THE POTENTIAL FOR STEREOTYPE CONTROL: A Dual-Process Approach Much of the controversy surrounding stereotypes centers around the possibility that, as schemas that organize knowledge about the meaning of group membership (Lutz, 2008), they may also have negative implications. Indeed, schemas have the power to direct attention, guide the encoding of relevant information and memory, as well as influence judgment (Fiske, 1994). Given the ability of schemas to effect processing in such salient ways, a key question becomes whether or not it is possible to control the influence of stereotypes. This essay examines the potential for controlling the impact of stereotyping on judgment and behavior at multiple levels of processing and across situations. In order to understand the possibility of controlling stereotyping, it is necessary to understand first how it happens. A review of dual-process models reveals several important overarching features. Most importantly, each conceptualizes stereotyping as occurring along a continuum. Beginning with identification (Brewer, 1988) or stereotyping (Fiske, Lin, and Neuberg, 1999), dual-process models argue for the possibility of moving beyond simple schematic processing to integrate salient information about the target piece by piece to achieve individuation (Brewer, 1988; Fiske, Lin, and Neuberg, 1999). The heuristic-systematic model (Chaiken, Liberman, and Eagly, 1989) is another dual-process model similar to the one described above. Chaiken et al. posit a process by which we move from heuristic processing, relying on previous knowledge such as schemas and stereotypes, to systematic processing which enables us to examine detailed qualities and behaviors of the target. These models also share an implicit understanding of the distinction between automatic and controlled processing. Automatic processing occurs at the beginning of the continuum and is unintentional, initiated by environmental cues that activate prior knowledge (Tesser, 1994). Thus, it is more difficult to inhibit than controlled processing, which requires active intention and the intentional activation of information in memory (Tesser, 1994). These two levels of processing provide a framework within which we can examine the potential for stereotype control. Evidence of the ability to control the influence of stereotyping on judgments at a controlled level is demonstrated in studies asking both low- and high-prejudice subjects to list their thoughts on a particular social group (e.g Devine, 1989). High-prejudice subjects generally name negative traits (e.g. lazy), while low-prejudice subjects are more likely to list positive beliefs (e.g. we are equal). These results indicate that it is possible for motivated perceivers to prevent the influence of stereotyping on judgments at the controlled level. This is not to say, however, that it is easy to stop the effects of stereotyping. Even as perceivers consciously attempt to control their behavior, their processing may still be biased. A number of challenges are presented to individuals attempting to control the influence of stereotyping. For example, selective attention (Darley and Gross, 1983; Fiske, Lin, and Neuberg, 1999) dictates that perceivers pay more attention to facts that confirm their original hypothesis. Individuals also consider consistent facts to be more relevant to their judgment (Lutz, 2008) and discount or reinterpret inconsistent information (Darley and Gross, 1983). Thus, awareness of processing biases such as these is important to recognizing the potential impact of stereotyping on judgment. While this is clearly difficult, certain factors do encourage stereotype control. Situational factors, such as time (Devine, 1989), are necessary for perceivers’ ability to process information at a deeper, i.e. more individuated, level. Additionally, as we can only integrate knowledge we have access to, available information may limit our potential for individuation. Personal factors also determine the level of processing individuals engage in. Cognitive capacity (Devine, 1989) must be present in order for deeper processing to occur, as well as motivation (Fiske, Lin, and Neuberg, 1999) and commitment (Moskowitiz, 1999). Practice has also been shown to be central to stereotype control (Moskowitiz, 1999; Sassenberg and Moskowitz 2005). These factors are important to reducing and preventing the influence of stereotypes because the wellestablished nature of such automatic processes makes them difficult to bypass. Recent evidence suggests, however, that it is possible to control the effect of stereotyping at the automatic level. By showing long-term commitment to egalitarian goals, perceivers can encounter category members without activating the relevant stereotype (Moskowitz, 1999). This, of course, requires practicing non-activation until it replaces stereotyping as the default response for each social group the perceiver encounters. Although this is an effective method of automatic stereotype control, it is tedious for even the most motivated (and egalitarian) perceivers to practice non-activation to this extent for each social group they wish to think and behave toward in an individuated manner. Luckily, at least one alternative has been shown to have the same effect. Perceivers in a creative mindset are less likely to rely on existing knowledge to process information (Sassenberg and Moskowitz, 2005). The finding that this mindset leads to preconscious inhibition is an important new way of understanding our ability to control the influence of stereotyping. The idea that we are able to control stereotyping at both the automatic and controlled levels has several implications for behavior. It seems likely that decreasing or preventing activation of stereotypes upon contact with a target will reduce the chance of behaving in a stereotypical manner toward that target (Sassenberg and Moskowitz, 2005). However, this may not always be the case. Even when the schema is not activated, it is still possible that we may behave stereotypically. For example, one of the most common stereotypes of African Americans is that they are hostile. Imagine that a perceiver dedicated to egalitarian goals and well-practiced in non-activation is having a bad day and gets into an argument with an African American man who accidentally bumps into her on the sidewalk. This could be representative of stereotypical behavior (the bump was interpreted as hostile), or it could be unique situational and personal factors leading an otherwise low-prejudice person to behave in an atypical manner. Thus, our ability to control stereotyping is only one determinant of our behavior. There is one other major implication of our ability to control stereotyping at automatic and controlled levels. That is, we have a responsibility to do so. While it is true that stereotyping, as a heuristic, can be useful in reducing the amount of environmental information we are forced to process, we must also recognize that allowing this heuristic to activate and influence our judgment and behavior can have negative effects. Thus, in order to counter these potentially damaging consequences of stereotyping, it is important to recognize our ability for stereotype control and situations that are conducive to that process. References Brewer MB. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In A Dual-Process Model of Impression Formation: Advances in Social Cognition, ed. RS Wyer Jr, TK Srull, 1:1–36. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Chaiken, S., Liberman, A. & Eagly, A. (1989). Heuristic and Systematic Information Processing within and beyond the Persuasion Context. In J. Uleman & J. Bargh, Eds. Unintended Thought (pp. 212-252). New York: Guilford Press. Darley, J. M. & Gross, P. H. (1983). A Hypothesis-Confirming Bias in Labeling Effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44. Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and Prejudice: Their Automatic and Controlled Components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (56), 5-18. Fiske, S. T. (1994). Social Perception. In A. Tesser, Ed. Advanced Social Psychology. McGrawHill. Fiske, S. T., Lin, M. & Neuberg, S. L. (1999). The Continuum Model: Ten Years Later. Lutz, S. (2008, Mar. 12). Stereotyping. Psychology of Social Behavior. New York University. Lutz, S. (2008, Mar. 26). Stereotype Control. Psychology of Social Behavior. New York University. Moskowitz, G. (2005). Dual-Process Models. Social Cognition: Understanding Self and Others (p. 193-232). New York: Guilford Press. Sassenberg, K. & Moskowitz, G. (2005). Don't stereotype, think different! Overcoming automatic stereotype activation by mindset priming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 506-514. Tesser, A. (Ed.). (1994). Advanced Social Psychology. McGraw-Hill.