Running head: RURAL ACCENT STEREOTYPE

advertisement
Midwestern Rural Accent 1
Running head: RURAL ACCENT STEREOTYPE
Midwestern Rural Accent Stereotypes
Doug Hardesty and Claire Morgan
Earlham College
Midwestern Rural Accent 2
Abstract
How individuals with rural accents are perceived was investigated. Researchers
hypothesized that accented speakers would be seen as less effective and less educated
than a control. Thirty-two participants listened to an audiotape with either a standard
urban or a Midwestern rural accent. The speaker was identified as being either from
Earlham College or Richmond, Indiana. Participants completed a survey describing how
effective they believed the speaker was. A statistically significant interaction, F(1,32) =
5.36, p  .05, for the speaker’s motivation and a main effect for residence, F(1,32) = 4.83,
p  .05, describing how well-educated the speaker appeared were found. The hypothesis
was thus only partially supported.
Midwestern Rural Accent 3
Midwestern Rural Accent Stereotypes
A stereotype may be defined as a set of rigid beliefs, positive or negative, about
the attributes of a group (Punzo, classroom lecture, 2003). These beliefs influence how
groups and group members are perceived and evaluated. Oftentimes, certain
characteristics are attributed to a given group. These characteristics are closely linked
with how the group is expected to act or perform. The strength of this influence can be
seen in an experiment performed by Darley and Gross (1983).
In this study, participants were led to believe that “Hannah” came from either a
low socioeconomic (SES) background or a high socioeconomic background. The
participants were then asked to evaluate the child’s performance on completing several
math problems. While the child’s performance in both cases was the same, participants
who believed that the child came from a high-SES background rated the child’s
performance above grade level. Participants who believed the girl came from a low-SES
background rated the child’s performance below grade level. These results indicate that
stereotyping-cuing information, such as the girl’s socioeconomic background, led
participants to make assumptions about Hannah’s intellectual abilities. These
assumptions can lead to prejudice, when group members are expected to act in negative
ways (Allport, 1954).
Accented speech can affect a listener’s impressions about a speaker (Giles &
Powesland, 1975). In particular, non-standard accents are often thought of less favorably
than standard accents. This thought process leads to prejudice against individuals with
non-standard accents.
Midwestern Rural Accent 4
Certain regional accents lend positive or negative traits to speakers. In one study,
researchers investigated how various Irish regional accents affected students’ impressions
of a man reading a passage on Irish history (Edwards, 1977). The matched-guise
technique was used to record five stimuli accents. Students then rated each speaker on
nine qualities, such as intelligence, ambition, and friendliness. Regional accent had a
significant effect on all traits, with a Donegal accent rated most positively, and a Dublin
accent rated least positively (Edwards, 1977).
The negative effects of accent prejudice have been powerfully demonstrated
(Dixon, Mahoney, & Cocks, 2002). A study presented participants with an audiotape of a
criminal investigation, in which a man was interrogated by the police. The researchers
varied the accent of the accused man between standard British and Birmingham British
using the matched-guise technique. The type of crime (blue- or white-collar) was also
manipulated. Participants saw the non-standard Birmingham accent as more guilty,
especially when the crime was described as blue-collar (Dixon, Mahoney, & Cocks,
2002).
Some of the prejudices that arise from non-standard accents are associated with
class. Speech and accents are used as social cues (Foon, 1986). Thus, some of the
prejudices against accented individuals may be reflective of the prejudices against the
social class that such an accent presents. Participants in the British accent study (Dixon,
Mahoney, & Cocks, 2002) may have linked the stereotypically lower-class Birmingham
accent with the stereotype that blue-collar crimes are committed by poor individuals.
Lastly, the median household income in Wayne County (in which this study takes
place) is $34,885 per year, whereas the average income of a student’s family is $69,400
Midwestern Rural Accent 5
(U.S. Census Bureau, 2000; Arnold, personal correspondence, 2003). Students may
consider community members to be of a lower class and stereotype them accordingly
(Foon, 1986). A rural accent, which many community members have, may also cue these
class stereotypes due to association.
In summary, a non-standard or regional accent can negatively affect the way a
speaker is viewed. Some of these prejudices are activated due to the social class
stereotype an accent holds. These prejudices have negative consequences for individuals
with non-standard accents. There is a class discrepancy between local community
members and students at the college, which may cue stereotypical class beliefs,
regardless of accent.
This leads researchers to hypothesize that individuals with Midwestern rural
accents, such as local community members have, will be stereotyped by students. This
stereotype will be stronger towards individuals with rural accents that are also identified
as being from the local community, due to the class discrepancy between students and
local citizens. This stereotype will manifest itself in lower ratings of the intelligence,
education, and competence of a speaker with such an accent, when compared to a control.
Method
Participants
Thirty-two students at Earlham College were used as participants. A portion of
the participants were known by the researchers, but most were recruited via
announcements in psychology courses. Extra credit points were offered to those
participants recruited from courses. Participants were not filtered by age, sex, race, class
year, place of origin, or socioeconomic status.
Midwestern Rural Accent 6
Materials
Participants listened to an audio recording of a man reading a 30-second passage
on minimum wage increase. The matched-guise technique was used to record the
audiotape in either a standard urban American accent (control condition), or a
Midwestern rural accent (experimental condition). The voice actor read the following
passage:
Americans need an increase in wage! Minimum wage in this country has gone up
only 200% since 1970. Living expenses have gone up 500%. This isn’t fair! Even
working more than 40 hours a week, people on minimum wage can’t longer
afford basic needs of life. More and more paychecks are going towards taxes and
welfare instead of supporting hard-working families. Why can’t the government
raise minimum wage to a reasonable level? I don’t see how factory workers can
continue to live like this. If someone doesn’t do something soon, there are going
to be a lot of good people living on the streets!
Participants completed a questionnaire after hearing the tape. Three Likert-scale
questions asked participants to evaluate the speaker’s effectiveness, persuasiveness, and
his knowledge of the issues involving minimum wage increase. Two Likert-scale
questions asked participants to rate the education level of the speaker, and if the speaker’s
motivation for speaking was for his own financial benefit. All Likert-scale items were on
a seven-point scale, with 1 being strongly disagree, and 7 being strongly agree.
Participants were also asked to select two words they believed best described the speaker:
wealthy, trustworthy, idealistic, middle-class, underpaid, condescending, ignorant,
unhappy, passionate, upset, confident, or hopeful. The background information of
Midwestern Rural Accent 7
participants was also gathered, including gender, class year, parent’s annual income,
highest level of education obtained by either parent, state/country of origin, and if the
participant worked or volunteered in the local community.
Procedure
Participants were told that the experimenters were conducting a study on public
speaking. Participants were randomly assigned to listen to a speaker with either a
Midwestern rural accent or a standard urban accent. Upon dividing participants into
groups, they were taken into different rooms and given a handout describing the speaker.
The speaker was identified on the handout as being either from the local community of
Richmond, Indiana or from Earlham College, depending on the condition to which the
participant had been assigned. Therefore, there were four conditions: a rural accent from
Richmond, a rural accent from Earlham, a standard accent from Richmond and a standard
accent from Earlham. Participants were told the recording came from a local community
meeting in 2001.
Participants then listened to the stimulus material (the audiotape) and completed a
survey. The survey asked questions pertaining to the speaker’s performance and the
personal background of the participant. After completing the survey, participants were
debriefed and told the true nature of the study.
Results
A 2 (Accent Type) X 2 (Residence) factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) was
conducted for each of the speaker evaluation items. Analysis of the financial motivation
of the speaker yielded a statistically significant interaction, F(1,32) = 5.36, p  .05. These
findings indicate that participants in the standard accent, college condition did not believe
Midwestern Rural Accent 8
that the speaker was arguing for his own financial gain as compared to participants in the
other three conditions. No main effects were found for accent type or residence when
examining the speaker’s financial motivation for speaking.
Analysis of how well-educated the speaker was perceived as being revealed a
main effect for residence, F(1,32) = 4.83, p  .05. In both the rural accent and standard
accent conditions, speakers described as being from the college were seen as better
educated than those described as being from the community (refer to Table 1). No
interaction between accent type and residence for this variable were found. Intelligence,
influence or knowledge of the speaker did not yield any significant interactions or main
effects. No significant results were found for the adjective selection in which participants
described the speaker.
Discussion
In this study, participants listened to a brief audio recording of a man speaking
about minimum wage increase. Accent (Midwestern rural or standard urban) and
residence (Earlham College, or Richmond, Indiana) were independent variables.
Researchers hypothesized that both a Midwestern rural accent and being identified as
from the local community would negatively affect participants’ impressions of the
speaker. Researchers also predicted that accent and residence would affect how educated
the speaker appeared, and if the speaker was arguing for his own financial gain (i.e., if he
was employed in a minimum-wage job).
The hypothesis was only partially supported. Two statistically significant results
were found: an interaction between accent and residence on the speaker’s motivation, and
a main effect of residence of speaker on perceived education level.
Midwestern Rural Accent 9
The interaction between accent type and residence in relation to the speaker’s
financial motivation for speaking partially supported the hypothesis. As expected, the
standard accent, college speaker was described as being the least likely to be speaking for
his or her own financial gain. Surprisingly, the rural accent, college speaker was
described as being the most likely to be speaking for an increase in personal financial
gain. Researchers anticipated that the rural accent, community member would be
perceived as the most likely to be speaking for such a reason.
It is possible that the rural accent, college speaker was considered more capable
of strongly arguing for such a point. This suggests that education level may be more of a
deciding factor than accent type in evaluating the effectiveness of a speaker. It is possible
that the stereotype held by college students towards community members was not as
strong as had been anticipated.
Participants considered the speaker less educated when identified as being from
the local community. The standard accent speaker identified as being from the college
was rated as being the most educated; the rural accent speaker identified as being from
the college was rated second most educated out of the four conditions. Participants may
have assumed the speaker was an Earlham student or faculty member, even though he
was identified as “a man from Earlham College.” These results indicate that Earlham
students seem to view citizens of the local town as less educated than members of the
Earlham community.
Several problems may have influenced the results of this study. One was the
limited sample size, with only eight participants in each condition. Significant results
may have manifested in other categories had there been a greater number of participants.
Midwestern Rural Accent 10
Another area of concern was the stimulus material. The voice actor, although convincing,
was not a replacement for a native rural accent. Some of the finer distinctions in the
Midwestern rural accent may not have been accurately represented. The prejudices of
participants may have been cued more strongly had a native speaker been used in the
audio recording. Because the tapes were identical except for accent type, no mannerisms
or slang words were used. This may have lessened the intended stereotyping-cuing effect
of the tape. Lastly, two-thirds of participants were freshmen. It can be assumed that
freshmen have had less contact with the local community than have upperclassmen.
Increased contact might result in more, or less, prejudice against community members.
In future studies it would be interesting to see if perceived education level is a
more influential factor than accent type in evaluating the effectiveness of an individual’s
public speaking ability. It would also be useful in the context of accent study to examine
whether the word choice and mannerisms of a speaker or a speaker’s accent influences
participants’ perceptions more strongly.
Midwestern Rural Accent 11
References
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Oxford, England: Addison-Wesley.
Darley, J.M., & Gross, P.H. (1983). A hypothesis-confirming bias in labeling effects.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 20-33.
Dixon, J., Mahoney, B., & Cocks, R. (2002). Accents of guilt? Effects of regional accent,
race, and crime type on attributions of guilt. Journal of Language and Social
Psychology, 21, 162-168.
Edwards, J.R. (1977). Students’ reactions to Irish regional accents. Language & Speech,
20, 280-286.
Foon, A.E. (1986). A social structural approach to speech evaluation. The Journal of
Social Psychology, 126, 521-530.
Giles, H., & Powesland, P.F. (1975). Speech evaluation and social evaluation. London:
Academic Press.
U.S. Census Bureau (2000). 2000 census of population and housing. Retrieved
December 4th, 2003, from http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/states/18/18177.html
Midwestern Rural Accent 12
Table 1. Means and standard deviations for apparent education of speaker and motivation
of speaker.
Midwestern Rural Accent
Community
College
Perceived level
of education
Standard Accent
Community
College
3.625
4.375
4.000
4.875
Standard dev.
1.06
0.916
1.20
0.991
Motivation of
speaker*
3.000
3.625
3.375
2.000
Standard dev.
0.926
1.60
1.19
1.07
* Higher means reflects agreement that the speaker
was arguing for his own financial benefit
Download