Poverty and Child Development: Scientific Advances and Policy Implications J. Lawrence Aber, Ph.D. Professor of Applied Psychology and Public Policy Institute for Human Development and Social Change New York University Comments prepared for the: National Summit on America’s Children Cannon House Office Building U.S. House of Representatives Washington, D.C. May 22, 2007 1 Speaker Pelosi, Chairman Miller, Representative DeLauro, Representative Fattah and distinguished Members of Congress. Thank you for convening this “National Summit on America’s Children” and for asking me to participate on this panel addressing issues of “Income and Family Support”. This afternoon, I want to briefly update Members on: first, a few basic facts about child poverty and family economic security; second, recent advances in the scientific understanding of the effects of poverty and hardship on children’s development; and third, possible policy implications of these scientific advances. A Few Basic Facts Nationwide, 18% of all children live in families with incomes below 100% of the Federal Poverty Line (about $20,000/year for a family of 4). This amounts to 13 million children living in poverty across the U.S. Especially relevant to this Summit, the poverty rate tends to be higher for infants, toddlers, and preschoolers than for school-age children (20% vs. 16%). Child poverty rates also vary greatly across race/ethnic groups (White children: 10%, Latino children: 28%; Black children: 35%), and states (from 7% in New Hampshire to 27% in Mississippi). As troubling as these statistics are, they only tell part of the problem. Research consistently shows that, on average, families need an income of about twice the federal poverty level to make ends meet. Children living in families with incomes below this level—for 2006, $40,000 for a family of four—are referred to as low income. An additional 21% of all children live in such low income families that earn “too much” to be officially poor but too little to reach self-sufficiency and basic economic security. Low family incomes mean substantial economic and material hardships for too many of America’s children. For instance, 16% of America’s children experience significant food insecurity and 41% of families who rent pay more than 1/3 of their income on rent. (For these and other details, I refer you to the National Center for Children in Poverty’s website www.nccp.org and their fact sheet on “Who are America’s Poor Children?”) Scientific Evidence 2 Over the last decade, converging evidence from both experimental and longitudinal research makes one point crystal clear: Poverty has a pervasive negative effect on children’s health, learning and development. (Poverty also has a negative effect on children’s future life chances, which Dr. Rucker Johnson from U. California, Berkeley, will describe in a few moments). We have unequivocal evidence that poor children are more likely than non-poor children to manifest developmental delays and learning disabilities (Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997; Klerman, 1991), to have lower IQs (Brooks-Gunn, Klebanov, & Duncan, 1996; Duncan, BrooksGunn, & Klebanov, 1994), and to repeat a grade or to drop out of school (Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997; Children’s Defense Fund, 1997). Even after controlling for relevant parent characteristics (e.g., education), poor children begin kindergarten with significantly lower achievement in math, reading, and general knowledge than their higher income peers (Gershoff, 2003; Lee & Burkham, 2002; West, Denton, & Germino Hausken, 2000) and increasingly fall behind as they progress through school (Fryer & Levitt, 2004, 2005; Rathbun & West, 2004). Children from poor families are also at greater risk for experiencing behavioral or emotional problems such as antisocial behavior (Aber, Bennett, Conley, & Li, 1997; Duncan & BrooksGunn, 1997; Miech, Caspi, Moffitt, Wright, & Silva, 1999; Takeuchi, Williams, & Adair, 1991), as well as internalizing behavior problems, such as depression (McLeod & Shanahan, 1996; Miech et al., 1999; Takeuchi et al., 1991). Similarly, poor children show difficulties with aspects of social competence including self-regulation and impulsivity (Takeuchi et al., 1991), abilities associated with social-emotional competence (Eisenberg et al., 1996). Bolstering the case that family income matters for children are findings that with increases in family income, children’s cognitive-academic skills and social-emotional competence indeed improve (for reviews and examples, see: Dahl & Lochner, 2005; Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 1997; Gershoff, 2003; Gershoff, Aber, & Raver, 2003; Mayer, 2002; McLoyd, 1998; Seccombe, 2000). There is clear evidence from both natural experiments (Costello, Compton, Keeler, & Angold, 2003) and randomized experiments (Morris & Gennetian, 2003) that increases in family income, particularly among poor families, have positive impacts on children. It is incontrovertible that living in poor or low income families remains a significant barrier to children meeting their cognitive and social potential. 3 While the deleterious effects of poverty on children’s health, education and development should be concerning enough, two recent reports presented to the Committee on Ways and Means in the U.S. House of Representatives have highlighted the economic costs to the country posed by such high child poverty rates. One report by the Government Accountability Office concluded that higher poverty rates in the general population are associated with slower economic growth, particularly in areas of concentrated poverty (Nilsen, 2007). A second report, published by the Center for American Progress, estimated that child poverty costs the U.S. $500 billion per year, roughly equivalent to 4% of GDP, through reductions in productivity and economic output, increases in crime, and increases in health expenditures (Holzer, Schanzenbach, Duncan, & Ludwig, 2007). Both reports conclude that interventions aimed at augmenting parents’ human capital, improving disadvantaged schools and neighborhoods, and providing more income supports are indicated and stand a fair chance of helping poor and low income families. The convergence of the longitudinal and experimental scientific research allows us to conclude that those negative effects of poverty and low-income on children’s health, education and development are causal. Much like earlier research on the effects of smoking, we now know for certain what most of us always suspected: poverty is bad for children’s development and the nation’s wellbeing. Recent Scientific Advances The most recent research on poverty and child development has now turned away from asking the question “does poverty have an effect?” to questions of “how? by what mechanisms?” and “what can be done to reduce child poverty and ameliorate its negative effects?”. You’ve heard the advice of Nobel Laureate James Heckman (which has been echoed by others today): INVEST EARLY in our most vulnerable children. But in what? How? My research colleagues and I have developed an “evidence-based conceptual framework” to guide future scientific research on these questions and to inform policy development (Gershoff, Aber & Raver, 2003). This framework is presented in Figure 1. It looks complicated but I hope it tells a clear story. 4 At the family (micro) level, 1. It is important to consider not only income, but also other factors (like material hardship) when considering how economic disadvantage influences children’s development. 2. Income and hardship influence children’s development in large measure through parents: their investments in their children; the stress they experience in raising children and making ends meet; and their behaviors toward their children. 3. The specific paths of influence from socio-economic disadvantage through parenting to different features of children’s development can provide a scientific guide to how to target programs and policies. At the community and policy (macro) level, 4. The family system is embedded in the institutions and neighborhoods in which families and children live. 5. Different policy and program strategies are aimed at different features of the familymicro-level model. (In Gershoff et al. (2003), we lay out in detail how specific human capital policies, income policies, in-kind supports, parenting and two generation strategies, and child-directed strategies target different parts of the family micro-system.) In Figure 2, I present the key results from a study that used this conceptual framework to understand how low family income and material hardship influence parenting and children’s development in a large and nationally representative sample, the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study (which is directed by the National Center for Education Statistics in the U.S. Department of Education). In these analyses, we clearly identified two distinct paths of influence: one goes from low family income to low-parent investment to non-optimal cognitive/academic development of young children (we call this the Family Investment path); the second goes from high material hardship to high parent stress and harsh/punitive parenting behavior to non-optimal social-emotional development of children (we call this the Family Stress path). In short, income matters: low income constrains parents’ ability to invest time and money in their young children; and low parental investment constrains young children’s cognitive/academic 5 development. In addition, material hardship matters: material hardship generates parental stress and harsh-punitive parenting styles which in turn constrains young children’s social-emotional development. Finally, these constraints on the development of children in low-income families contribute to the growing inequalities of development across childhood, adolescence and adulthood in the U.S. These are some of the key processes by which poverty gets into family relationships and into kids’ heads and hearts. Potential Policy Implications What are some potential implications of these scientific advances in our understanding of poverty’s effects on parents and children for the policy choices facing the nation? I wish to highlight three main implications today. First, they suggest that U.S. social policies and programs should be reformed to more effectively target family income and material hardship as key upstream determinants of non-optimal parenting and children’s development. If very low family incomes can be significantly and predictably increased, if material hardship can be significantly and predictably reduced, parents will invest more in their children and stress out less. In turn, children will learn more and act out less. Second, federal, state and local governments should build on those policy strategies that are already in place that can help poor families effectively increase their incomes and reduce their material hardship. Positive work incentives built into some types of welfare reforms (like the New Hope Project in Milwaukee), increases in the minimum wage, improving the uptake and benefit levels of Food Stamps, and expansion of the federal earned income tax credit are four good examples. Perhaps the single best opportunity on the national policy horizon and consistent with these scientific findings to increase income available to children in poor families would be to make the Child Tax Credit fully refundable. Then, the CTC would function for poor families the way it does for middle income families in the U.S., as a home-grown version of a “children’s allowance”. (Cross-national comparative research clearly indicates that social policies like children’s allowances help explain why child poverty rates are so much lower in most other advanced industrial democracies than they are in the U.S.) 6 Third, and finally, we must continue to support the most rigorous and creative scientific research on the causes and consequences of child poverty and experimental social policy research on how to reduce poverty and protect children from its negative effects. In my home town of New York City, we have begun to do just that. Our moderate Republican Mayor, Michael Bloomberg, named a blue-ribbon antipoverty commission. The Commission identified evidence-based strategies that were ready to be brought to scale in the City (like Nurse-Family Partnerships which have been shown to reduce family stress and harsh-punitive parenting). It also identified exciting new strategies developed in other countries (like the UK and Mexico) to reduce child poverty and increase family investments in children’s human capital (like Conditional Cash Transfers). City government and the private sector are adapting and pilot-testing them in NYC to see if they are ready to be brought to scale. (Fellow panelist Gordon Berlin’s organization, MDRC, is designing the evaluation.) In keeping with the message you’ve heard throughout the day, I urge you to harness the best science available to craft the income and family supports needed by the youngest and most vulnerable of America’s children. Reducing poverty and material hardship will not solve all the problems America’s youngest children face. But it will make virtually every other problem we’ve discussed today easier to solve. Thank you. 7 Figure 1. Heuristic HeuristicModel Model Federal -, State -, and Community -Level Policy and Program Interventions Parent - and Family Level Predictors of Income And Hardship Parent Work Status Job Prestige Education Level Parent Marital Status Race -Ethnicity Family Income Poverty Financial Hardship Parent Investment Child Physical Development Parent Behavior Child Cognitive Development Parent Distress Child Social Emotional Development Neighborhood - and Community -Level Influences From: Gershoff, E. T., Aber, J. L., & Raver, C. C. (2003). Child poverty in the United States: An evidence-based conceptual framework for programs and policies. In F. Jacobs, D. Wertlieb, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Applied Developmental Science: Promoting positive child, adolescent, and family development through research, policies, and programs. Vol. 2 (pp. 81-136). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 8 Figure 2. .13 .05 Family Highest Education .45 Marital Status .30 Father Work Status .51 Family Income -.17 African-American -.15 .15 Hispanic-American Asian-American Other Race-Ethnicity Family Size -.04 .52 R2 = .43 .20 Mother Work Status .14 Parent Investment .22 .04 .13 R2 = .33 -.58 -.05 .01 Parent Stress -.23 Material Hardship 2 R = .34 -.02 Child Cognitive Competence .81 2 R = .35 .70 .18 -.88 -.06 .38 Child Social-Emotional Competence R2 = .26 Positive Parenting Behavior R2 = .66 .43 -.11 -.08 CFI = .972, RMSEA = .066, AIC = 41,714.76, 2 (445) = 41,484.76 Note. Standardized paths are shown; all paths are significant at at least p < .05 except for the dashed path which was not significant. Gershoff, E. T., Aber, J. L., Raver, C. C., & Lennon, M. C. (2007). Income is not enough: Incorporating material hardship into models of income associations with parent mediators and child outcomes. Child Development, 78, 70-95. 9 Bibliography and References Cited Aber, J. L., Bennett, N. G., Conley, D. C., & Li, J. (1997). The effects of poverty on child health and development. Annual Review of Public Health, 18, 463-483. Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G. J., & Aber, J. L. (Eds.). 1997. Neighborhood poverty: Context and consequences for children (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation Press. Brooks-Gunn, J., Klebanov, P., & Duncan, G. J. (1996). Ethnic differences in children’s intelligence test scores: Role of economic deprivation, home environment, and maternal characteristics. Child Development, 67, 396-408. Children’s Defense Fund. (1997). Poverty matters: The cost of child poverty in America. Washington, DC: Author. Costello, E. J., Compton, S. N., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: A natural experiment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 290, 2023-2029. Dahl, G., & Lochner, L. (2005). The impact of family income on child development. IRP Discussion Paper no. 1305-05. Madison, WI: Institute for Research on Poverty. Retrieved 2/18/06 from http://www.irp.wisc.edu/publications/dps/pdfs/dp130505.pdf Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1997). Consequences of growing up poor. New York: Russell Sage. Duncan, G. J, Brooks-Gunn, J., Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic deprivation and early childhood development. Child Development, 65, 296-318. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Maszk, P., Holmgren, R., & Suh, K. (1996). The relations of regulation and emotionality to problem behavior in elementary school children. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 141-162. Fryer, R., & Levitt, S. (2004). Understanding the Black-White test score gap in the first two years of school. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 86, 447-464. Gershoff, E. T. (2003b). Low income and the development of America’s kindergartners. Living at the Edge, Research Brief No. 4. New York, NY: National Center for Children in Poverty. Available at: www.nccp.org/media/lat03d-text.pdf Gershoff, E. T., Aber, J. L., & Raver, C. C. (2003). Child poverty in the United States: An evidence-based conceptual framework for programs and policies. In F. Jacobs, D. Wertlieb, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Applied Developmental Science: Promoting positive child, adolescent, and family development through research, policies, and programs. Vol. 2 (pp. 81136). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 10 Gershoff, E. T., Aber, J. L., Raver, C. C., & Lennon, M. C. (2007). Income is not enough: Incorporating material hardship into models of income associations with parent mediators and child outcomes. Child Development, 78, 70-95. Holzer, H. J., Schanzenbach, D. W., Duncan, G. J., & Ludwig, J. (2007). The economic costs of poverty in the United States: Subsequent effects of children growing up poor. Washington, DC: Center for American Progress. Downloaded January 24, 2007, from http://www.americanprogress.org/issues/2007/01/pdf/poverty_report.pdf Klerman, L. V. (1991). Alive and well?: Research and policy review of health programs for poor youngchildren. National Center for Children in Poverty, New York, NY. Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. 2002. Inequality at the starting gate: Social background differences in achievement as children begin school. Washington, D.C.: Economic Policy Institute. Mayer, S. E. (1997). What money can’t buy: Family income and children’s life chances. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Mayer, S. E. (2002). The influence of parental income on children’s outcomes. Wellington, New Zealand: Knowledge Management Group, Ministry of Social Development. Retrieved October 30, 2004, from http://www.msd.govt.nz/documents/ publications/csre/influence-of-parental-income.pdf McLeod, J. D., & Shanahan, M. J. (1996). Trajectories of poverty and children’s mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37, 207-220. McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist, 53, 185-204. Miech, R. A., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Wright, B. R. E., & Silva, P. A. (1999). Low socioeconomic status and mental disorders: A longitudinal study of selection and causation during young adulthood. American Journal of Sociology, 104, 1096-1131. Morris, P. A., & Gennetian, L. A. (2003). Identifying the effects of income on children’s development using experimental data. Journal of Marriage & Family, 65, 716-729. Nilsen, S. R. (2007, January 24). Poverty in America: Consequences for individuals and the economy. Testimony before the Chairman, Committee on Ways and Means, House of Representatives. Washington, DC: Government Accounting Office. Downloaded January 24, 2007, from http://waysandmeans.house.gov/media/pdf/110/NilsenTESTIMONY.pdf Rathbun, A., & West, J. (2004). From kindergarten through third grade: Children’s beginnign school experiences (NCES 2004-007). National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education. 11 Seccombe, K. (2000). Families in poverty in the 1990s: Trends, causes, consequences, and lessons learned. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62, 1094-1113. Takeuchi, D. T, Williams, D. R., & Adair, R. K. (1991). Economic stress in the family and children’s emotional and behavior problems. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 10311041. West, J., Denton, K., & Germino Hausken, E. (2000). America’s Kindergarteners (NCES 2000070). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education. 12