/artworkinstructions. Note: since video and animation cannot be MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 56 embedded in the print version of the journal, please provide text for both the electronic and the print version for the portions of the article that refer to this content. Supplementary data Elsevier accepts electronic supplementary material to support and enhance your scientific research. Supplementary files offer the author additional possibilities to publish supporting applications, high-resolution images, background datasets, sound clips and more. Supplementary files supplied will be published online alongside the electronic version of your article in Elsevier Web products, including ScienceDirect: http://www.sciencedirect.com. In order to ensure that your submitted material is directly usable, please provide the data in one of our recommended file formats. Authors should submit the material in electronic format together with the article and supply a concise and descriptive caption for each file. For more detailed instructions please visit our artwork instruction pages at http://www.elsevier.com/artworkinstructions. Submission checklist The following list will be useful during the final checking of an article prior to sending it to the journal for review. Please consult this Guide for Authors for further details of any item. Ensure that the following items are present: One author has been designated as the corresponding author with contact details: E-mail address, full postal address, phone numbers. All necessary files have been uploaded, and contain: Keywords, all figure captions, all tables (including title, description, footnotes). Further considerations: Manuscript has been 'spellchecked' and 'grammar-checked', references are in the correct format for this journal, all references mentioned in the Reference list are cited in the text, and vice versa, permission has been obtained for use of copyrighted material from other sources (including the Web), color figures are clearly marked as being intended for color reproduction on the Web (free of charge) MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 57 and in print, or to be reproduced in color on the Web (free of charge) and in black-and-white in print. If only color on the Web is required, black-and-white versions of the figures are also supplied for printing purposes For any further information please visit our customer support site at http://support.elsevier.com. After Acceptance Use of the Digital Object Identifier The Digital Object Identifier (DOI) may be used to cite and link to electronic documents. The DOI consists of a unique alpha-numeric character string which is assigned to a document by the publisher upon the initial electronic publication. The assigned DOI never changes. Therefore, it is an ideal medium for citing a document, particularly 'Articles in press' because they have not yet received their full bibliographic information. Example of a correctly given DOI (in URL format; here an article in the journal Physics Letters B): http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physletb.2010.09.059 When you use a DOI to create links to documents on the web, the DOIs are guaranteed never to change. Proofs One set of page proofs (as PDF files) will be sent by e-mail to the corresponding author (if we do not have an e-mail address then paper proofs will be sent by post) or, a link will be provided in the e-mail so that authors can download the files themselves. Elsevier now provides authors with PDF proofs which can be annotated; for this you will need to download Adobe Reader version 7 (or higher) available free from http://get.adobe.com/reader. Instructions on how to annotate PDF files will accompany the proofs (also given online). The exact system requirements are given at the Adobe site: http://www.adobe.com/products/reader/tech-specs.html. If you do not wish to use the PDF annotations function, you may list the corrections (including MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 58 replies to the Query Form) and return them to Elsevier in an e-mail. Please list your corrections quoting line number. If, for any reason, this is not possible, then mark the corrections and any other comments (including replies to the Query Form) on a printout of your proof and return by fax, or scan the pages and e-mail, or by post. Please use this proof only for checking the typesetting, editing, completeness and correctness of the text, tables and figures. Significant changes to the article as accepted for publication will only be considered at this stage with permission from the Editor. We will do everything possible to get your article published quickly and accurately – please let us have all your corrections within 48 hours. It is important to ensure that all corrections are sent back to us in one communication: please check carefully before replying, as inclusion of any subsequent corrections cannot be guaranteed. Proofreading is solely your responsibility. Note that Elsevier may proceed with the publication of your article if no response is received. Offprints The corresponding author, at no cost, will be provided with a PDF file of the article via email (the PDF file is a watermarked version of the published article and includes a cover sheet with the journal cover image and a disclaimer outlining the terms and conditions of use). For an extra charge, paper offprints can be ordered via the offprint order form which is sent once the article is accepted for publication. Both corresponding and co-authors may order offprints at any time via Elsevier's WebShop (http://webshop.elsevier.com/myarticleservices/offprints). Authors requiring printed copies of multiple articles may use Elsevier WebShop's 'Create Your Own Book' service to collate multiple articles within a single cover (http://webshop.elsevier.com/myarticleservices/offprints/myarticlesservices/booklets). MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 59 Elsevier NIH Policy Statement As a service to our authors, Elsevier will deposit to PubMed Central (PMC) author manuscripts on behalf of Elsevier authors reporting NIH funded research. This service is a continuation of Elsevier's 2005 agreement with the NIH when the NIH introduced their voluntary 'Public Access Policy'. Please see the full details at: http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/authorsview.authors/nihauthorrequest (this site also includes details on all other funding body agreements). Elsevier facilitates author response to the NIH voluntary posting request (referred to as the NIH "Public Access Policy", see http://www.nih.gov/about/publicaccess/index.htm) by posting the peer-reviewed author's manuscript directly to PubMed Central on request from the author, 12 months after formal publication. Upon notification from Elsevier of acceptance, we will ask you to confirm via e-mail (by e-mailing us at NIHauthorrequest@elsevier.com) that your work has received NIH funding and that you intend to respond to the NIH policy request, along with your NIH award number to facilitate processing. Upon such confirmation, Elsevier will submit to PubMed Central on your behalf a version of your manuscript that will include peer-review comments, for posting 12 months after formal publication. This will ensure that you will have responded fully to the NIH request policy. There will be no need for you to post your manuscript directly with PubMed Central, and any such posting is prohibited. Author Inquiries For inquiries relating to the submission of articles (including electronic submission) please visit this journal's homepage. For detailed instructions on the preparation of electronic artwork, please visit http://www.elsevier.com/artworkinstructions. Contact details for questions arising after MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 60 acceptance of an article, especially those relating to proofs, will be provided by the publisher. You can track accepted articles at http://www.elsevier.com/trackarticle. You can also check our Author FAQs at http://www.elsevier.com/authorFAQ and/or contact Customer Support via http://support.elsevier.com. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 61 Appendix C: Ethical Approval Psychology Research Ethics Committee Psychology, College of Life & Environmental Sciences Washington Singer Laboratories Perry Road Exeter EX4 4QG Telephone +44 (0)1392 724611 Fax +44 (0)1392 724623 Email Marilyn.evans@exeter.ac.uk To: From: CC: Re: Date: Willem Kuyken, Kath Weare, Felica Huppert, Nicola Motton Cris Burgess Application 2011/527 Ethics Committee March 12, 2016 The School of Psychology Ethics Committee has now discussed your application, 2011/527 – An evaluation of a mindfulness programme in schools. The project has been approved in principle for the duration of your study and I can now confirm that we are happy for you to proceed, using the measures you supplied and a parental opt-out procedure. As an advisory note, you may wish to consider informing participants at the briefing stage of the nature and number of the items you intend to ask them to complete, however, this is at your discretion. The agreement of the Committee is subject to your compliance with the British Psychological Society Code of Conduct and the University of Exeter procedures for data protection (http://www.ex.ac.uk/admin/academic/datapro/). In any correspondence with the Ethics Committee about this application, please quote the reference number above. I wish you every success with your research. Cris Burgess Chair of Psychology Research Ethics Committee MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 62 Appendix D: Questionnaire Measures CES-D 8 Item Version Here is a list of the ways you might have felt or behaved during the past week. Please indicate how much of the time during the past week: None or Some of Most of All or almost almost the time the time all of the none of the time time … you felt depressed? 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 … your sleep was restless? 1 2 3 4 … you were happy? 1 2 3 4 … you felt lonely? 1 2 3 4 … you enjoyed life? 1 2 3 4 … you felt sad? 1 2 3 4 … you could not get going? 1 2 3 4 … you felt that everything you did was an effort? MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised CAMS-R Item rated on a Likert Scale from 1 to 4: 1 = rarely / Not at all; 2 = Sometimes; 3= Often; 4 = Almost Always People have a variety of ways of relating to their thoughts and feelings. For each of the items below, please rate how much each of these ways applies to you. 1. It is easy for me to concentrate on what I am doing. 2. I am preoccupied by the future. 3. I can tolerate emotional pain. 4. I can accept things I cannot change. 5. I can usually describe how I feel at the moment in considerable detail. 6. I am easily distracted. 7. I am preoccupied by the past. 8. It's easy for me to keep track of my thoughts and feelings. 9. I try to notice my thoughts without judging them. 10. I am able to accept the thoughts and feelings I have. 11. I am able to focus on the present moment. 12. I am able to pay attention to one thing for a long period of time. 63 MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 64 CERQ: Rumination Scale Four items are rated on a five-point scale: 1 = Never or almost never, 2 = Sometimes, 3 = Regularly, 4 = Often, 5 = Always of almost always. Sometimes nice things happen in your life and sometimes unpleasant things might happen. When something unpleasant happens to you, how often do you have each of the following thoughts? 1) I am preoccupied with what I think and feel about what I have experienced 2) I dwell upon the feelings the situation has evoked in me 3) I think about how I feel about what I have experienced 4) I want to understand why I feel the way I do about what I have experienced MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 65 Appendix E: Parent and Participant Consent Forms, Participant Debriefing form Dear Parents Next term your child will be learning about mindfulness once a week for 9 weeks during [PSHE lessons]. Mindfulness is a training in concentration and self-awareness which can help young people with stress and anxiety. Pupils have said that this programme can help them not only with stress but with concentration, sleep and even sport. For further information on the specific course we will be teaching please visit the Mindfulness in Schools Project website: www.mindfulnessinschools.org. For a broader overview of mindfulness in the adult world, a website has been set up by the Mental Health Foundation: www.bemindful.co.uk. We are currently working with Professors Katherine Weare and Willem Kuyken at Exeter University and Professor Felicia Huppert at Cambridge University to see how effective this programme. This would involve your child completing some short questionnaires before, after, and three months following the course. During the lessons, the teachers may be filmed for the sole purpose of monitoring the quality of their teaching. Your child’s participation in the evaluation is voluntary and they will be asked at school if they wish take part in the evaluation. They can choose not to take part or withdraw from the study at any time without giving a reason and we will find an alternative activity for them during the time. The information provided by your child will be anonymous so that it is impossible to trace this information back to them individually. Following completion of the study, pupils will be provided with feedback about the study findings, including some individualised feedback on their scores if they specifically ask for this. As a parent you can withdraw your child from the evaluation research if you do not wish them to take part. If you have any questions, please contact me using the details below. If you wish for your child to not take part in this research please complete and return the slip below by [date 2 weeks following] With best wishes [Class teacher] [contact details of class teacher] MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 66 Attached Slip: If you wish for your child not to take part in this research then please complete and return this form via mail or email by [date] and their involvement will not be requested. I, ________________________________(NAME) do not wish for my child ________________________________(CHILD’S NAME) to participate in this study. Date: Participant Consent Form Your school is offering or considering offering a programme that aims to help young people learn, cope with stress and enjoy life to the full. It is called the .B mindfulness in schools programme and you can read more about it at www.mindfulnessinschools.org. We want to find out how useful students find this programme by looking at things like stress, well-being and learning before and after the programme. You are in a school where the .B programme is being offered and taking part involves filling out some questionnaires before the programme starts, immediately after the programme completes and then again in term after the programme completes. Each set will take no more than 50 minutes of your time. Or You are in a school where we plan to offer the .B programme, but for now we are a comparison site for schools that are already teaching the programme. Taking part involves filling out some questionnaires in January, again at the end of term and then again in the summer term. Each set will take no more than 50 minutes of your time. If you have questions or concerns about any of the questions please speak to your teacher. You don’t have to take part in this. If you want to stop taking part at any time, just let your teacher know. All information you give will be kept anonymous and confidential. If you have any questions, just ask your teacher. [Additional part to be presented before the online questionnaires:] If you are happy to take part, please click to get to the next page and start the questionnaires. [Additional part to be presented after the online questionnaires:] Do you want some feedback on how you did in the questionnaires? If tick here and we will send you an email at the end. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 67 Participant Debriefing Form Thank you for taking part in this study. Some of you who filled out these questionnaires also took part in a programme called “.B”, designed to teach people ways of coping with the stresses and strains of everyday life. The school were hoping to see whether people that took part in “.B” changed in any way, such as whether they coped with stress better, or felt any happier overall. Your taking part in this has helped the school find out whether the “.B” programme is helpful. Thank you once again for taking part. MINDFULNESS FOR ADOLESCENT DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS 68 Dissemination Statement The findings of the current study will be disseminated in two ways; an individualised school profile (including details of how the school scored compared to the rest of the sample according to depression and wellbeing) as well as overall study results will be provided to all schools that took part, and the findings will be submitted for publication in Behaviour Research and Therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy is a fitting journal as it invites publications that evaluate interventions, and seek to answer questions relating to the process as well as the outcomes of interventions. The journal also invites publications that examine the processes that underlie psychological disorders, and considers how this knowledge can be applied to preventative interventions. This speaks to the issues discussed in this paper relating to the processes that maintain depressive symptoms and lead to depression. The findings will also contribute to the formulation of the next phase of evaluating this mindfulness intervention, which will be conducted by the wider research team involved in this project (Kuyken et al., 2013).