Forensic Psychology Turning to Crime Making a case Reaching a verdict After a guilty verdict Module: G543 Forensic Psychology 1 – Turning to Crime Social Explanations 1.1 Disrupted Families Farrington 1.2 Learning from Others Sutherland/Akers (EXT) 1.3 Poverty Wilkstrom (Peterborough) 2.1 Criminal Thinking Patterns Yochelson & Samenow 2.2 Moral Development 2.3 Social Cognition Kohlberg/Chen and Howitt(EXT) Gudjohnsson 3.1 Brain Dysfunction Raine/Raine 3.2 Genes & Serotonin Brunner/Caspi(EXT) 3.3 Gender Daly & Wilson Cognitive Explanations Biological Explanations 2 - Making a Case Interviewing Witnesses 1.1 Recognising Faces Bruce 1.2 Factors Influencing Identification Cognitive Interview Loftus (weapon focus)/Pickel(EXT) Fisher 2.1 Detecting Lies Mann 2.2 Interrogation Techniques Inbau/Granhag (EXT) 2.3 False Confessions Gudjohnsson 3.1 Top Down 3.2 Bottom up Theory (Ressler and Hazlewood) Study Canter Canter 3.3 Case Study Canter: John Duffy 1.3 Interviewing Suspects Creating a Profile 2 3 – Reaching a Verdict Persuading a Jury 1.1 1.2 1.3 Effect of Order of Testimony Persuasion Pennington & Hastie Effect of Evidence Ruled Inadmissible Pickel Attractiveness of Defendant Witness Confidence Castellow/ Theory Dion Loftus Witness Appeal 2.1 2.2 2.3 Penrod Effects of Shields and Video Ross on Children giving Evidence Reaching a Verdict 3.1 Stages in Decision Making Hastie 3.2 Majority Influence Asch 3.3 Minority Influence Nemeth/Moscovici(EXT) 4 – After a Guilty Verdict Imprisonment 1.1 Gillis & Nafekh 1.2 Planned Behaviours when free from Jail Depression & Suicide Risk 1.3 Prison Situation and Roles Zimbardo Alternatives to Imprisonment 2.1 Probation Mair & May 2.2 Restorative Justice Sherman & Strang 2.3 Looking ‘Deathworthy’ Eberhardt 3.1 Cognitive Skills Programme Cann 3.2 Anger Management Ireland 3.3 Ear Acupuncture Wheatley/Margolin(EXT) Palmer and Connelly Treatment Programmes 3 What is Forensic Psychology? The word “forensic” comes from the Latin forensic, “pertaining to a forum”. In ancient Rome the forum was a marketplace where people gathered not just to buy things but to also conduct all kinds of business, including public affairs and legal disputes. The word later came to be restricted to refer to the courts of law. The word “forensic” has been used in England since 1659 and now indicates the application of a particular area of scientific knowledge to legal problems and legal proceedings, for example, in forensic anthropology, where anthropological knowledge is applied to identify human remains found at a crime scene. Forensic psychologists apply psychological knowledge to help them understand the behaviour of criminals, the police, judges, barristers, witnesses and the jury n the courtroom and in preparation of cases for legal proceedings. Unit G543: Forensic Psychology This option is designed to follow a rational progression through the criminal justice system, applying psychological understanding to four parts of the criminal justice experience: 1. Turning to crime – this area looks at three influences that have been used to explain how people become criminal: their upbringing (family), cognition (thinking patterns) and biology (genetic factors). 2. Making a case – this area examines how psychologists’ can inform the investigative process, methods of interviewing, factors influencing witnesses, detecting lies and creating an offender profile. 3. Reaching a verdict – this area looks at how psychologists’ have contributed to understanding the influences on jurors as they watch a trial unfold, and their discussions and group behaviours in coming to their verdict. 4. After a guilty verdict – as prison only works as a deterrent for about 35-40% of offenders, this area examines the ways in which psychology can inform the penal system by studying research into the effects of imprisonment, alternatives to imprisonment and a variety of treatment programmes. 4 Evaluation Think! about the study/approach/ method/issues/debates you have just covered. Ψ Approach/Perspective - Physiological, Social, Cognitive, Ind diffs, Developmental, Behaviourist, Psychodynamic Methods –Design- Independent/ Repeated measures (adv/disadv) Type of study (eg experiment, self-report, observation, case study – adv/disadv) Ψ Issues - Participants (representative?) setting, controls Ecological Validity (how realistic is it; can the findings be applied to everyday life?) Longitudinal and snapshot (is the study conducted over a long period or one point in time?) Qualitative and Quantitative data (is the data descriptive or numerical?) Usefulness (how is the research/theory/model useful in terms of how it explains human behaviour?) Application (how can the results of the research/theory/model be applied in everyday life settings?) Ethics (consent/informed consent, deception, withdrawal, debriefing, confidentiality, protection of participants, observation without consent) Generalisability (can these findings be applied to all individuals/situations?) Reliability (is the method used within the research/theory/model consistent?) Validity (is the method used within the research/theory/model measuring what it is supposed to?) Ψ Debates – Determinism and Free will (does this study/perspective suggest we have freewill or that our behaviour/experience is determined?) Reductionism and Holism (do the results of the study focus on one single level of explanation, ignoring others or do they consider many explanations?) Nature and Nurture (is this characteristic/behaviour due to genetics or learning?) Individual and situational explanations (can this behaviour be explained by the situation/environment or is it due to personal characteristics?) Ethnocentrism (can this behaviour be considered to be biased towards one ethnic group or society?) Approach bias (does the study support the beliefs of a specific approach?) Psychology as a Science (is the method used within the study rigorous, ie. objective, reliable, falsifiable?) 5 G543: Exam Guidance 1. 10 Mark Question: o Describe a study (aims, method, procedure, result, conclusions) o Describe the theory (7 points for the theory and 3 marks for the study to support) 2. 15 Mark o o o Question: PCEC: Point, Comment, Evidence, Comment Followed by a slap back However, PCEC (the slap back must contradict the point you just made) For example, if you have just said the internal validity is high and appropriate slap back would be that the ecological validity is low. It would make sane to slap back with the research is reductionist! Think of it as a game of tennis. You are rallying to see who can score the point. Therefore, it makes no sense to slap back with an argument that doesn’t challenge what you have just written! For Example: The Biological approach to an extent can provide an explanation for criminal behaviour as the research into the biological approach is often high in ecological validity. This has the effect that the research has more relatable findings that can be as a result generalised to real life situations and therefore can why people turn to crime. For instance, Wilson conducted a content analysis of real case of violent behaviour and the reasons behind this behaviour. The study had high ecological validity as it used content analysis of homicide cases in Detroit in 1972, analysing age and sex of perpetrator and victim to test the evolutionary theory. The Biological approach can therefore be used to provide an explanation for criminal behaviour as biological research uses real life cases and therefore can be used to explain that individuals commit crimes because of their gender. Slap Back On the other hand, the biological approach can be seen as being limited in its ability to provide an explanation for Criminal Behaviour as much of research into explaining why people turn to crime is ethnocentric. Therefore, the findings cannot be generalised to wider populations and therefore cannot provide valid explanations for why people turn to crime. For example, Daly and Wilson conducted a study to examine gender and age patterns in crime, using homicide cases from 1972 in Detroit. They found that males are likely to commit homicide in areas where there is a low life expectancy than females. Therefore, we cannot conclude that all males in areas with short life expectancies will commit crimes as an explanation for criminal behaviour as those circumstances might be limited to those area of Detroit. 6 Topic 1 – Turning to Crime 1. Upbringing Disrupted families Learning from others Poverty and disadvantaged neighbourhoods 2. Cognition Criminal thinking patterns Moral development and crime Social cognition 3. Biology Brain dysfunction Genes and serotonin Gender 7 Topic 1 – Turning to Crime What makes a criminal? What do you know about the Kray Twins? Ronnie and Reggie Kray were a pair of identical twins who were famous London gangsters in the 1960s. They ran a number of nightclubs and socialised with actors, lords and politicians, but were also involved in robbery, arson, torture and murder. A dedicated squad of detectives brought them to justice in 1969 and they were both given life sentences. But here's the thing: Ronnie suffered from paranoid schizophrenia, was volatile and violent and provoked most of their atrocities; Reggie would have liked to "go straight" or at least turn their business into an ordinary (ie not especially violent) criminal operation, but was dominated by his brother. It was claimed that a jealous Ronnie actually murdered Reggie's wife! So, were these twin brothers born to a life of murder and evil? Or could things have worked out differently for them and their victims? Your first unit in Forensic Psychology looks at what makes some people turn to crime, while other people never seem to feel the temptation. In the past, this used to be viewed as a purely moral question, and people who did "stray from the path" were regarded as wicked or weak. Psychology is more concerned with finding out the reasons behind criminal or deviant behaviour, rather than judging it or preventing it (though psychologists do hope their research will help prevent it too). It is never easy or straightforward to explain why people turn to crime. We must be careful to avoid reductionist (simplistic) or deterministic (criminal behaviour is outside the control of the individual) arguments. It must be remembered that everyone has free will; we choose whether or not to break the law. Forensic Psychology has, however, considered a number of different explanations of criminal behaviour. Studies of upbringing, cognition (thought processes) and biology can all contribute to our understanding of criminality, but none of these factors alone can explain why a person turns to crime. It is the interaction between these factors and individual differences between people that influences whether or not people turn to crime, and this means that the explanation for crime is very complex and that criminality is very hard to predict. 8 Theories of Upbringing The Theories/Studies 1. Farrington (2001) – Detangling the Link Between Disrupted Families and Delinquency 2. Sutherland– Theory of Differential Association 3. Wikstrom & Tafel (2003) - The Peterborough Youth Study The main approach in this area is social psychology. Many would argue that the biggest influence on criminality is the family. If your family are criminals it is likely that you will also be a criminal. However, this is obviously a very deterministic explanation, as it ignores individual differences, some people do manage to buck the trend and turn their lives around. Conversely, some people from law abiding families go on to become criminals. Subsection = Disrupted families - do problem families produce problem children? Many would argue that the biggest influence on criminality is the family. If your family are criminals it is likely that you will also be a criminal. However, this is obviously a very deterministic explanation, as it ignores individual differences, some people do manage to buck the trend and turn their lives around. Conversely, some people from law abiding families go on to become criminals. e.g. Juby & Farrington (2001): Disentangling the link between disrupted families and delinquency. There have been increases in marital instability since the 1960s. There have also been increases in juvenile delinquency since that time. Much research has been done into the link between these two factors – is family disruption associated with delinquent and criminal behaviour? This study by Judy and Farrington aimed to investigate the extent of the relationship between delinquency and disrupted families and to consider the difficulties in studying the association between “broken homes” and delinquency. According to the authors, “broken homes” and “disrupted families” are unsatisfactory terms in that they include a wide range of family types and experiences. Studies that simply look at intact versus broken homes often fail to address the fact that in some intact families there is parental disharmony and high conflict which can also have a negative impact on delinquency rates. This study, then, aimed to look not simply at broken vs. intact homes but also to disentangle a number or pre- and post- disruption variables to consider their effect on delinquency. These included: Pre-disruption variables Reasons for the disruption Timing of the disruption Gender of the lost parent Level of conflict Post-disruption variables Gender of the custodial parent Subsequent family reconstitution 9 Disrupted families 1. Key Study: 1.1.1 Disrupted families Farrignton, ‘Disentangling the link between disrupted families and delinquency', British Journal of Criminology, 41 (l), 22-40 Hoys from disrupted families who continued living with The OCR specification focuses on four types of sampling their mothers had similar delinquency rates to boys from method, random sampling, stratified sampling, self- intact harmonious families. selecting and opportunity. However, researchers Discussion: Juby and Farrington concluded from their Approach: Social/developmental schools measured individual characteristics such as evidence that the multiple stressors involved in separation Aim: To compare delinquency rates among boys intelligence and personality. Interviews with the participants' explain the link between disrupted families and delinquency. living in permanently disrupted families at age 15 parents were carried out annually from age 8 to age 14-15 by They concluded that disrupted families in general are with delinquency rates among those living in intact families a psychiatric social worker. Parents (mainly mothers) associated with relatively high delinquency rates but they reported on family income, parenting practices and family Hypothesis: That delinquency is more common among boys argued that analysis of the relationship should not be situation (for example, parental separation). Teachers also from simplistic. Some kinds of disrupted families are criminogenic sometimes use mixed approaches or approaches that do not fall clearly into one of those categories. The term 'representative population- has been used here to refer to a method which should more properly be called 'purposive sampling’. This is when researchers seek out participants who share the characteristics of their target population, excluding others that do not. They hope to capture in this completed questionnaires when the boys were age 8, 10, 12 compared to boys from intact families (likely to lead to crime) but so are some kinds of intact way a representative sample of that population but it cannot claim to be representative in the true statistical and 14 providing data on topics such as truancy, aggressive families (those with high parental conflict). Equally some sense. For example, if all the young offenders in one Method: Prospective longitudinal survey behaviour and school behaviour. Searches were carried out kinds of disrupted families - in particular where the mother is institution participate in a study the sample cannot claim to in the Criminal Record Office for data on criminal offences a stable figure - are no more criminogenic than intact be representative of all young offenders in that country. excluding minor offences such as traffic infractions. harmonious families. However, it is a practical approach that will provide meaningful insights into the attitudes of the target group. permanently •'u :i. disrupted families (broken homes) ;11 I.' 1-'^ Results: Juby and Farrington found that: Delinquency rates were higher among 75 boys who The Cambridge Study is a prospective, longitudinal survey of were living in permanently disrupted families on their boys born in South London in the 1950s. It is called the fifteenth birthday compared to those boys living in Cambridge study despite the fact that the participants lived in intact families. Twenty-nine per cent of those from London because the research team is based at the University of Cambridge. It is longitudinal in design because disrupted families were convicted as juveniles as data was collected over a long period - since the 1960s. It is prospective because the data was originally collected when (odds ratio = 1.9). opposed to 18 per cent of boys from intact families Prospective longitudinal studies allow hypotheses to be tested even though they do not use the experimental method. This is because the researchers the boys were aged 8-9 before it was known how the lives of the boys would turn out. Since then data has been collected make predictions then test those predictions by studying long term outcomes. at regular periods enabling researchers to monitor how the lives of the participants were shaped by their early V _______________________________________ y experiences. The key study discussed in 1.1.3 — the Peterborough Youth Study is by contrast a What is an odds ratio? snapshot or cross-sectional study. It examined Odds are the number of times an event occurs/ number of the offending behaviour of all the Year 10 students times it does not occur. It is a measure similar to in state schools in Peterborough at one particular probability. time. To find the odds ratio you divide the odds of one group by the odds of the other group An odds ratio greater than 2 is considered significant. Delinquency rates were similar in disrupted families to Type of sample: Representative population Participants: 411 boys from 6 state schools in South London aged 8-9 at the start of the study in 1961. Procedure: Data were collected on the parents of the boys and the boys themselves. The data included juvenile convictions, juvenile self-reported delinquency and adult convictions. The participants were interviewed and tested at age 8, 10, 14 at school, at age 16, 18 and 21 in a research office, and at age 25, 32 and 46 in their homes. The tests in 2 those in intact high-conflict families (OR 1.0 to 1.4). Key LO-M/F-DI: Lone mother after father's death LO-M/F-LEF; Lone mother after father left LO-M/F-STP: Lone mother with stepfather RELATIVES: Living with relatives LONE F: Lone father NON RELATIVES: Living with nonrelatives, e.g. foster family Boys who lost their mothers were more likely to be delinquent than boys who lost their fathers. Odds ratio for juvenile convictions for those who lost mothers was 3.7. Disruptions caused by parental disharmony (broken homes) were more damaging than disruptions caused by parental death. Graph to show percentage of delinquents in differently composed households Source. H. Juby and D P. Farrington (2001). 'Disentangling the link between disrupted families and delinquency'. British Journal of Criminology. 41(1 ):22-40. Reproduced with purmission o( Oxford University Press Subsection = Learning from others – can social learning theory and learning from others explain criminal behaviour? e.g. Sutherland (1939) – Differential Association Hypothesis KEY TERM Differential association theory = Sutherland’s theory of crime stating that criminal behaviour is learned through social groups. The more associations a person has with attitudes favourable to criminal activity in proportion to attitudes unfavourable to criminal activity, the more likely they are to commit crimes. Edwin H.Sutherland (1939) used social learning theory principles to produce his differential association theory. Sutherland suggests that criminal behaviour is learned through exposure to criminal norms, which happens within the family and peer group. Criminal behaviour results from two factors: Learned attitudes (criminal behaviour becomes the accepted social norm) Imitation of specific acts (how to do the behaviour is observed and then imitated) Sutherland presented his theory in the form of nine principles. 1. Criminal behaviour is learnt. 2. Criminal behaviour is learned in interaction with other persons in a process of communication. 3. The principle part of the learning of criminal behaviour occurs within intimate personal groups. 4. When criminal behaviour is learned, the learning includes the techniques of committing the crime, which are sometimes very complicated, sometimes very simple and the specific direction of motives, drives, rationalisations and attitudes. 5. The specific direction of motives and drives is learned from definitions of the legal codes as favourable or unfavourable. 6. A person becomes delinquent because of an excess of definitions favourable to violation of law over definitions unfavourable to violation of law. 7. Differential associations (number of contacts with criminals over non-criminals) may vary in frequency, duration, priority and intensity. 8. The process of learning criminal behaviour by association with criminal and anticriminal patterns involves all of the mechanisms that are involved in any other learning. 9. While criminal behaviour is an expression of general needs and values, it is not explained by those general needs and values, since non-criminal behaviour is an expression of the same needs and values. Conclusions: Sutherland’s theory is based on TWO core assumptions: • Deviance occurs when people define a certain human situation as an appropriate occasion for violating social norms or criminal laws. • Definitions of the situation are acquired through an individual’s history of past experience. The theory emphasises the social-psychological processes by which people produce subjective definitions of whether an action is criminal. Evaluation of Sutherland’s theory: Strengths Sutherland’s study accounts for what he called white collar crime, and explains how in the world of politics, business and the professions illegal practices (e.g.tax evasion, fiddling expenses, bribery etc.) could be widely accepted as the social norm for how things are done. This showed that crime was not all carried out by deviant psychopathic individuals from impoverished backgrounds. This theory gives a useful explanation for crimes carried out in peer groups (e.g. the violence of street gang crime). The theory is useful as it has been highly generative, that is it has led other Psychologists to researcher and refine the impact of differential associations on criminality. Weaknesses/limitations Sutherland's theory is reductionist (takes a purely social learning approach) and ignores other factors such as biology (see principle 1) which might explain criminal behaviour better than the differential association theory does – for example it fails to account for acts of impulsive violence or crimes of passion carried out by those who have not been exposed to criminal norms or witnessed criminal behaviour. The theory also fails to explain crimes carried out by people acting individually. The theory does not address individual differences in susceptibility to the influence of others and assumes all people will acquire criminal behaviour in the same way, and according to principle 7 of his theory criminality ought to be predictable by quantifying exposure to differential associations. This could only be true if there were no individual differences between people. Sutherland fails to explain the processes by which criminality is learned, does he mean by classical and operant conditioning? Is his theory a precursor to social learning theory? The study was carried out in 1939 and is outdated in parts. For example, modern theorists consider that the media can have a strong influence on criminal behaviour and attitudes to crime (see principle 3). 12 Key Study: 1.1.2 Learning from others Akers• - Food for thought 'Social Learning and deviant behaviour: a specific test of a general theory’, American Sociological Review, 44, 636—55 2) Approach: Behaviourist Differential peer association - a scale of three items measuring how many of respondents’ friends use the Aim: To test social learning theory of deviant behaviour substance. Differential reinforcement - social: This with survey data oil adolescent drinking and drug Can you think of factors that might explain why Variance is a measure of Ihe amount of variation within young people use/don't use drink/druc other than the values of a variable. If a model explains 55 per cent of the variance then (in rough terms) if you know those investigated in this study? SLT appears to the independent data for someone then you can young people (68 per < of the variance compared lo predict the outcome and you will be right 55 per cent of Ihe lime. 55 per cent of variance explained). Can you think of any reasons why this migh the case? assessed encouragement, praise, reward and punishment behaviour Method: Self-report Design: Cross-sectional (snapshot) survey Type of sample: Sample of representative population I’niiicipnnts: Approximately 2500 male and female adolescents attending grades 7-12 (age 13-18) in three midwestern states in the USA. Procedure: Parental permission was obtained for the explain more fully drug-lakir lhan drinking among from parents/friends over use/abstinence. Differential reinforcement - social/non-social: This analysed positive The factors selected for analysis together have strong and negative outcomes of use/ abstinence including explanatory power regarding young people’s drinking- and social reinforcers and non-social rcinforcers such as a drug-taking habits, although they do not completely good high or a bad high or feeling good or bad explain afterwards. predictive variable was imitation, although it was still differences between individuals. The least expected direction. The most effective predictive variable Research based on self-reporl Is sometimes seen as iinrolialhle becauoo Ihoro i:i no objective dala A subsample of respondents was selected for a follow-up set was found to be differential association and within that collected. We cannot know whelher respondents are interview to check reliability of responses. Dependent the specific factor of differential peer association was the variables were abstinencc-use of alcohol measured by a 6- single most important variable. administration of a questionnaire about abstinence from/use ofalcohol and marijuana to the participants. regression techniques. point frequency-of-use scale: 1 = never; 6 = nearly every day. Abuse was measured by asking the respondents to check whether or not they had experienced problems on move than one occasion such as ’having an accident’, ‘not being able to remember later’ what they had done. Influenced by Bandura’s (1971) Social Learning Theory (SLT) and Sutherland's Differential Association theory, The data collected were analysed using multiple the experimenters selected five concepts as predictor variables: imitation; differential associations; definitions; differential social reinforcement and differential The purpose of multiple regression is to learn more about the relationship between several predictor significantly related to the dependent variable in the Discussion: It is perhaps surprising that imitation comes so low in the list of explanatory factors, as the concept of variables and a dependent (criterion) variable. Multiple regression procedures are widely used in research lo Iry imitative learning was originally at the heart of SLT. to answer the general question "what is the best predictor of . . .7’ The general computational problem that needs to be solved in multiple regression analysis imitative learning and the researchers concluded that their is to fit a straight line to a number of points. In the simplest case - one dependent and one independent variable - one can visualise this as a scatterplot with a However SLT is in fact broader than just the concept of findings supported SLT. Moicover the results support Sutherland's 1941 theory that both differential association and definitions of acceptable/unacceptable behaviour are key to an understanding of why young people develop deviant behaviours. The fact the peer group influence is so Imitation index: Total of admired models (parents; line of regression. In the multivariate case, when there is more than one independent variable, the regression line cannot be visualised in Iwo dimensional space, but friends; other adults) whom the respondent reported can be still be computed. In analysing factors in school imitate their behaviour, but more importantly because they having observed using the substance (alcohol/drug). achievement, for example, one could have a number of predictor variables, for example IQ, motivation and self esteem. II is possible lo construct a linear equation containing all those variables. / provide social reinforcement for our behaviour and provide non-social reinforcement. Definitions: Three aspects of definitions were examined - neutralising definitions, for example, denial/excuse; law-abiding/violating definitions; respondents' own approval/ disapproval of use. Differential association:Three aspects of differential associations were examined: I) Respondents' perception of approving/ disapproving attitudes towards them by a) adults whose opinion they valued b) other teenagers whose opinion they valued. Results: The results of the regression analyses show strong support for the social learning theory of adolescent alcohol and drug behaviour. When all the independent variables arc incorporated into the equation, the model explains 55 per cent of the variance in drinking behaviour and 68 per cent of the variance in marijuana behaviour. high is not surprising, as previous research has shown this. Friends arc important in part because we obsei ve and norms by which we define particular behaviours as acceptable/unacceptable. telling (he truth or whether their answers are affected by social desii-eability factors. In this study however, the sample size is very large (2500) and generally Ihe larger Ihe sample size Ihe more reliable Ihe conclusions drawn from the data. V y Subsection = Poverty & Disadvantaged Neighbourhoods Although there is an undisputed link between disadvantaged areas and likelihood of being involved in crime, it is far from the case that the poorer or more disadvantaged you are the more crime you will be involved in: it simply is not the case that the poorest people commit the most crime. In itself poverty is not simple to define. For example there is absolute poverty, where people have nothing and struggle to meet their basic needs of food, shelter and warmth, and then there is relative poverty where people have less than others and may not be struggling to live, but may have significantly less than other people in terms of financial security, disposable income, possessions etc. In terms of the relationship between poverty/disadvantaged neighbourhoods and crime, there is not a direct link. Instead, poverty and disadvantage are associated with the risk factors that may lead a person into crime. For example government figures show that the most disadvantaged 5% are 100 times more likely than the most advantaged 50% to have multiple problems which increase their risk of being involved in crime, such as: mood disorders, conduct disorder, police contact, cannabis use, and alcohol abuse. The question for researchers then is to investigate how individual risk factors, lifestyle and living in a disadvantaged area/poverty interact to lead a person into crime, and to consider the relative importance of individual and situational factors in turning to crime. Just how big an influence does social disadvantage have on whether a person turns to crime or not? Wikstrom & Tafel (2003) - The Peterborough Youth Study Aim: To investigate why young people offend. Approach: Social Type of Data: Details: A cross-sectional study was carried out on nearly 2000 year 10 students (14-15) [representing all the year 10 students in all 13 state schools in Peterborough, UK] Students completed a questionnaire, and a random sample of them were also interviewed. Results: 44.8% of males and 30.6% of females had committed at least one crime (violence, vandalism, shoplifting, burglary and car theft) during the year 2000. 9.8% of males and 3.8% of females have committed a serious crime of theft. High-frequency offenders commit a wide range of crimes. One in eight were reported to or caught by the police for their last offence. Offenders are victimised more than non-offenders. Violent offenders are more likely to become victims of violence. Offenders are more likely to abuse drink and/or drugs. Explanatory factors include: Family social position (social class, ethnicity, family composition) Individual characteristics (self-control, morality, family bonds and monitoring) Social situation (family and school bonds, opportunity for truancy) Lifestyle and routine activities Community context (neighbourhood disadvantage and school attended) How these factors influence offending: o Gender, community contexts, family social class, ethnicity and split families are not strong predictors of adolescent offending. However, they are significant predictors of the youths' individual characteristics and lifestyles. o The youths' individual characteristics and the way they live their lives strongly affects their involvement in crime. o For well-adjusted youths lifestyle is a less important risk factor for their offending, while for average youths (youths with neither strong risk, nor strong protective factors), lifestyle has a strong impact on their level of offending. o The key new finding of this study is the strong interaction between individual characteristics and lifestyle in producing offending risk; high-risk lifestyles have a different importance for the risk of offending for youths with different levels of individual risk factor. 15 Conclusions: Wikstrom & Tafel suggest three groups of adolescent offenders: The three groups of offenders identified in the Peterborough Youth Study 1.Propensity Induced These youths have an enduring propensity to offend, it is a personality or individual characteristic. It is only a small group, but is responsible for a disproportionate number of offences. The have a number of risk factors such as weak family and social bonds, low levels of self-control, anti-social values, low levels of shame and a high risk lifestyle. 2.Lifestyle Dependent This group are average in terms of social adjustment. They offend when they have high risk lifestyles, for example socialising with delinquent peers and using drugs or alcohol. 3.Situationally limited These are youths who may occasionally offend if they are exposed to high levels of situational risk. Substance abuse seems to be of particular importance in explaining their occasional offending. As their offending is very closely linked to the situation they find themselves in, they are unlikely to re-offend. Suggested targeted strategies aimed at reducing offending the prevention of the propensity-induced offenders' criminality may primarily be a question of addressing more fundamental problems arising from their development history and their current family and school social situation (through social preventative strategies rather than deterrent strategies for the lifestyle dependent adolescent offenders, it is argued that strategies focusing on situationally oriented measures (e.g. mobilising parents and schools in influencing their lifestyles, for example through improved parental monitoring and combating truancy) may have a greater prospect for success. for the situationally-limited adolescent offenders, it is questionable whether there is a need to develop any particular crime prevention strategy. This group's occasional offending is perhaps best interpreted as a result of uncharacteristic behaviour triggered by factors, such as alcohol intoxication, that sometimes may act to momentarily reduce their inhibitions toward offending. The study shows that youths with many individual risk factors offend frequently, and that there is a strong interaction between individual risk factors and lifestyle in producing offending risk. In contrast, youths with many individual protective factors rarely offended. Social disadvantage was NOT a strong predictor of delinquency in comparison to individual factors and lifestyle factors, but those from a lower social class (more poverty) did have more risk factors than those from a more comfortable background. 16 Summary: Upbringing In considering the influence of upbringing, we have looked at how families are clearly influential in a person’s chances of becoming criminal. It seems clear that disrupted families with criminal parents give any individual a high risk of following them into crime. We can also assume that early intervention programs may mitigate (lessen) the risk of a person becoming a criminal. If a young person belongs to a deviant peer group the risk factors increase as criminal acts become norms of behaviour. Each group will make their own definition of the rules they will adopt and those they will ignore. If the family lives in an area of poverty or disadvantage, the risks increase as they feel alienated from the mainstream of society and its values of an honest work ethic based on a good education leading to a job or career. There may be individuals with characteristically high-risk behaviours who adopt a permanently criminal lifestyle while others are more influenced by situational factors and therefore are potentially easier to rehabilitate. 17 Theories of Cognition The Theories/Studies 1. Yochelson & Samenow (1976) - A study of thinking patterns in criminals 2. Kolberg (1958)- Moral Development in children 3. Gudjonsson and Sigurdsson (2007): Motivation for offending The main approach in this area is Cognitive psychology. The basic assumption here is that criminals must think in a fundamentally different way to law-abiding citizens. They must be able to rationalise their own behaviour and decide the risks involved are worth the possible gains. The big problem here is how do we ever really know how and what another person is thinking? Subsection = Criminal thinking patterns Do criminals think differently from other people? The basic assumption here is that criminals must think in a fundamentally different way to law-abiding citizens, e.g. they must be able to rationalise their own behaviour and decide the risks involved are worth the possible gains. Free Will vs Determinism How far are cognitive factors under the control of the individual (free will) or are they predetermined by other factors (determinism) when someone has committed a crime? For the most part we take responsibility for our guilt, however when some offenders are viewed mentally unstable they may make a ‘plea of diminished responsibility (behaviour the outcome of lost sanity, which maybe temporary).’ The rules a court uses to decide whether to uphold the plea are the McNaghten Rules and in the event the plea is upheld usually indefinite imprisonment is the consequence. In order for an offender to be found guilty and sentenced they need to be of Mens Rea, ‘guilty mind’ and Actus Reus, ‘guilty act.’ 18 Yochelson & Samenow (1976) - A study of thinking patterns in criminals Background: Yochelson & Samenow were Dr’s who worked together at St. Elizabeth’s Psychiatric Hospital in Washington, DC where they profiled and counselled male offenders using psychodynamic (Freudian) techniques. Aims: 1. To understand the make-up of the criminal personality. 2. To establish techniques that could be used to alter the personality disorders that produce crime. 3. To encourage an understanding of legal responsibility. 4. To establish techniques that can be effective in preventing criminal behaviour. Approach: Cognitive Details: A longitudinal study conducted over 14 years. 255 male participants from various backgrounds; black, white, those from the inner city, suburbs, wealthy, poor etc., were interviewed at various points over the years. The population was mainly those confined to a mental hospital who had been found guilty of a crime, but pleaded insanity and a roughly equal group of convicted prisoners, not confined to a mental institution. Most of the participants dropped out of the study, only 30 completed the programme of interviews. Results: Criminals….. Are restless, dissatisfied and irritable While at school, considered requests from their teachers and parents as impositions Continually set themselves apart from others Want to live a life of excitement, at any cost Are habitually angry Are lacking empathy Feel under no obligation to anyone or anything Are poor at responsible decision-making, having pre-judged situations. By the criteria set at the beginning of the study only 9/255 criminals showed any improvement. This shows little support for the psychoanalytical methods of treatment in which both Yochelson and Samenow had been trained during their psychiatric training. From early on in the study it became clear to the researchers that the participants were manipulating the study in order to improve their situation and were not improving their bvr at all but instead were, for example, lying, abusing drugs/alcohol 19 and stealing hospital supplies. As a result, Yochelson and Samenow changed the focus of the study to investigate criminal thinking patterns (as the data on reasons for criminality would be invalidated by lying). This moved the focus of the study from a psychoanalytic perspective to a cognitive one. The key finding on criminal thinking was that 52 thinking patterns were distinguished in the criminal personality. These were considered to be ‘errors’ in thinking and while not claimed to be unique to criminals were believed by the researchers to be used more by criminals than non-criminals (although since the study had no control group of non-criminals to make comparisons with this assumption was not proven in this study) Examples of criminal thinking “errors” Criminal thinking patterns Characterised by fear and a need for power and control. Other features include a search for perfection, lying and inconsistencies or fragmentation in thinking Automatic thinking errors These include a lack of empathy and trust, a failure to accept obligations, and a secretive communication style Crime-related thinking These include optimistic fantasising about specific criminal errors acts with no regard to deterrent factors Conclusions: 52 thinking patterns were distinguished in the criminal personality. These were considered to be ‘errors’ in thinking. However, as there was no control group of noncriminals we cannot be certain that these traits are only found in criminals. Subsection = Moral Development Morals are a set of norms and values, usually learnt from our parents about what is right and wrong. In the UK the age of criminal responsibility is 10; children over 10 are deemed to clearly know the difference between right and wrong. Other countries have different age limits; German 18, Scotland 8. Piaget showed that children of different ages think differently, and he also associated this with different types of moral reasoning, and his work heaviliy influenced Kohlberg. Kohlberg’s research and the work it has generated helped to answer the following questions: Are there different stages of moral reasoning where the reasons for acting morally, that is doing the right thing, differ between the stages? Does the development of these stages occur over time? If someone has an immature level of moral development for their age does this make them more likely to commit a crime? 20 Kohlberg was heavily influenced by the work of Piaget and believed that children’s cognition developed through stages. His research involved presenting groups of boys with moral dilemmas and then asking them questions about them. His most famous moral dilemma is below. Heinz Dilemma Heinz’s wife was suffering from terminal cancer. In an effort to save her he went to a chemist who had developed a cure which might help her. Unfortunately, the chemist wanted much more money for his cure than Heinz could afford and refused to sell it for less. Even when Heinz borrowed enough money for half the cost of the drug, the chemist still refused to sell it to him. Having no other means of getting the drug, Heinz broke into the chemist’s laboratory and stole it. Should he have broken into the laboratory? Why? Should the chemist insist on the inflated price for his invention? Does he have the right? What should happen to Heinz? What if Heinz did not love his wife – does that change anything? What if the dying person was a stranger? Should Heinz have stolen the drug anyway? Level 1 Pre-morality Level 2 Conventional Morality Level 3 Post-conventional morality Stage 1 Punishment & Obedience Orientation – Doing what is right because of fear of punishment Stage 2 Hedonistic Orientation – Doing what is right for personal gain, perhaps a reward Stage 3 Interpersonal Concordance Orientation – Doing what is right according to the majority Stage 4 Law & Order Orientation – Doing what is right because it is your duty and helps society. Laws must be obeyed for the common good. Stage 5 Social Contract or Legalistic Orientation – Doing what is morally right even if it is against the law because the law is too restrictive. Stage 6 Universal Ethical Principles Orientation – Doing what is right because of our inner conscience which has absorbed the principles of justice, equality and sacredness of human life. Aim: - To find evidence in support of a progression through stages of moral development. Procedure: - 58 working and middle class boys from Chicago aged 7, 10, 13, 16 were given a two hour interview with 10 dilemma (like the Heinz) to solve. Some of these boys were followed up at 3 yearly intervals and the study was repeated in 1969 in the UK, Mexico, Taiwan, USA and Yucatan. Findings: - Younger boys tended to perform at stages 1 and 2, with older boys at stages 3 and 4. This pattern was consistent across different cultures. Conclusions: - The evidence does support the idea of a stage theory of moral development. More recent research by Thornton & Reid (1982) with criminal samples suggests that criminals committing crime for financial gain show more immature reasoning than those committing violent crimes. 21 Subsection = Social Cognition Social Cognition refers to the way our thoughts are influenced by the people we mix with but also how we can understand social phenomena by looking at an individual’s cognitions. We all justify and explain our behaviours using either internal or external attributions. An internal attribution is when a person accepts full responsibility for their own behaviour and sees the cause as being within themselves. An external attribution is when a person sees the cause of their behaviour as being an external factor, e.g. ‘I was provoked, it’s his fault I hit him’, ‘I had a bad childhood’, ‘I’ve got no money”. A criminal is considered rehabilitated when they can fully accept responsibility for their crime, in other words have an internal attribution, they accept their guilt. An extension of this, and one which we will look at in the study in this subsection is the self-serving bias. This points out that we tend to make attributions that present us in the best light. If something good happens which will increase our self esteem or raise us up in the esteem of others, we make an internal attribution, e.g. “My target grade for Psychology was a D, but I got a B. This was because I am a good student and worked hard.” (supporting the actor-observer effect – this is an internal or individual attribution about the actor’s own behaviour). On the other hand, if the behaviour reduces our esteem or makes others judge us negatively, then we make external or situational attributions for it: “I got a U in Psychology A-level when my target grade was a D, but that was because the teacher didn’t teach me properly/my parents wouldn’t help me with my studies by buying me the laptop I needed…” This self-serving bias means we take the credit for anything that makes us look or feel good, and make excuses or try to give justifications for those things that make us look or feel bad. Attributional biases are faulty cognitions, and in the criminal they can be a barrier to their rehabilitation, as someone who has committed a crime can’t really be considered to be rehabilitated unless they can fully accept responsibility for their crime and accept their guilt. Sexual assault and rape are very serious crimes which carry long prison sentences and which follow the offender beyond the sentence as they are required to be recorded on the database of known sex offenders. The reason for this is that they are statistically likely to reoffend. This suggests that they are not able to be successfully treated or rehabilitated in prison. Social cognition Gudjohnsson Key Study: 1.2.3 Social cognition , ‘Motivation for offending and probation’, Personality anti Individual personality. A study among young offenders on Differences, 42, 1243-53 Approach: Cognitive/individual differences Aim: To examine ilie relationship between motivation for offending and personality, anger problems and attitudes towards offending Method: Self-report Design: Cross-sectional (snapshot) survey Type of sample: Representative sample population Participants: 128 male youths, age range 15-21, from Reykjavik, Iceland, who had been given a conditional discharge after a guilty plea. Most of the offences were property offences followed by car crimes, assatdt and criminal damage. Procedure: Participants had pleaded guilty to an offence and were required to attend regular supervision sessions with a probation officer during their period of discharge. Each participant was asked during a visit to their probation officer whether they would be willing participate in a study into offending and personality. Several psychological scales were administered: the Offending Motivation Questionnaire (OMQ); the Gudjonsson Compliance Scale; the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (El’Q) and Eysenck Impulsivity Scale (E1S); the Novaco Anger Scale and the Blame Attribution Inventory. 2. Offending Motivation Questionnaire (OMQ) (Gudjonsson ami Sigurdsson, 2004): A 22-item questionnaire that measures the motivation behind offending which comprises four main factors: 1. Compliance: Items relating to the offence being committed in order to please a peer or because the person felt pressured into it (example, ‘Gave in to Excitement: Items indicating that the main motive for the offence was fun or excitement (example, ‘Did it for Provocation: Items involving taking revenge, losing control, and selfdefence (example, ‘To take revenge on somebody'). 4. three main ‘superfactor’ personality dimensions (psychoticism, extraversion, neuroticism). See Psychology A2 for OCR textbook Section 1.1, p.4 for more on the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire The Eysenck Impulsivity, Venturesomeness and Empathy Scale (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1991): This is a 54-item questionnaire which was included because it has been argued that primary personality traits (for example impulsivity, need for stimulation or sensation seeking) may be better predictors The Novaco Anger Scale (Novaco, 1994): This two-part scale measures anger reactions. Part A contains items divided into three domains: cognitive, arousal and behavioural. Part B consists of items providing an index of angei intensity in potentially anger provoking situations. The Blame Attribution Inventory: This is a 42-item inventory, with a true-false format, which measures how offenders subscales: External Attribution (that is blaming the offence on provocation and society), Mental Element Attribution (that is blaming the offence on mental factors, such as poor self- excitement'). 3. 1975): A much tested personality questionnaire that measures attribute blame for a specific criminal act. It comprises three pressure from peer(s)'). 2. The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Eysenck and Eysenck, of delinquency than the three super-factors. Measures: The The Gudjonsson Compliance Scale (Gudjonsson, 1997): a scale comprising two factors: eagerness to please; conflict avoidance. This is a 20-item scale rated with true or false that measures the tendency of the person to go along uncritically with requests made by others in order to avoid conflict. The higher the score, the more compliant the individual is. Financial: Items reflecting a financial or a monetary need as the explanation for the offence (example, ‘In hope of financial gain'). control), and Guilt Feeling (that is the extent to which the offender expresses regret and feelings of remorse for the offence). Results: The first finding was that out of 120 cases, 103 respondents (86 per cent) said that they had committed the offence claims that they were pressured into crime, or that they had been trying to impress peers by committing the offence. It also shows in the company of others, Gudjonsson and Sigurdsson concluded that motivation for offending therefore must take into consideration peer group influence and pressure. that perceived peer pressure can encourage youngsters to offend (for example, being unable to resist offending when pressured to do so by peers). Excitement was found to be the single most important motive for youth offending. The excitement motive had the strongest overall relationship with the other psychological measures. This study also demonstrates the importance of anger in relation to offending motivation. It was an important predictor in three out of the four OMQ factors (excitement, provocation, and financial). This fits with Agnew’s (1985) strain theory which proposes that adolescents are pressured into delinquency by strong negative reactions, such as anger, that arise from adverse life events and frustration. Pood for thought How does this finding relate to the findings of Akers el al. (Key study 1.1.2) and the Peterborough Study (Key study 1.1.3)? The second finding was that the highest mean scores on the OMQ were for the excitement and financial motives. In addition, 38 per cent of the participants claimed that they did not think about the consequences of what they were doing and 36 per cent were very confident that they would get away with the offence. Food for thought How does this finding relate to the ‘superoptimism' variable in the Psychological Inventory of Criminal Thinking Styles (Key study 1.2.1)? Gudjonsson and Sigurdsson present the argument that there are individual differences in offending motivation factors and that these are related to personality variables. It was hypothesised that different types of motivation for the offence (compliance, provocation, excitement and financial) would be predicted by different psychological measures. It was found that neither Hxtraversion (El’Q) nor Empathy (EIQ) correlated with any of the OMQ factors. The Gudjonsson Compliance scale correlated significantly with the compliance motive, but not with any other OMQ factors. Psychoticism (EPQ) correlated significantly with the excitement and provocation motives. The Novaco Anger Scales correlated significantly with the excitement, provocation and financial factors. External attribution of blame (Gudjonsson Blame Attribution Inventory) correlated with the compliance motive and there was a significant negative correlation between guilt (Gudjonsson Blame Attribution Inventory) and the excitement motive. Discussion: The study shows that a compliant disposition is significantly related to participants’ The homogeneity of the sample (young men who had committed relatively minor offences) is both a strength and weakness. The strength is that there are likely to be fewer confounding variables associated with age. gender, substance abuse and repeated offending. The disadvantage is that the influences of different types of offences could not be ascertained and possible gender differences could not be investigated. These are invisible variables that may affect findings thlis reducing reliability Summary: theories of Cognition Research by Yochelson and others has suggested that criminals think differently from law-abiding people. They have certain biases in their thinking which means they see themselves existing apart from the mainstream of society without obligation to others, lacking empathy and seeking excitement. Their decision-making is poor and they lie habitually. A criminal’s moral development is restricted to the lower levels of Kohlberg’s hierarchy, reflecting the biases found by Yochelson. In stage one and stage two of Kohlberg’s theory a person does what is right to avoid getting into trouble or for a reward or personal gain. A criminal may believe that because the chances of getting into trouble are quite slim and the rewards for the crime are quite substantial then there is little moral pressure on them to behave within the law. It was also found that attributions are affected by the social context and social pressures surrounding the criminal. Understanding how criminals think offers potentially the greatest opportunity to change faulty thinking and reduce re-offending. Theories of Biology The Theories/Studies 1. Raine et al (1997/2002) – Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by PET 2. Brunner et al (1993) –A study of violence in a family with genetic abnormality 3. Daly & Wilson (2001) - Investigation of gender-related life expectancy The main approach in this area is Physiological psychology as this section focuses on reasons for turning to crime and clearly illustrates the nature/nurture debate. The basic assumption here is that criminals are biologically different to non-criminals. In particular, some bio-psychologists argue that special structures inside the brain control our social behaviour - giving us empathy for other people, a sense of conscience and a concern for the consequences of our actions. People without these brain structures might be heartless and impulsive with no sense of right and wrong. This is taking the NATURE side of the nature/nurture debate and the DISPOSITIONAL side of the dispositions vs situations debate. However, biological theories of crime are reductionist and determinist. These theories can only really explain certain types of crime, mainly violent ones. There is a danger of labelling individuals before a crime has actually been committed. That said, they are attractive and persuasive because they offer the possibilities of screening for criminality, intervention programmes and treatment. In reality, it is the interaction between upbringing, environment, cognitive processes, and biology and individual differences between people that influences whether or not they turn to crime. It must be remembered that everyone has free will; we choose whether or not to break the law. 26 Subsection = Brain Dysfunction Adrian Raine of the University of Southern California has conducted research using PET scanning and found abnormalities in some parts of the brain in violent criminals. He has found that low physiological arousal, birth complications, fearlessness and increased body size are early markers for later aggressive behaviour. Raine et al (1997) – Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by PET Background: Earlier work by Raine and others had demonstrated the relationship between low levels of activity in the prefrontal cortex and violent behaviour (Raine 1994). This is an area of the brain behind the forehead and Raine describes the function of this part of the brain as like ‘the emergency break on behaviour’, which prevents us from acting on violent or aggressive impulses. It is thought that the prefrontal cortex sends messages to the limbic system to tell us to fear the consequences of acting on our impulses. The limbic system is part of the old brain which governs our emotional and aggressive impulses. The hippocampus (remember Maguire) and amygdale are key structures in the limbic system. Raine suggests that violent offenders have shown abnormalities in these areas in the function of the different hemispheres (remember Sperry) and developments of brain imaging techniques have allowed research to be carried out into the possible brain dysfunction in violent offenders. Aim: To investigate patterns of brain activity in murderers compared to a matched sample of non-murderers using PET scanning Approach: Physiological Participants and Details: Experimental group of 41 participants charged with murder/manslaughter who had pleaded ‘not guilty by reasons of insanity’ (NGRI) but had been convicted (referred to as ‘murderers’) Murderers (mean age 34.3). There were 39 men and 2 women. 23 had a history of brain damage, 6 schizophrenia, 3 substance abuse, 2 affective disorders, 2 epilepsy, 3 learning disability/hyperactivity, 2 paranoid personality disorder A control group of 41 people were matched for sex and age (mean age 31.7 years) and who were similar in other ways, eg. 6 had schizophrenia who were matched with experimental group. Rest of group screened and showed no history of psychiatric illness. No participants were on medication. The IV is whether a participant is a murderer or not, the DV is the measures of brain activity and brain structure found using PET scans. 27 What is a PET scan? A brain positron emission tomography (PET) scan is an imaging test that uses a radioactive substance (called a tracer) to look for disease or injury in the brain. A PET scan requires a small amount of radioactive material (tracer) to be given through a vein, usually on the inside of your elbow. The tracer travels through your blood and collects in organs and tissues. The tracer helps the radiologist see certain areas or diseases more clearly. As the PET scanner detects signals from the tracer, a computer changes the results into 3-D pictures. The images are displayed on a monitor for your doctor to read. In Raine’s study, the P’s were given the tracer the carried out a ‘continuous performance task’ (a visual task that increases brain activity in the frontal lobes of normal participants-CPT) for 32 minutes before having their PET scan. Results: No difference on task performance but significant differences in brain metabolism of glucose in number of areas were found between murderers and controls Summary table of results for the prefrontal, limbic and corpus callusum of the murderers compared to the control group. Area of the Brain Findings Prefrontal cortex Lower glucose metabolism (i.e. less activity) in some prefrontal areas of the brain. Limbic system Different levels of activity in the amygdale and hippocampus Corpus Callosum Less activity in corpus callosum (structure that joins the two halves of the brain). Conclusions: Raine at al. conclude that the study shows that murderers pleading NGRI have significant differences in glucose metabolism in certain areas of the brain compared to the non-murderers. They also suggest that reduced brain activity in certain areas may be one of the many predispositions toward violence. The areas identified as having abnormal activity are associated with LACK OF FEAR, LOWER SELF-CONTROL, INCREASED AGGRESSION, IMPULSIVE BEHAVIOUR AND PROBLEMS CONTROLLING AND EXPRESSING EMOTIONS - All of these could lead to an increased risk of committing acts of extreme violence. However, although the study suggests that violence has a biological cause, Raine DID NOT conclude that there were only biological causes for violence, just that there might be a PRE-DISPOSITION to violence in some people, depending on environmental triggers. 28 29 Subsection = Genes and Serotonin Background: Inside every living thing are sets of instructions called GENES. Genes are the building blocks of DNA and they tell bodies how to grow and develop. Genes affect whether you have have brown eyes or blue and influence things like your height, build and possibly your intelligence and personality. Scientists have been researching genes since the 19th century monk Gregor Mendel discovered how genes were passed on in pea plants. However, we're still a long way from identifying what every single gene does, particularly since most genes work in complicated combinations with other genes. We don't even know how many genes there are in humans; estimates range from 20,000 to 150,000. Could there be a gene for criminality? First, it's important to clear up what this means. Criminality is a social construct - what's criminal in one society or at one time might be perfectly acceptable somewhere else. Our genes don't know anything about "laws" so you can't have a gene that makes you want to break any particular law. However, genes certainly can give people PREDISPOSITIONS. Predisposition is a natural built-in tendency creatures have to behave or develop a certain way. For example, human babies have a predisposition to walk and talk and learn to do these things with very little encouragement; however, they can still end up not doing these things if they are paralysed, deaf or neglected. Genes might give some people a predisposition to take risks, not think about consequences, be aggressive or behave selfishly. Even that doesn't mean they have to become criminals. They could become contestants on reality TV shows instead. In the 1960s it was proposed that males with an extra Y chromosome (males should have one X and one Y chromosome) were predisposed to be violent criminals (Price et al., 1966). This genetic abnormality became known as 'the supermale syndrome'; individuals who had it were above average height and below average intelligence. The XYY theory offered the prospect of a genetic explanation, which carried with it the weight of scientific knowledge and potentially offered possibilities of screening people before they had committed a crime, cutting the risk to the population at a stroke. However, later research failed to find more than low correlational support for the original findings. The over-representation of XYY individuals in prison populations may have had more to do with their lower intelligence, making them more likely to get caught and to have suffered poor educational performance, which in itself is a risk factor for becoming criminal. Despite this early setback, researchers have continued to search for genetic explanations for criminality, looking at how crime often runs in families and conducting twin and adoption studies for evidence. Christiansen (1977) looked at 3586 twin pairs in Denmark. (correlation) A 52% concordance rate for criminality was found for monozygotic (identical) twins, compared to just 22% for dizygotic (non-identical) twins. However, we must remember the effects of shared upbringing and if crime really was genetic we would expect a 100% concordance rate for monozygotic twins as they share 100% of their genes. Further research suggests that Monoamine oxidase (MAO) is an enzyme in the mytochondria (the powerhouses found in large numbers in every cell that provide the cell 30 with energy). MAO is the enzyme responsible for breaking down several neurotransmitters (brain chemicals which relay, amplify, and modulate signals between a neuron and another cell) including monamines: serotonin, dopamine and norepinephrine. Therefore it also has an effect on the levels of these neurotransmitters in the body. There are actually two MAO enzymes, MAOA and MAOB, and they are produced by two different genes that have loci (or “addresses”) close to each other on the short end of the X chromosome. This means that these genes are sex-linked. It is well known medically that inhibiting the production of MAO, particularly MAOA, using drugs is a very effective way of treating depression. So, by inference, it would seem that altering MAO enzymic activity can affect the brain by altering its chemistry. e.g. Brunner et al (1993) – A study of violence in a family with genetic abnormality Aim: - A case study was carried out on a family from the Netherlands where males were affected by a syndrome of borderline mental retardation and abnormal violent behaviour. These included impulsive aggression, arson, attempted rape and exhibitionism. Procedure: - 5 affected males were studied. Data was collected from the analysis of urine samples over a 24 hour period. The urine samples measured the metabolites of several neurotransmitters, including serotonin (Metabolites are formed as part of the natural biochemical process of metabolism of neurotransmitters and the levels of metabolites excreted in urine reveal whether metabolism is normal or not) Findings: - The tests showed disturbed monoamine metabolism associated with a deficit of the enzyme monoamine oxidase A (MAOA). A mutation was identified in the X chromosome of the gene responsible for the production of MAOA. Conclusion: - MAOA is involved in serotonin metabolism. Impaired metabolism of serotonin is likely to be responsible for mental retardation and this could be linked to aggressive behaviour e.g. high levels of serotonin (caused by failure to metabolise the serotonin as normal) might cause impulsive bouts of aggression. The criminal violence in the effected members of this unusual family appeared to be directly connected to their faulty gene. 31 32 Subtopic: Gender Daly & Wilson (2001) - Investigation of gender-related life expectancy Background: In all cultures young males appear more often in crime statistics than any other groups. Although, females do commit crime, the figures are far lower. Why? Daly & Wilson (2001) noticed that young male offenders had a ‘short term horizon’. This means that these individuals want instant gratification. They have a short lifespan expectation due to the risky behaviour that they engage in. Evolutionary Psychology theorists believe that every gene that influences our behaviour is subject to evolution by natural selection. Specific genes will be spread through a population if the individuals bearing that gene reproduce more than the individual bearing other genes. Using the example of status competition, in most species male success is based upon competition for a mate, through display, fighting or competing for status or territory with other males. Evolutionary Psychologists argue that in human society males have these same pressures and will participate in such behaviours even if they involve danger or even the possibility of death. 33 34 Summary: Biology The biological influence on criminality covers many separate strands as researchers work in small areas of the different fields. One criticism of this is their tendency to promote their own particular body of evidence as though it existed separately from others. It is unlikely ever to be possible to find a single biological cause of criminal behaviour, because crime exists in many forms. The attraction of a biological explanation is that it offers the possibility of screening for the future. Against this is the danger of labelling individuals before a crime has been committed and ignoring the possibility of free will being able to override any predisposition to criminal behaviour. 35 TURNING TO CRIME January 2010 a) Outline a biological explanation of why males commit more crimes than females. [10] b) To what extent does the biological approach provide an explanation of criminal behaviour? [15] June 2010 a) Outline one piece of research into criminal thinking patterns. (10) b) To what extent does the cognitive approach explain criminal behaviour? (15) January 2011 a) Outline how brain dysfunction can explain criminal behaviour. (10) b) Evaluate individual biological explanations for criminal behaviour. (15) a) How can an upbringing in a disrupted family cause criminal behaviour? (10) b) Evaluate the longitudinal research when considering upbringing as an explanation of crime. (15) January 2012 a) Outline evidence which shows genes may influence criminal behaviour. (10) b) To what extent are biological explanations to why people turn to crime reductionist? (15) June 2012 1. Describe how social cognition can explain criminal behaviour. (10) 2. Evaluate the validity of research into cognitive explanations of criminal behaviour. (15) January 2013 1. How can criminal behaviour be learnt from others? (10) 2. Discuss the view that some people turn to crime because of their upbringing. (15) June 2013 1. What does research into moral development tell us about criminal behaviour? (10) 2. Discuss whether individuals have fee will when turning to crime. (15) June 2014 3. How can upbringing in poverty and disadvantaged neighbourhoods explain criminal behaviour. (10) 2. Evaluate the methodology used to investigate upbringing as an explanation of crime. (15) 36 Topic 2 – Making a case 1.Interviewing witnesses Recognising and recreating faces Factors influencing accurate identification The cognitive interview 2. Interviewing suspects Detecting lies Interrogation techniques False confessions 3. Creating a profile Top down typology Bottom up approaches John Duffy Case study 37 Theories of Interviewing witnesses The Theories/Studies 1. Bruce et al (1988) - The importance of external and internal features in facial recognition 2. Loftus et al (1987) - Weapon Focus 3. Fisher and Geiselman (1992) - The Cognitive Interview The main approach in this area is Cognitive psychology. How we process information and are able to recognise and remember it accurately is important for witnesses of crime. This area highlights factors that may influence the accuracy of such information. Subsection = Recognising and recreating faces by E-fit Recognising Faces: The Thatcher illusion Task: - Look at these two pictures of Margaret Thatcher and then turn the page upside down! This is known as the Thatcher illusion. This illustrates some of the problems with facial recognition. Being able to accurately identify a face is key to many convictions. However, cases of mistaken identity show that this is not always as easy as it seems. Vikki Bruce has shown that recognising a familiar face is something which we are all quite good at, but unfamiliar faces are a completely different matter. Sinha et al (2006) found that there are eight important factors involved when witnesses try to reconstruct a stranger’s face. 1. We can recognise familiar faces even when the resolution is low (poor quality CCTV) 2. The more familiar they are the easier it is. 38 3. Facial features are processed holistically (all together as a single unit). This has implications for piece by piece facial reconstruction, such as the old “Identikit” method. 4. Eyebrows and hairline are most important features. 5. Illumination changes influence recognition (the suspect may look very different in poor light or side light). (see the Einstein/Marilyn example) 6. Motion of the face helps recognition (We can better recognise a video than a photo). 7. There appears to be specialised neurones for face recognition. 8. Face identity and expression may be processed by two different systems in the brain. Key concept: E-Fit The Electronic Facial Identification Technique, commonly known as E-FIT, is a computerised method of synthesizing images to produce facial composites of the faces of suspects based on eyewitness descriptions. e.g. Bruce et al (1988) - The importance of external and internal features in facial recognition Background: Cognitive psychologists have done a lost of research into how we recognize faces. Loftus has shown again and again how people’s memories are reconstructive – we “invent” our memories from SCHEMAS (stereotyped ideas) and we are very influenced by leading questions. Even the difference between asking a witness to start by remembering the eyes or the ears could have a huge effect on the face they come up with. Psychological research on face recognition suggests that we recognise familiar faces by their internal features (eyes, brows, nose and mouth), but recall unfamiliar faces according their external features (head shape, hair and ears) Where witnesses are constructing photofit images or informing police artists in sketches they will be describing unfamiliar faces, so will be focusing on, and more accurate at describing, the external features. One of the great breakthroughs in Forensic Psychology has been the FACIAL COMPOSITE technique. This involves helping witnesses put together the image of a face from the different components a face is made from – choosing the right nose from a selection of noses, the right eyebrows from a selection of eyebrows, etc. The first successful facial composite was IDENTIKIT, developed in the 1940s by the Los Angeles police. This was developed in England into PHOTOFIT, which used more realistic photographs rather than drawings. The latest 39 computer packages, like E-FIT, help witnesses pick facial features from huge databases of faces. The purpose of these photofits is publication and identification by someone who knows the subject and is therefore familiar with their face, but the way witnesses produce photofits engages unfamiliar face perception. Bruce et al aimed to provide evidence for the theory that witnesses produce poor internal features in their photofits. The fact that witnesses are likely to produce better external features in their photofits and are not very good at reproducing the internal features of unfamiliar faces is a reason why police photofits are poor quality and named only in around 20% of cases. They fail to give the viewer accuracy in the facial areas (internal features) that we generally use to identify faces familiar to us. Bruce et al (1988) - The importance of external and internal features in facial recognition Aim: To investigate the relative recognisability of internal (eyes, brows, nose and mouth) and external (head shape, hair and ears) features in facial recognition. Participants, details & results: Experiment 1: 30 staff and students from Stirling University were paid £2 to sort the composites. 15 males and 15 females with a mean age of 29.2 This was an independent measures design with three conditions. All participants were given pictures of 10 celebrities, the task was to match the correct composite image to the celebrity from the 40 composites given. The target celebrities were actors Ben Affleck, Matt Damon, Jeremy Edwards, Joshua Jackson, Philip Olivier and James Redmond and pop singers Kian Egan, Mark Feehily, Ronan Keating and Ian 'H' Watkins. The composite images were produced by E-Fit, PRO-fit, Skethc and EvoFit (all commonly used by the police). Group 1 were given complete composites, group 2 composites containing only internal features (eyes, nose, mouth) and group 3 external feature (head shape, hair and ears) composites. Each face was clean shaven and spectacles were avoided. 40 The 40 composites used had been made as a result of interviews with participant-witnesses (in a previous study) who were unfamiliar with the target face (the celebrities were chosen as ones generally unknown to people aged over 30, who were the participant-witnesses in the previous study) and researchers waited a couple of days prior to construction of composites from memory in order to increase validity. Results: 42% of whole composites and those of external features were sorted correctly, compared to only 19.5% of internal features. Experiment 2: 48 Undergraduates at Stirling University. 21 males and 27 females, all volunteers. This experiment used a photo array (photo line-up); the task was to identify the celebrity composites from the array. The task was made either easy (very different to target face) or hard (very similar to target face). The composites were presented one at a time along the photo array. The participant had to pick out the celebrity face from the array which matched the composite. Once again the composites were composed of either external or internal features. This gave the following test conditions: External features only, easy task (composite was very similar to the target photograph) Internal features only, easy task (composite was very similar to the target photograph) External features only, hard task (composite was very different from the target photograph) Internal features only, hard task (composite was very different from the target photograph) The materials consisted of the 40 external composite features and the 40 internal composite features of Experiment 1, plus two sets of photo line-ups (complete faces). Ppts were told that they would be evaluating a set of composites of famous faces by picking out the targets from a photo line-up. Participants were then presented with each composite sequentially in a booklet, along with the associated line up, and selected a celebrity face in 41 their own time. Results: External features were identified 42% of the time, compared to just 24% of internal features. Conclusions: Participants performed at just above chance level for internal features. Participants performed equally well with both whole composites and external features. This shows that external features are more important for facial recognition and that faces are processed holistically. This has implications for facial reconstructions which involve witnesses picking internal features from a book. Newer face reconstruction software takes these finding into account. E-fit or EvoFIT both change faces holistically. Facial Recognition research faces the same problems as any other type of memory recall. Delay Exposure mode-intense emotion associated to the face Stimulus variable- How much meaning to the individual has the face to be remembered? Subject variable- How much individual variation is there in the ability to remember a face? New developments in facial morphing (many images used to come up with one composite) should significantly improve effectiveness. Subsection = Factors Influencing Accurate Identification Loftus et al (1987) - Weapon Focus Weapon focus refers to the concentration of a witness’s attention on a weapon which results in them having difficulty recalling other details of the scene and identifying the perpetrator of the crime. Previous research carried out has shown that people fixate their gaze for longer, faster and more often on unusual or highly informative objects. Aim: To provide support for the Weapons focus effect when witnessing a crime. 42 Method: Laboratory experiment Participants & Details: The participants were 36 students from the University of Washington aged 18-31, half were recruited via advertisements and paid $3.50, the others were psychology students participating for extra credits. All participants were shown 18 slides of a series of events in a Taco Time restaurant. For both groups the slide was the same except for one slide. This slide was the independent variable. In the control group the second person in the queue hands the cashier a cheque, in the experimental group the same person pulls a gun on the cashier. The dependent variable was recognition of that person; it was measured by a twenty item multiple choice questionnaire. Participants were also show 12 photos in random sequence and asked to rate how confident they were of their identification on a scale of 1-6 (1 = guess, 6 = very sure). Results: Answers to the questionnaire about the slide show showed no significant difference between the two groups. In the control condition (cheque) 38.9% made a correct identification, in the experimental condition (gun) it was only 11.1%. Eye fixation data showed an average fixation time of 3.72 seconds on the gun, compared to 2.44 seconds on the cheque. Conclusions: The participants spent longer looking at the weapon and therefore had more difficulty in picking the suspect from the line-up. It was assumed that it was the high risk to the witness that drew their focus to the weapon, but later research has shown that it is not quite that simple. The example we will look at is by Dr Kerri Pickel. e.g. Pickel, K.L. (1998). Unusualness And Threat As Possible Causes Of "Weapon Focus.” Background : The weapon focus effect is one in which the presence of a weapon decreases a witnesses' ability to remember details about the event. But, why does this effect occur? One explanation is the cue-utilization hypothesis (Easterbrook, 1959). This theory basically says that the weapon indicates a threat, which increases arousal, which makes the witness focus on the weapon. But could it just be the unusualness of the weapon is that causes “weapon focus” – would any unusual object and not just a high threat object have an effect on witness focus? 43 Experiment 1 : Aim : To investigate both threat and unusualness as reasons for witness focus Method: Participants watched a two-minute video consisting of a scene from a hair salon. A man walks to the receptionist and she hands him money. Conditions depended on what the man held in his hand: High threat, high unusualness Handgun Low threat, high unusualness Raw chicken Low threat, low unusualness Wallet Nothing Control condition Then, ppts did a 10-minute filler activity after which they completed a questionnaire. The first part of the qre focused on the receptionist who was seen as a control since she was viewed before the man walked in. The second section covered the man (target). Witnesses were, for example, asked to describe the target’s features and clothing, identify the object held by him (if any). They were also asked what he was actually doing and were then asked to identify him from a photo line-up. The results showed that the ppts focused for longest on the raw chicken (high unusualness, low threat). The accuracy of witnesses’ descriptions was affected by the unusualness of the object, but not threat. Witnesses had difficulty remembering the low-threat, low unusualness object (wallet): many failed to identify it. “Weapon focus” was only evident in ppts’ descriptions: identification accuracy (identifying the perpetrator form the line ups) did not differ by condition. 44 Subsection: Cognitive interview What is a Cognitive Interview? The Cognitive interview technique (CI) is based largely on the work of Loftus and other psychologists, following their theoretical work into memory (remember her AS Core Study?). It is designed to take account of well known cognitive functions and avoid any chance of leading the witness. The CI involves the interviewer giving a set of instructions to the witness to reinstate the context of the original event and to search through memory by using a variety of retrieval methods. There are 4 basic principles: 1. Interview similarity – memory of an event is enhanced when the psychological environment at the interview is similar to the environment at the original event. The interviewer should therefore try to reinstate in the witness’s mind the external (weather), emotional (fear) and cognitive (relevant thoughts) features that were experienced at the time of the crime. 2. Focused retrieval – The interviewer is to generate focused concentration. There should be no interruption to the chain of thought and plenty of encouragement to try hard. 3. Extensive retrieval – Witnesses should be encouraged to make as many retrieval attempts as possible. Even if they say they cannot remember, they should be encouraged to try another angle, eg. imagine the scene from another viewpoint. 4. Witness-compatible questioning- Events are stored and organised uniquely for each witness. Successful retrieval therefore reflects how compatible the questioning is with the witness’s unique mental representation. Interviewers should be flexible and alter their approach to meet the needs of each witness. 45 Fisher and Geiselman (1992) - The Cognitive Interview Aim: To test the cognitive interview (CI) in the field Method: Field experiment Participants & Details: 16 detectives from the Robbery Division of Dade County, Florida. All were experienced In the first phase of the experiment, detectives were asked to record a selection of their next interviews using the standard techniques they normally used. This took 4 months and 88 interviews were recorded, mostly related to bag snatches or robberies. The detectives were then divided into two groups and one group was trained in CI techniques. The group undergoing CI training comprised of 7 detectives. Training was over four 60-minute sessions. Interviews by both groups were then recorded over the next seven months. The interviews were then analysed by a team at the University of California, who were blind to the conditions (that is, they did not know if the interview they were analysing was by a trained CI detective or not). The amount of information gathered by the two groups of detectives was collated. Results: After training, CI detectives elicited 47% more information than before, and 63% more information than the untrained detectives. The time taken to interview witnesses was not significantly different, but CI’s do take longer. Both groups had a statement accuracy rate of 85%. Conclusions: This field experiment confirmed findings from laboratory based studies that Cognitive Interview Techniques do seem to work in as much as they allow the interviewer to gather more information. The authors conclude that because the Cognitive Interview reliably enhances memory and is easily learned and administered, it should be useful for a variety of investigative interviews. This has an obvious application in helping the police elicit information from witnesses to hopefully help them to solve more crimes. The CI technique is used by police forces in the UK, although often using a reined version (Fisher and others are still researching and refining the CI technique) 46 Summary: Interviewing witnesses The work of Bruce and others shows that the old ways of constructing photofits relied on a false idea of how we recognise faces. Her research is leading to seeing face reconstruction as a holistic process with much more satisfactory results. It has been known for some time that if a weapon is involved in a crime then a witness is likely to be less accurate at identifying a suspect, as the shock of seeing a weapon will dominate over recall of the suspect’s facial features. When interviewing a witness it is important not to lead them in any way, yet prompts can help recall immensely. A cognitive interview approach allows the witness to improve recall by reconstructing the event in their mind, inducing smells, feelings and taking different perspectives on the event. 47 Theories of Interviewing suspects The Theories/Studies 1. Mann et al (2004) - Police officers’ ability to detect suspects’ lies 2. Inbau et al (1986) - The Reid “nine steps” of interrogation 3. Gudjohnsson et al (1990) - A case of false confession The main approaches within this area are Individual Differences, Cognitive and Social psychology. This area focuses upon how suspects when being interviewed/interrogated will react when in specific conditions, such as being coerced. The differences between individual’s experiences, thought patterns and likelihood to obey/conform will all influence their likelihood to confess guilt. Subsection: Detecting Lies Mann et al (2004) - Police officers ability to detect suspects’ lies Background: Some forensic psychologists prefer biological measures to detect lying. A polygraph is a device that measures changes in skin conductivity and heart rate when the suspect becomes tense (ie. is lying). It’s possible to fool polygraphs by making yourself tense up by digging nails into your own skin, etc. Also, some groups oppose polygraphs, since they claim they lead to false convictions. Drugs like Sodium Pentathol are advertised as “truth serums” but only make the suspect relaxed and careless – they won’t make you say anything against your nature and don’t force people to tell the truth. Behavioural psychology has a lot to contribute to the subject of spotting liars. Common signs (or CUES) of lying include: Avoiding eye contact Touching face, throat or mouth Timing of emotional gestures (like smiles) – too late, too slow or separate from speech Placing objects as a “barrier” in front of the questioner Repeating the wording of the question in the answer Discomfort with silence leads to adding unnecessary details to an answer Police officers tend to think they are good at spotting lies when they are interviewing suspects. They often claim that suspects reveal they are lying by minor body language or by being inconsistent under repeated questioning. It’s very hard to test this because it’s not permitted for the public to look at recordings of police interviews with suspects. This is because a suspect is innocent until proven guilty and even suspects have a right to privacy until their case comes to court. Aim: To test police officers’ ability to distinguish truth and lies during interviews with suspects. Method: Field experiment 48 Participants & details: 99 Kent police officers (24 female, 75 males), mean age 34.3 years old. 78 detectives, 8 trainers, 4 traffic officers and 9 uniformed response officers. The participant’s task was to judge the truthfulness of people in real-life police interviews. At the beginning of the experiment, participants were asked to fill out a questionnaire about their experiences of detecting liars. They then saw video clips of 14 suspects showing their head and torso so that expression and movement were visible (the clips were backed up by other evidence which established whether the suspect was lying or telling the truth at any point). The 54 clips varied in length from 6 to 145 seconds. After watching each clip, the participant had to indicate whether it was a lie or the truth, and how confident they were about their decision. They were also asked to make a note of the cues they used to detect the liars. Results: Police Officers performed better than chance (50%) they were 66.2% accurate on lies and 63.6% accurate on truths. The most frequently mentioned cues were gaze, movements, vagueness, contradictions in stories and fidgeting. Conclusions: The police officers were good at detecting lies. However, to establish whether they were any better than the general public would require a control group. This study could not have had a control group because of issues of confidentiality and privacy (the clips were real police interviews). Subsection = Interrogation Techniques An interrogation is different to an interview because it is a guilt-presumptive technique and is therefore accusatory: the investigator starts by telling the suspect that there is no doubt as to their guilt. Interrogation usually follows an initial interview. The interrogation is designed to increase arousal and anxiety and the aim of the interrogation is to secure a confession from the suspect. However, a problem with techniques used to increase arousal and anxiety is that they have a tendency to lead to false confessions and, in some cases miscarriages of justice where the wrong person has been found guilty and imprisoned. This is especially true where vulnerable suspects, such as young people or the mentally impaired, are concerned. 49 Following a number of high profile examples of false confessions in the UK, the Police and Criminal Evidence Act (PACE) (1984), under PACE Code E, requires all interviews to be recorded in triplicate: a working copy for the police, one for the solicitor and one sealed at the end of the interview and not to be opened until the case is in court, to ensure evidence cannot be tampered with. For full details see: http://police.homeoffice.gov.uk/publications/operationalpolicing/2008_PACE_Code_E_(final).pdf According to the Home Office website: “PACE sets out to strike the right balance between the powers of the police and the rights and freedoms of the public”. In the British legal system a defendant is presumed innocent until proven guilty, so accusatory and anxiety-provoking interrogation techniques that, solicitors and suspects may argue, lead suspects to confess under duress, are not acceptable police practice in the UK. So the work of Inbau, see below, should not be applied in the interrogation of suspects under the PACE codes. Nonetheless, false confessions still happen (see next part of this section), despite the safeguards put in place to try to prevent this. “You have the right to remain silent…” – Why? Part of the PACE code of conduct includes informing a suspect of their rights, including the right to remain silent and the right to a solicitor. Suspects rights are given when they are arrested: “You do not have to say anything, but it may harm your defence if you do not mention when questioned something which you later rely on in Court. Anything you do say may be given in evidence”. The USA equivalent of this is the Miranda Rights, and the right to remain silent is considered to be a constitutional right, known as “taking the fifth amendment”. The Miranda rights inform suspects of their right to remain silent, and “taking the fifth amendment” refers to an amendment to the American Bill Of Rights which means that you have the right not to testify if that testimony might implicate you or lead to your conviction for a criminal offence. 50 The introduction of the Miranda Rights was not welcomed by Fred Inbau. Inbau, along with John Reid, is the author of the book “Criminal Investigations and Confessions”, a handbook still used widely in police training in the USA today. Inbau saw the Miranda Rights as a trend towards putting the rights of the suspect before the rights of society and he campaigned strongly against this trend. His book, which has been regularly updated, includes a number of techniques for interviewing and interrogating suspects, collectively known as The Reid Technique (or more correctly the Reid Techniques). This includes what is known as the Reid Nine Steps of Interrogation. Inbau et al (1986) - The Reid “nine steps” of interrogation Inbau developed an approach to interrogation which relied on presenting a mass of damaging facts to persuade criminals that they had no choice but to confess. He felt it was justifiable for the police to lie, deceive, or use tricks to force a confession. His techniques are described in a book written with John Reid, Criminal Interrogation and Confessions (1962), which is the mostly used interrogation manual in the USA. The “nine steps” of interrogation are: 1. Direct confrontation – the suspect is told directly that they are thought to have committed the offence. 2. Chance to shift blame (either to some else or circumstances) - the suspect is offered the chance to shift the blame away from themself by being offered some suggestions or justifications for what happened. The interrogator should show sympathy and understanding for their plight. This is designed to make it easier to admit guilt. 3. Never allow the suspect to deny guilt - the suspect should never be allowed to deny guilt. Interrupt any denial to prevent the suspect getting the psychological advantage. 4. Ignore excuses – at this point the suspect will often try to give reasons why they could not have committed the crime. Try to use this to move towards a confession by ignoring them. Eventually the suspect will give up trying. 5. Reinforce sincerity, eye contact, first names – to ensure that the suspect is receptive by staying close, keeping good eye contact and using first names. 6. If suspect cries, infer guilt – the suspect will eventually become quieter and listen; at this point move towards offering alternatives. If the suspect cries, infer guilt. 7. Pose the ‘alternative question’ – giving the suspect two choices of what they could do, one more socially acceptable than the other (it gives a reason why they could have done it) but whichever they choose they are admitting their guilt. 51 8. Admit guilt in front of witness – Get the suspect to admit guilt in front of witnesses. 9. Document suspect’s admission – Document the suspect’s admission and get them to sign a confession to avoid them retracting it later. Inbau justifies the use of psychological techniques in this way because he says they are being used on people presupposed to be guilty through their preliminary interview. 52 53 Subsection: False Confessions What is a false confession? A false confession is an admission of guilt in a crime in which the confessor is not responsible for the crime. False confessions can be induced through coercion or by the mental incompetency of the accused. According to Kassin & Wrightsman (1985), there are three types of false confessions: 1. The ‘voluntary confession’ – no obvious external pressure 2. The ’coerced compliant confession’ – after forceful or persistent questioning the suspect confesses to escape the interview situation. 3. The ‘coerced internalised confession’ where the suspect becomes temporarily persuaded that they are guilty. This persuasion is possible because are memories are fragile and susceptible, especially when we are in an anxious and vulnerable state. Some groups are more susceptible to this pressure than others, such as the young, the mentally ill, those with a low IQ, etc. Psychometric tests can help to determine how susceptible people are to persuasion techniques. Examples of those used within this study are: IQ test - a psychometric test of intelligence Gudjohnsson Suggestibility Scale - a test that tries to measure how susceptible a person is to coercive interrogation. It relies on two different aspects of interrogative suggestibility: yielding to leading questions; and shifting responses when interrogative pressure is applied. The test consists of a narrative paragraph read to the participant, who then reports all they can remember about the story. Later, the participant is asked a number of questions about the story, some of which are incorrect. Then, the person being tested is told in a forceful manner that they have made a number of errors and must answer the questions for a second time. Yielding refers to susceptibility to suggestive questioning. Shifting refers to changing answers as a result of interrogative pressure. Eysenck’s Personality Inventory (EPI) - personality test designed to measure the traits of extroversion and neuroticism 54 Gudjonsson and MacKeith (1990) – A Case of False Confession Aim: To document a case of false confession of a youth who was at the time distressed and susceptible to interrogative pressure. Method: Case study Participant, details & results: A 17 year old youth accused of two murders (FC). He was of average intelligence, suffered from no mental illness and his personality was not obviously abnormal. In 1987 two elderly women were found battered to death in their home, their saving’s were missing and they had been sexually assaulted. A few days later FC was arrested because of inconsistencies in accounts of his movements during an earlier routine enquiry and because he was spending more money than usual. There was no forensic evidence to link him to the case. After his arrest he was denied access to a solicitor and was interviewed for 14 hours by the police, leading to his confession. During this interview, he was repeatedly accused of lying, the questioning was leading and accusatory and the police even suggested he was sexually impotent! FC found this very distressing and eventually confessed. In a second interview the day after, FC retracted his confession; only to confess again under pressure about his failure to have successful relationships with women. There were three further interviews. In prison he was assessed by psychiatrists who found no evidence of mental illness, an IQ of 94 and a score of 10 (very high) on the Gudjohnsson Suggestibility Scale, making him abnormal in this respect. Using Eysenck’s Personality Inventory (EPI) he came out as a stable extrovert. After a year in jail he was released by a court after another person pleaded guilty to the crimes. Conclusions: This is a clear case of ‘coerced compliant false confession’. He gave in to the pressure of persistent and forceful questioning to escape from the interview situation. 55 Summary: Interviewing suspects The police think they are good lie detectors and often rely on non-verbal cues to try and tell if a suspect if lying. Research suggests that this is not the case and that inconsistencies in stories are a better indicator. The police are better at this, but not as good as they think they are. Interrogation is sometimes used to force a confession, but in the UK this is only done under special circumstances. Inbau advocates using psychological techniques to break down suspects’ resistance and his handbook is used by police in the United States. The problem with strong interrogation techniques is that they can lead to false confessions. This can happen with anyone, not just those with lower self-confidence. To protect witnesses, the PACE guidelines must be followed through the legal process. 56 Theories of Creating a profile The Theories/Studies 1. Hazlewood and Douglas (1980): Lust Murders (Top Down) 2. Canter et al (2004) - Investigation of the organised/disorganised theory of the serial murder 3. Canter & Heritage (1990) - Developments in offender profiling 4. Canter - The case of John Duffy The main approaches within this area are Individual Differences, Cognitive and Social psychology. This area attempts to categorise offenders based upon their unusual criminal activity patterns, thought patterns and the environment in which they commit their offences. Profiling is one of the many techniques used to solve crimes, particularly when dealing with unusual or serial crimes. This section will look at theories that are in favour of the “topdown” typology and bottom-up approach to profiling. "Top-down" approaches exist in lots of subjects where things have to be analysed and described. A top-down approach tries to fit everything in to a set of pre-designed categories. A good example might be astrology. Everyone knows about the 12 signs of the Zodiac - Ares, Pisces, Leo, etc. This is a top-down approach to predicting someone's future because if you identify some very basic information about someone (their birthday), you can fit them into an astrological type (eg. Capricorn) and make all sorts of predictions about them (ie. cautious, good with money, ambitious). Top-down approaches exist in Offender Profiling. Profilers have created TYPOLOGIES of criminals. A typology is a set of clear and distinct categories or types, rather like the signs of the Zodiac. When investigators arrive at a crime scene, they are looking for clues that reveal what type of criminal was at work there. Sometimes the clues might point to more than one type, so the investigators need to go for "best fit", choosing the type that best fits the crime scene rather than looking for a perfect fit. Once the criminal type has been identified, investigators can make predictions about whether the criminal will strike again, and where, and who the victim might be - rather like an astrologer can tell you your lucky Lottery number, to look for love on Tuesday and be prepared for a surprise at work on the 19th. On the other hand, there are "bottom-up" approaches to all sorts of subjects. A bottom-up approach makes no assumptions about the person or object you're dealing with. Instead, a researcher will gather together all the information then build a logical description based on that. If astrology is a "top-down" approach to fortune-telling, then a "bottom-up" approach might be reading Tarot Cards: the fortune-teller gets you to draw a number of cards then 57 studies all their different meanings and puts them together to make a prediction about you, who you're going to marry, how many children you're going to have, etc. The "bottom-up" approach to Offender Profiling is popular in Britain and is championed by David Canter, who runs the Centre for Investigative Psychology at Liverpool University. Canter started out as an environmental psychologist, looking at how people's behaviour in buildings stayed the same, even in emergencies like a fire. From this he developed his CRIMINAL CONSISTENCY HYPOTHESIS, which states that an offender's behaviour while committing a crime will be consistent with his behaviour in his everyday life. Canter believes that “Criminals are performing actions that are direct reflections of the sorts of transactions they have with other people.” Canter avoids typologies and categorising offenders, arguing it is important to treat each offender as unique and individual. The type of approach used within profiling is dependent upon the situation and type of crime to be solved, however it should be noted that both of these approaches to profiling are not without flaws. 58 Subsection: Top Down Typology An example of Top-down typology… In 1980 Hazelwood and Douglas published their account of the ‘lust murderer’ whereby they outlined their theory that lust murderers can be categorised as organised or disorganised. An organised offender leads an ordered life and kills after some sort of critical life event. Their actions are premeditated and planned, they are likely to bring weapons and restraints to the scene. They are likely to be of average to high intelligence and employed. A disorganised offender is more likely to have committed the crime in a moment of passion. There will be no evidence of premeditation and they are more likely to leave evidence such as blood, semen, murder weapon etc. behind. This type of offender is thought to be less socially competent and more likely to be unemployed. Activity Identify the type of offender for each of the scenarios below: Scenario 1 Scenario 2 Police have arrived at a house where a woman has been raped and killed. The killer gained entry to the house by posing as a prospective buyer for the property, which was for sale. The killer raped and carried out mutilation on parts of the woman’s body whilst she was still alive. The attack lasted 4 hours after which the woman bled to death due to her injuries. There were no weapons left at the scene or forensic evidence (eg,, sperm). Police have arrived at a house where a woman has been strangled. The woman’s ex-boyfriend was seen by neighbours hanging around the property a week before her murder. The suspect broke a window to gain entry and cut themselves whilst climbing through. The cord used to strangle the woman was left at the scene as the murderer fled upon being disturbed by her flatmate. Type of offender: _______________________ Type of offender: __________________ 59 Background: America, and in particular the FBI, are historically associated with using the Top-down approach. In 1980 Hazelwood and Douglas published their account of the ‘lust murderer’ whereby they outlined their theory that lust murderers can be categorised as organised or disorganised. An organised offender leads an ordered life and kills after some sort of critical life event. Their actions are premeditated and planned, they are likely to bring weapons and restraints to the scene. They are likely to be of average to high intelligence and employed; whereas a disorganised offender is more likely to have committed the crime in a moment of passion. There will be no evidence of premeditation and they are more likely to leave evidence such as blood, semen, murder weapon etc. behind. This type of offender is thought to be less socially competent and more likely to be unemployed. In 1992, Douglas added another typology called the “mixed” offender – this offender did not fit either of the earlier typologies as this type offender may have an accomplice or the offence may escalate into a different pattern. Criticisms of the “Top-down” approach include the theory is based upon an opportunity sample of 36 murderers in prison who volunteered to take part in unstructured interviews. Also, no other tests were carried out upon this sample so low in reliability. Aim: Canter et al wanted to test the reliability of the top-down typologies by applying them to 100 cases. Details: A content analysis of 100 cases of serial killers from the USA in order to see if the features hypothesised to belong to each typology would be consistently and distinctively different. The cases came from published accounts of serial killers and were crosschecked with court reports and officers where possible. They had been collected over several years by an independent researcher and were called the Missen Corpus. The third crime committed by each serial killer was analysed using The Crime Classification Manual (Douglas et al 1992) in order to determine the crime as being organised or disorganised. Results: Twice as many disorganised as organised crime-scenes were identified, suggesting that disorganised offenders are more common or easier to identify. Two behaviours in the organised typology that occurred at a level significantly above chance (5%) were in 70% of cases where the body was concealed and in 75% of cases where sexual activity occurred. Further analysis failed to reveal any significant differences between organised and disorganised variables. 60 Conclusions: Canter concluded that instead of their being a distinction between two types of serial murder, all of the crimes had to have an organised element to them, as we might expect from the fact the killers were not caught after three killings! The distinctions between serial killers may be a function of the different ways in which they exhibit disorganised aspects of their activities. It would be better to look at personality differences between offenders. 1. Differences between organized and disorganized 2. murder scenes and lust murderers based on Hazelwood et al: 3. 4. Organized murder scene Disorganized scene Spontaneous Victim — known by offender Little control Planned Victim — targeted stranger Control including restraints, Sexual acts before death controlled talk Body not hidden or left at crime scene. Aggression before death Evidence present Body hidden or moved from crime scene Weapon and evidence absent Organized murderer Disorganized murderer More-than-average lQ Less-than-average IQ Skilled occupation Unskilled Controlled mood Uncontrolled Living with partner Living alone Mobile — for example, car Lives near crime Socially competent Socially incompetent Sexually competent Sexually incompetent High birth order status Low birth order status Fathers work stable Unstable Inconsistent discipline as child Harsh discipline as child Use of alcohol during crime Alcohol not used during crime Follows crime on news Does not follow crime on news Limited change in behaviour after crime Major behaviour change after crime 5. Source: FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 1985. 61 Subsection: Bottom Up Approaches Bottom up approaches Canter is the UK’s foremost profiling expert. His bottom-up approach looks for consistencies in offenders’ behaviour during the crime. These can be inferred from the crime scene or from surviving victims’ accounts. No initial assumptions are made about the offender until a statistical analysis using correlational techniques Smallest Space Analysis has been carried out on the details of the cases. The approach relies heavily on computer databases being accurate. It can be the little details that are often overlooked that can be crucial to the success of a case. This approach to profiling can be considered more objective and reliable that top-down typology because they are always based on data analysis and a theoretical basis of human behaviour. Canter’s most famous case is that of the ‘Railway Rapist’ John Duffy, the third study to be addressed within this topic. 62 Canter & Heritage (1990) - Developments in offender profiling Aim: To identify a behaviour pattern from similarities between offences. Details: A content analysis was carried out on 66 sexual offences from various police forces committed by 27 offenders to find 33 offence variables that were clearly linked to a potential behaviour characteristic, eg. variable 2 was “surprise attack”. It was possible to say “yes” or “no” to each variable. A computer was used to work out the correlations between each aspect of the crime and identify the key factors central to a sexual assault. This method is known as a smallest-space analysis, because it presents the results visually, like a map with contours on it, with the most common factors closer to the centre and less typical behaviours appearing out at the edges Results: Five variables were found to be central to the 66 cases: 1. Vaginal Intercourse 2. No reaction to the victim 3. Impersonal Language 4. Surprise Attack 5. Victim’s clothing disturbed This suggests a pattern of behaviour where the attack is impersonal and sudden and the victim’s response is irrelevant to the offender. Conclusions: This has become known as the five factor theory. These five aspects have now been shown to contribute to all sexual offences, but in different patterns for different individuals. An analysis of these factors can enable Police to decide if an offence has been committed by the same individual. 63 Subsection = Case Study Canter - The case of John Duffy “The Railway Rapist” In November 2000, John Duffy, who was serving life for the rape and murder of several women, confessed to his prison psychologist that in fact he was responsible for many more and that he had committed many of these acts with an accomplice, David Mulcahy. He therefore testified in front of the jury at Mulcahy’s trial, giving the evidence to “clear this conscience”. Key terminology – GEOGRAPHICAL PROFILING Geographic profiling is an information management system and investigative methodology that evaluates the locations of connected serial crimes to determine the most probable area of offender residence. It can be applied in cases of serial murder, rape, arson, robbery and bombings. An offender has to know about a particular geographical area before he or she begins selecting crimes to commit; and where the offenders movement patterns intersect within this geographical area, will to a large extent determine where the crime takes place. ‘By establishing the probability of the offender residing in various areas and displaying those results on a map, police efforts to apprehend criminals can be assisted. This information allows police departments to focus their investigative efforts, geographically prioritise suspects, and concentrate patrol efforts in those zones where the criminal predator is likely to be active’. (Kim Rossmo) For more details, go to http://www.all-about-forensic-psychology.com/geographicprofiling.html Canter became involved in the case after reading an account in the London Evening Standard in January 1986. The police were investigating 24 sexual assaults in London over the previous four years. It was believed, because of the modus operandi of the criminals that these assaults had all been carried out by the same man, although on some occasions 2 men had been involved. Canter, who was working in geographical psychology at the time, drew up a map of the crimes to see if there was a pattern to the offences. This is one of the elements of GEOGRAPHICAL PROFILING. 64 Background: In 1982 a woman (KJ) was raped near London's Hampstead station and dozens more were attacked over the next 2 years. The police set up a workshop to find the perpetrators, called Operation Hart. The press called the attacker "the Railway Rapist" but the nickname changed to "Railway Killer" on December 29, 1985, when Alison Day was dragged off a train at Hackney station and repeatedly raped. She was then strangled with a piece of string. In April, 15-year-old Maartje Tamboezer was raped and murdered, then her body was set on fire. A month later, local TV presenter Anne Locke was abducted and murdered after leaving a train in Brookmans Park, Hertfordshire. The police investigation was getting nowhere, because although the surviving women could give detailed accounts of the attacker's Modus Operandi (method of working), they were uncertain about his height and appearance. Possibly they were distracted by the weapon focus effect (Loftus et al, 1987). Canter’s assumptions about the Railway Rapist were based on the following psychological principles - that people are influenced by one another’s actions, so any differences between the attacks involving one man and those involving 2 might give up clues - that people’s behaviour changes over time and that looking at the changes that took place over the 4 year period could also provide clues as to the identity of the offenders. Canter believes strongly that a criminal’s crime life will be closely related to their everyday life, so their criminal life would offer clues to their everyday life and again offer clues to the identity of the criminal(s). With the rapist turning to murder in December 1985 the police, at least those at senior level, were keen to use “scientific procedures, methods, and theories as central to their investigations rather than as an optional extra” (Canter, 1994). Canter was invited to work with the metropolitan police to offer whatever assistance he could in the case. Aim: To show successful application of a “bottom-up” approach of profiling to solving a crime. Details: In 1988 Canter was invited by the Metropolitan Police to draw a profile of the “Railway Rapist”. Canter examined the details of each crime and built up a profile of the attacker's personality, habits and traits. The police and Canter started looking at the patterns in the rapist’s behaviour, e.g. - What exactly was said to the victim before the assault? - Were the victims’ clothes pulled off, torn or cut? - What sorts of threats were made? - What sort of sexual activity took place? - How did the assailant deal with the victim after the assault? The data were fed into a computer (an innovation at the time) looking at the consistencies and changes in the attacks. On the basis of all the information gathered, Canter drew up a 17-item profile of the Railway Rapist. John Duffy matched on 13/17 of these items. 65 Using the "bottom-up" approach, Canter identified two significant themes in the attacks: 1. The attacker seemed prepared to try to relate to the victim - this suggests previous relationships which were abusive 2. The minimum amount of force was used to dominate the victim - this suggests a weak, insecure individual He also studied the location of the crimes going back of 4 years, laying transparent acetate sheets with the crime scenes marked on top of a map of the area surrounding London. This built up a GEOGRAPHIC PROFILE which helped him pin down where the attacker lived because the pattern suggested the killer was a MARAUDER who operated out of a home base. Canter drew up a 17-item profile of the Railway Rapist. John Duffy matched on 13/17 of these items. Other features of the profile included: living in Kilburn or Cricklewood, married, no children, marriage problems, loner, with few friends, physically small, feelings of unattractiveness, interest in martial arts or body building, need to dominate women, fascination with weapons (especially knives), fantasies about sex and violence, semi-skilled job, little contact with the public, aged 2030 years old. Results: One suspect, John Duffy, was 1505th on the police’s list of 2000 suspects with the right blood group to be the attacker. He was on the police database because of a previous incident where he had threatened his ex-wife at knifepoint. However, he fit the profile very closely: living in Kilburn, married, and infertile, separated from his wife, only two male friends, 5’4” with acne, member of a martial arts club, violent (had often attacked his wife), collector of martial arts weapons, collector of hard core porn videos, trained as a British Rail carpenter, age 28 at the time. Because of Canter’s profile, Duffy was reinvestigated and convicted for two murders and five rapes. Canter had predicted that the attacker would have a SIGNATURE - most likely he took "souvenirs" from his victims. Duffy had a collection of 33 house keys taken from the women he had raped and murdered. Police suspected Duffy’s friend David Mulcahy to be his collaborator, but could not prove this. Mulcahy, a married father-of-4, was eventually convicted in 2001, but based on DNA profiling and Duffy’s own testimony in prison. Conclusions: This case study lends support to Canter’s “bottom-up approach” approach to profiling. 66 The 13/17 items of Canter’s profile of the Railway Rapist on which Duffy was matched: Profile of Likely Offender Match Live in the Kilburn or Cricklewood area Duffy lived in Kilburn of London, the area where the first three offences occurred Be married, with no children Duffy was married, and a low sperm count meant he was infertile Have marriage problems Duffy and his wife were separated Be a loner, with few friends Duffy had only 2 male friends, one of whom was David Mulcahy, his co-offender Duffy was 5’4”, with acne Be physically small, with feelings of unattractiveness Have an interest in martial arts or body-building Need to dominate women Duffy was a violent man who had already raped his wife Have fantasies about rape and bondage Duffy liked tying his wife up before sex Have a fascination for weapons, especially swords and knives Have fantasies about sex and violence Duffy had many “Kung Fu” style weapons in his home Keep souvenirs from crimes Duffy had 33 door keys, each taken from a victim as a souvenir Duffy trained a carpenter with British Rail Hold a semi skilled job with little contact with the public Be in the age range 20-30 Duffy spent much of his time at a marital arts club Duffy collected hard-core porn videos Duffy was 28 when arrested and had been a rapist for 4 years 67 Summary: Creating a profile Profiling has become more accepted in recent years as it has become more methodical and statistically based. It is still just one of many techniques used to solve crimes and is really only useful for particularly unusual or serial crimes. Top-down typologies look for common patterns in murder scenes and categorise them as organised or disorganised. From this decision, a series of behaviour patterns is assumed which can be helpful in reducing lists of potential suspects. Bottom-up approaches examine statistical correlations from clues found at the scene with other, similar crimes. Other correlations are looked for in geographical location and consistencies in behaviour patterns. The case study of John Duffy illustrates the style of profiling adopted by David Canter. This was his most successful attempt and gave hope that these techniques would transform the search for suspects in murder cases. 68 MAKING A CASE January 2010 1 (a) Describe the cognitive interview. (10) (b) Discuss qualitative and quantitative approaches to collecting information when interviewing witnesses. (15) 2 (a) Describe the bottom up approach to creating a profile. (10) (b) Assess the reliability of offender profiling. (15) June 2010 1 (a) Describe one piece of research into how lies can be detected when suspects are interviewed. (10) (b) Discuss the reliability of information gained from suspects in an interview. (15) January 2011 1 (a) Describe one case study of offender profiling. (10) (b) Compare different approaches to creating a profile. (15) January 2012 1 (a) Outline techniques for interrogation of crime suspects. (10) (b) How useful is research into interviewing crime suspects? (15) June 2012 1. (a) Describe how top down typology is used to create a profile. (10) (b)Assess the usefulness of qualitative and quantitative data when creating a profile. (15) January 2013 1 (a) Describe how researchers have used E-Fit to investigate face recognition. (10) (b)Assess the strengths and limitations of research into interviewing witnesses to a crime. (15). June 2014 1. Describe the use of the cognitive interview technique when interviewing witnesses. (10) 2. Discuss the ecological validity of research into interviewing witnesses. (15) Topic 3 – Reaching a verdict This topic will consider how psychology can inform behaviour in the courtroom. The courtroom is very important in the British Criminal Justice system. Minor cases are dealt with by the local Magistrates Court, all other cases are referred by the Magistrates to the Crown Court. There is usually a Magistrates Court in each major town, eg. Keighley, and a Crown Court in major cities, eg. Bradford and Leeds (although both Bradford and Leeds have a Magistrates Court as well as a Crown Court). Anyone has the right to watch a criminal trial, and you might like to go to the local Magistrates Court to experience this. However, be warned it is not like the TV and can be very boring! The British Justice System was first enshrined in law by the Magna Carta. It is an adversarial system, where two sides (the prosecution and the defence) argue the case in front of judge and jury. A jury consists of 12 ordinary members of the public. Everyone over 18 on the electoral right is eligible for jury service (normally two weeks), except prisoners, ex-prisoners and those with mental illness. Jurors are only paid expenses. On arrival at Court jurors are allocated randomly to courtrooms and take an oath to try the defendant fairly. Both the prosecution and the defence have the right to reject jurors if they think they could be biased. After the jury has listened to all the evidence, the judge instructs them on procedures and verdicts and the jury retires to reach a decision. A jury has to reach a majority decision. Persuading a Jury Effect of Order Testimony Persuasion Effect of Evidence Being Ruled Inadmissible Witness Appeal Attractiveness of the Defendant Witness Confidence Effect of Shields and Videotape on Children Giving Evidence Reaching a Verdict Stages in Decision Making Majority Influences Minority Influences Persuading a jury The Theories/Studies 1. Effects of memory structure on judgement (Pennington & Hastie, 1988) 2. The Impact of Expert Psychological Testimony (Loftus, 1980) 3. Investigating the effect of instructions to disregard inadmissible evidence (Pickel, 1995) The main approaches in this area are Cognitive, Social and psychology of Individual Differences. This is because this area looks at how memory can be influenced by the order in which information is presented, whether juries’ verdict’s are influenced by expert witnesses and how much weight juries give to evidence deemed as inadmissible by the judge. Subsection: Effects of Order of Testimony The adversarial system is all about persuading a jury, whichever side the counsel is representing. Psychologists have known for a long time that how we receive information affects how well we process and remember it. The primacy effect is one such factor. This was investigated by Murdoch. Murdoch (1962) in a laboratory study using word lists discovered an effect know as the Primacy effect, where words at the beginning of a list were more likely to be recalled than those from the middle or the end, hence the primacy effect. This might be something to do with our arousal level at the start of a task, its novelty or how much effect we are using, but it is a robust finding that translates into the courtroom where the jury are fresh and interested at the start of a case where the opening statement is presented by the prosecution. This would suggest that the prosecution had an advantage, as this evidence is always given first. However, later research by Glanzer & Cunitz (1966) discovered the primacy/recency effect, where participants recall words from the beginning and end of a list, but forget the middle. If we apply this to the courtoom, in real life although the defence closes last, this is followed by the Judge’s summing up, which might wipe out any recency effect for the defence. This would suggest that the most important pieces of 71 information are at the beginning of the trial (prosecution statement) and end (Judge’s summing up). Reconstructive memory theory suggests that people are inclined to “make sense” of any material that we are presented with, and research suggests that in order to do this we often try to fit the events into a linear narrative, or “story”, with a beginning a middle and an end. In addition, we are likely to fit our “story” around our existing schemas of what might or could have happened in the given situation. Imagine you are told: “Billy went to Johnny’s birthday party. When all the children were there, Johnny opened his presents. Later, they sang Happy Birthday and Johnny blew out the candles.” Was there a cake at the party? Many listeners will infer spontaneously, and most will agree when asked, that there was a cake at the birthday party. Yet, no cake is mentioned in the sentences above; indeed it is not certain that there was a cake. The cake is inferred because we share knowledge about birthday party traditions and about the physical world (the candles had to be on something)… [Pennington and Hastie 1993). Also, of interest is how counsel decide to structure their cases. Should they present their case as a story unfolding in chronological order, or should they use the benefit of the primacy and recency effects to present their best witness first or last even if this disrupts the order of events in the juror’s mind? In light of the above, careful consideration must be given to the order in which information is presented in court. 72 Effects of memory structure on judgement (Pennington & Hastie, 1988) Aims: To investigate whether or not story order evidence summaries are true causes of the final verdict decisions and the extent to which story order affects confidence in those decisions. Methodology: A laboratory experiment Participants: 130 students from Northwestern University and Chicago University were paid to participate in an hour long experiment. They were allocated to one of four conditions in roughly equal numbers: 39 prosecution items (guilty) in story order 39 prosecution items (guilty) in witness order 39 defence items (not guilty) in story order 39 defence items (not guilty) in witness order Story order = Chronological order of events; Witness order = Presenting best witness first/last Procedure: All participants listened to a tape recording of the stimulus trial (Commonwealth of Massachusetts v Caldwell) and then responded to written questions. They were told to reach either a guilty or not guilty verdict on a murder charge, and then asked to rate their confidence in their own decision on a 5-point scale. They were separated by partitions and did not interact with each other. In the story-order condition, evidence was arranged in its natural order. In the witness-order condition, evidence items were arranged in the order closest to the original trial. The defence items comprised 39 not-guilty pieces of evidence and the prosecution items, 39 guilty pieces of evidence from the original case. In all cases the stimulus trial began with the indictment and followed the normal procedure, ending with the Judge’s instructions. 73 Results: Table to show % of participants giving a guilty verdict Experimental condition Defence Prosecution Guilty verdict A Story order Story order 59% B Witness order Witness order 63% C Story order Witness order 31% D Witness order Story order 78% This shows that story order persuaded more jurors of Caldwell’s guilt in the prosecution case. If the defence presented its evidence in witness order, even more jurors would find a guilty verdict; if the positions were reversed and the defence had the benefit of the story order, the guilty rate drops to 31%. As predicted, the greatest level of confidence in their verdict was expressed by those who heard the defence or prosecution in story order. Least confidence was expressed by those who heard both witness order prosecution and defence. Discussion: Because the primacy and recency effect were controlled for, Pennington and Hastie are confident that they have shown the persuasive effect of presenting information in story order. In this case there is a difference in the defence case, which seems not to be as persuasive as the prosecution case, even when presented as a story. Pennington and Hastie account for this by referring to the case itself, in which they feel the defence case is less plausible (the victim was drunk and lunged forwards onto his assailant’s knife). The case was adapted from a real-life case. Conclusion: This shows that presenting the case in story order has a persuasive effect. Furthermore, any other order can be confusing for the jury. In this study the effect in the defence case was much smaller. 74 Subsection = Persuasion Loftus (1980) – Impact of Expert Psychological Testimony Background: Eye witnesses are widely used in criminal trials to add scientific credence to evidence, however from previous research by Loftus we know that eye witness testimony is unreliable. Psychologists such as Loftus are called in as expert witnesses for the defence to warn the jurors of possible problems with eye witness testimony. Explaining such factors as weapons effect, delays in giving testimony, etc. overall the research would suggest that any witnesses are likely to have an inaccurate or reconstructed version of the true events and therefore the jury should treat their testimony with caution. However, expert testimony does still have an impact on the decision the jury makes. Aim: To investigate the influence on jurors of expert testimony about eye witness identification. Hypothesis 1: That expert psychological testimony may have a greater influence on jurors attitudes to eyewitness testimony in violent crimes than non-violent crimes Hypothesis 2: That jurors exposed to expert testimony about eye witness testimony might pay more attention to eyewitness accounts Approach: Cognitive Details: A lab experiment using independent measures with a self-selected sample. Exp 1: 240 students and Exp 2: 120 students at Uni of Washington who all volunteered to participate in return for course credits. Experiment1 240 students randomly assigned to 4 conditions in a lab, participants were given booklets Containing: instructions, description of crime, a summary of the trial and a questionnaire requesting a verdict. CONDITION ONE CONDITION TWO CONDITION THREE CONDITION FOUR read expert no expert read expert no expert psychological psychological psychological psychological testimony testimony testimony testimony introduced by the introduced introduced by the introduced defence read a violent defence read a non-violent read a violent version of the read a non-violent version of the version of the crime version of the crime crime crime Expert Testimony included factors such as presence of a weapon, stress and the fact that witness had been drinking which could all interfere with accurate identification. Results Experiment1: percentage of guilt verdicts Expert psych testimony reduced the percentage of guilty verdicts from 58% to 39%. 75 Fewer guilty verdicts in the non-violent compared to violent 41% to 56% Expert testimony led to 12% reduction in guilty verdicts in the non-violent case opposed to 25% reduction of guilty verdicts in violent case. Experiment 2: 120 participated in groups of 6 in a lab. All were given a booklet containing instructions, description of crime, and a summary of the trial. This time however, they were in groups and had to deliberate for 30 minutes then asked for group verdict. All had the violent versions of the crime but only half the juries (groups) read the expert psychological testimony. Observers, listened in adjoining room and timed amount spent discussing eye witness testimony. Results Experiment 2: percentage of guilt verdicts No expert testimony: 7 juries voted to convict, 2 to acquit and 1 failed to reach verdict With exert witness: 3 juries voted to convict, 4 to acquit and 3 failed to reach verdict. Latter finding may be due to more time spent discussing eye witness testimony in these groups. In presence of expert testimony the juries spent over 10 mins discussing EWT, whereas in the absence of expert testimony they spent average of 7 mins. Discussion Expert testimony on EW identification influenced the jurors in 2 ways: 1. It promoted discussion of the eye witness evidence, and 2. Appeared to increase doubts of defendant’s guilt As Loftus points out it is not the aim of the expert to reduce guilty verdicts but ensure that jurors’ base decisions on the best possible understanding of the evidence in front of them. Clearly the word of an expert is influential, as Loftus accepts if both sides bring their own experts then it may lead to a battle of experts and as a result issues may be further confused rather than clarified. Subsection: Effect of evidence being ruled inadmissible The law says that in order for evidence to be admissible in That info is court, its relevance must outweigh its potential for prejudice. inadmissible! Inadmissible evidence includes hearsay, past convictions and evidence obtained by illegal means. If inadmissible evidence is presented in error by a witness, the judge will direct the jury to disregard what they have just heard. However, by drawing their attention to it, it may be that the jury in fact pays even more attention to it. This is know as reactance theory and suggests that jurors perceive instructions to ignore evidence as undermining their freedom to take all evidence into account. Juries have to make their judgements on the basis of evidence that is “on the record”. During a trial the proceedings are transcribed and this provides the “record” referred to. However, for evidence to be admissible, i.e. for it to enter the record, it must not be deemed to be prejudicial against the defendant. 76 Inadmissible evidence includes evidence from a confession that turned out to be gained under coercion, or gained illegally (e.g. through illegal recording , or a “wiretrap”), or evidence of prior convictions (because the jury may assume that having once committed a crime a defendant may be inclined to do it again), or hearsay evidence (second hand evidence that the witness did not personally see or hear happening). There are different rules about such evidence in the USA and in the UK. In the UK since 2004 past conviction evidence and hearsay evidence have been admissible if the judge permits it, and judges commonly do allow this. If something major was introduced that would seriously prejudice the outcome, then the jury would be dismissed and a re-trial ordered, and the offending counsel would be severely disciplined. Minor issues, however, that may be inadvertently revealed, may lead the judge to instruct the jury to ignore it. In the USA a counsel “dropping in” evidence, almost as a tactic to undermine/defend the defendant (depending whether they are the defence or prosecuting counsel), is common practice. In these cases the judge will order that the evidence be struck off the record and that the jury should ignore it. The trouble is can jurors really ignore something they have heard and ensure that it does not influence their verdict? Psychological studies have shown that by drawing their attention to inadmissible evidence jurors may in fact pay MORE attention to it. In addition, if they are instructed by the judge to pay no attention to it they may believe that they are being undermined in their freedom to take all evidence into account and give more rather than less importance to the “stricken” evidence. This is known as reactance theory. It is also referred to as the “backfire effect” where the judge’s instruction to ignore the evidence “backfires” and leads the juror to focus more rather than less on the “inadmissible” evidence. This had been shown in studies to commonly happen where the judge explained that the law required the jurors to ignore the evidence and that they must not take it into account in their ruling. Kerri Pickel (1995) carried out a study to investigate the effects of evidence being ruled inadmissible under different conditions, including different instructions from the judge: Investigating the effect of instructions to disregard inadmissible evidence (Pickel, 1995) Aims: 1. To look at the effect of prior convictions. 2. To look at the role of the judge’s instructions when they were followed by a legal explanation. 3. To examine how much the credibility of the witness affects the juror’s ability to ignore inadmissible statements. 77 Methodology: An experiment using a mock trial of a fictional theft with a mock jury. The critical evidence was introduced ‘by accident’ by the witnesses. The item was objected to by the attorney and then either allowed or overruled by the judge. In the former case, when jurors were instructed to ignore the inadmissible evidence, this ruling by the judge was sometimes supported by a legal explanation: that the inadmissible evidence might be suggestive of bad character in the defendant and so bias the jury. Sometimes no legal explanation was provided. Participants: 236 Ball State University psychology students participated as part of a course requirement. They were assigned randomly to one of the conditions in an independent measures design. Procedure: To manipulate the role of the judge’s instructions, three conditions were tested, one where the judge ruled the evidence as admissible, one where the judge ruled the evidence inadmissible and a third where the judge ruled the evidence inadmissible and also explained the legal basis for the ruling, there was also one control group. The critical witness’ credibility was also manipulated to see whether it would affect the jurors’ ability to ignore inadmissible evidence. On arrival, participants listened to an audiotape of the trial and then completed a questionnaire asking them to make several decisions about the case. Participants were asked to complete a questionnaire which asked them: a. their verdict b. their estimate of the probable guilt of the defendant c. a rating on a 1-10 scale of the extent to which knowledge of the prior conviction caused them to believe that the defendant was guilty d. to give a rating of the credibility of the witness There was a control group who did not get the critical evidence. Results: Mock jurors who heard the evidence ruled inadmissible and who received no explanation were able to follow instructions and ignore the evidence 43% guilty. Those who heard the evidence ruled inadmissible and who were given the explanation were more likely to find the defendant guilty 55%. More participants chose a guilty verdict when the witness credibility was rated high rather than low. In addition there was no significant effect on the use of prior conviction evidence as measured by the 10-point scale. 78 Conclusions: It would seem that calling attention to inadmissible evidence makes it more important to the jury and they then pay it more attention. They apply their sense of fair play to decide whether or not to make use of it, which can create the ‘backfire effect’. As long as the tactic is available to both sides and administered impartially by the judge, it could be a persuasive tool and is a minor matter. Summary: Persuading a jury The evidence about story order being more convincing is now well established and can be applied to understanding any body of knowledge (it is usually easier to learn your A2 studies if you think of them as stories unfolding in some kind of context). The use of expert witnesses in the courtroom is increasing and psychologists are being used to tell juries about the credibility of witness evidence. The research we have looked at suggests that juries listen to the expert and remember what they have been told, but may still choose to ignore it when coming to a verdict. The research on evidence being ruled as inadmissible suggests that juries feel confident in coming to verdicts irrespective of judges’ instructions. It would be fascinating to be able to watch a real jury come to its verdict on a true case, but this is never likely to happen and so we have to use mock trials and mock juries. This creates many problems of generalisability and validity, which means we have to be cautious in applying the results in real life. 79 Witness Appeal The Theories/Studies 1. The effects of physical attractiveness on jury verdicts (Castellow et al, 1990) 2. The effect of witness confidence on jurors’ assessment of eyewitness evidence (Penrod & Cutler, 1987) 3. The impact of protective shields and videotape testimony on conviction rates (Ross et al, 1994) The main approaches in this area are Individual Differences, Social and Cognitive Psychology. In particular, this area looks at research that suggests that the more attractive a person is, the less likely they are to receive a harsh punishment within the criminal justice system. Furthermore, the area delves into the research of witness confidence and how this may influence the jury’s verdict. The last section of this area addresses the impact of how different ways of giving evidence and their effect upon verdicts. Subsection = Attractiveness of the Defendant Activity Amanda Knox, Murder charge Karen Matthews, kidnap charge Which woman would you consider to be most attractive? Which woman would you consider to be happier, nicer or friendlier? Which woman would you consider to be scary, miserable and boring? Both women are attending trial for their offences. Do you think that their attractiveness will influence the jury’s view of them? Much has been made of the attractiveness of witnesses and defendants in court. This can be explained by psychological concepts such as impression formation, attribution and the ‘halo effect’ described by Asch (1946) where a positive halo of pleasant characteristics is imagined when one favourable characteristic is known about an individual (the reverse is also true). Another relevant theory is implicit personality theory as described by Dion in 1972 who hypothesised that the attractiveness stereotype attributed to beautiful people included the supposition that they must have attractive personalities. What this means is that a jury may make decisions about witnesses and defendants based on their physical appearance, assuming that this relates to their personality, colouring their view of the evidence. This is why defendants’ legal teams advise smart dress and a neat and clean appearance for a day in course. One piece of research into this effect was by Castellow in 1990. The effects of physical attractiveness on jury verdicts (Castellow et al, 1990) Aims: To test the hypothesis that an attractive defendant is less likely to be seen as guilty. Secondly, when the victim is attractive, the defendant is more likely to be found guilty. Finally, to look for any gender differences in jury verdicts depending on attractiveness. Methodology: A laboratory experiment using the mock-trial format. It used an independent measures design. Participants: 71 male and 74 female students who participated for extra credit in their introductory psychology classes at East Carolina University. Procedure: Participants were told that they would be reading a sexual harassment case and would have to answer questions on it. With the case were attached photographs of the victim and defendant previously categorised as attractive or unattractive by a panel on a scale of 1 to 9, where 9 was very attractive and 1 was very unattractive. The dependent variable was measured by the answer to the question ‘Do you think Mr Radford is guilty of sexual harassment?’ Towards the end of the case booklet they were given, participants were asked to rate the defendant and the victim on 11 bipolar scales such as dull-exciting, nervous-calm, warm-cold. 81 Results: Analysis of the ratings revealed that physically attractive defendants and victims were rated positively on other personality variables as well. When the defendant was attractive, guilty verdicts were found 56% of the time against 76% for an unattractive defendant. When the victim was attractive, the guilty verdict followed 77% of the time with 55% for the unattractive victim. No significant gender differences were found and both sexes were equally influenced by appearance. Conclusion: Although the findings come from a mock trial, when applied in the courtroom it seems that appearance does indeed have a powerful effect, and this finding has been supported by much other research. A defendant would be well advised to make the best of their appearance when appearing in court. Subsection = Witness Confidence Activity I am 100% certain that is the man who robbed me I, er, I’m not sure that is the man who robbed me as, er, it was dark and, er, I didn’t have my glasses on Which victim seems to be more confident of identifying the person who robbed them? Which victim do you think is likely to have their opinion taken into account when deciding upon the verdict? Why? One of the most convincing features of a witness is their confidence when giving testimony. In a typical case, giving evidence in Court is a nerve-racking experience and it would be completely understandable for witnesses to appear nervous and hesitate when answering questions under examination with all eyes upon them. When a witness is confident, the jury gain confidence in what they are saying and believe it to be more accurate. However this confidence can come from a system variable (how the police have conducted the earlier interviews and recording of evidence). 82 Penrod and Cutler (1995) in a review of available evidence concluded that a major source of juror unreliability is reliance on witness confidence, even when measured at the time identification is made, when it should be at its most accurate. Confidence appears to be influenced by repeated questioning, briefings in anticipation of crossexamination and feedback about the behaviour of other witnesses (being told other witnesses had identified the suspect). This reliance on confidence appears to be unaffected by judges’ instructions and cross-examination. The effect of witness confidence on jurors’ assessment of eyewitness evidence (Penrod & Cutler, 1987) Aim: To examine several factors, including confidence, which jurors might consider when evaluating eyewitness identification evidence. Methodology: An experiment using a mock-trial scenario. Independent measures design. Participants: Undergraduates, eligible and experienced jurors. Procedure: - A videotaped trial of a robbery was presented in which eye witness identification played a key role. The witness testified that she was either 80 or 100% confident that she had identified the robber. Nine other variables, all at both high and low level, were introduced into the film, four of which are reported below. Participants experienced either the high or low condition variables on a random basis and after watching the film they were asked to decide whether the robber was guilty or not. Results: 4 of 10 factors and levels Suspect in disguise High – heavily disguised Low – minimal disguise Weapon focus – a weapon is used High – weapon clearly brandished Low – weapon visible % Conviction 63 63 64 63 As can be seen in this table, witness confidence is the only statistically significant effect of these listed variables under the conditions of the mock trial (all the other six variables now shown were virtually identical). In a further 9 studies, Cutler et al. looked at the relation between confidence and accuracy. The correlations across the 9 studies between the two variables were 0.00 to 83 0.20 which is very weak indeed. Retention interval 14 days 2 days 63 63 Witness Confidence about ID 100% Confident 80% Confident 67 60 Conclusion: The evidence in the field is consistent in showing that confidence is a poor predictor of witness accuracy. It also shows that jurors’ trust in it is undiminished, even if the judge advises the jury to be wary of it in their summing up. Subsection = Effect of shields and videotape on children giving evidence In many cases involving sexual abuse, kidnapping or domestic violence a child is the only witness. The experience of giving evidence in a courtroom is often traumatic for children, so they have been able to give evidence from behind a screen, or from outside the courtroom via a video link. It is hoped that by doing this, the psychological stress will be diminished. However, from the defendant’s point of view, having the child give evidence via video link may suggest increased likelihood of guilt to a jury by the implication that the child needs protection from the defendant. The study by Ross et al. (1994) attempts to find some empirical evidence to help decide whether using a protective screen is prejudicial to the defendant and whether the use of such measures enhances a witness’s credibility or makes them seem fragile and unreliable. This is an important study in an area of vital research, as the number of cases involving children continues to rise. The impact of protective shields and videotape testimony on conviction rates (Ross et al, 1994) Aims: 1. To find out if the use of protective shields and videotaped testimony increases the likelihood of a guilty verdict. 2. To investigate the effects of protective devices on jury reaction to testimony – do the experience credibility inflation (more likely to believe) or deflation (less likely to believe). Methodology: A mock trial based on an actual court transcript. A professional film crew recorded actors playing the roles in the case. Three versions of the case were created. The first was open court with the child in full view, the second had the child behind a 4 x 6 ft screen and in the third condition the child’s testimony came via a video link. 84 Participants: 300 college students (150 male, 150 female) from an introductory psychology class, the majority were white and middle class. They were told it was a study of psychology and the law. 100 students were assigned to each condition. Procedure: Participants watched one of the three versions of the 2-hour film of a court case of alleged abuse which had the child’s father as the accused defendant, and the mother, two expert witnesses, one for either side and the child herself taking part as witnesses. The alleged abuse was a single touch while the father was giving the child a bath and the case focused on whether or not it was innocent or sexual in nature. The judge in the case read a warning before directing the jury not to imply guilt by their use. After the case, the participants gave their verdicts and rated the credibility of the child witness on various aspects of her story. They also rated the defendant on a variety of dimensions of his credibility. Results: Guilty % Not Guilty % Open Court 51 49 Shield 46 54 Video 49 51 The guilty verdicts show no significant differences between conditions. However there was a significant difference between male and female participants – 58.6% of females against 38.6% of males found the defendant guilty. The jury’s perception of the credibility of the defendant did not differ across the three conditions, although once again there was a gender difference with more female students rating the defendant as less credible than the males. The same pattern emerged for the credibility of the witness, with no difference across the three conditions and a very significant difference between genders where again the female students rated the child as more credible. Conclusion: Before drawing the obvious conclusions from this study, it is worth looking at a followup research Ross et al. investigated. All remained the same as before, but this time the tape was stopped immediately after the child testified. This time the results showed a clear difference, with participants in the open court condition far more likely to convict and this time the effect of gender was not significant. The effect on the credibility of the witness was the same as before i.e. no significant difference between conditions. The results of the two pieces of research suggest that the defendant is not more at risk if protective devices are used, with the video condition slightly less likely to produce a conviction. The biggest differences were found in the second experiment 85 where the trial was artificially interrupted to test the jury’s response. When a case is allowed to run its full course and the judge’s warnings are in place there is no disadvantage to the defendant, which raised a dilemma – what of the position of the child witness? Summary: Witness appeal The effects of attractiveness and confidence are well researched and consistent, and create a worrying problem for a defendant who is unattractive and hesitant. Their legal counsel will do their best to prepare them for the courtroom experience, but these people may be treated more harshly by the jury. The issue of the use of shields is again a difficult issue in real life. The sight of a young child giving evidence in a courtroom would be emotional for the jury as well as the child, and it is thought better to protect the child from the experience. What if this means that the child’s story is not properly heard? This is an ongoing dilemma for the court and it will come down to individual judges’ discretion to ensure that each case is heard in the pest possible way. 86 Reaching a verdict The Theories/Studies 1. Stages and influences on decision-making (Hastie et al, 1983) 2. The power of majority influence and conformity (Asch, 1953) 3. Investigation of the influence of perceived autonomy on minority influence (Nemeth and Wachtler, ) The main approaches in this area are Individual Differences, Social and Cognitive Psychology. At the end of a trial the jury return to the courtroom to give their verdict. How do they reach that verdict? The problem for researchers is that juries are swore to secrecy about their deliberations, which take place behind closed doors, even after the trial, they are prohibited by law from discussing it. This means that researchers have to rely on mock trials and reconstructions to investigate jury behaviour. Some of the influences include size of the jury, cognitive processes, pre-trial publicity, ethnicity, gender, individual differences, leadership and the social processes which influence decision making such as majority and minority influence. This final section on how juries reach a verdict will consider the psychological processes that affect the jurors in the jury room. There is a lot of research into group processes and social psychology which can be applied here. Reaching a verdict involves complex processing of data, as shown in the next piece of research by Hastie et al (1983). Subsection = Stages and Influences in Decision Making Stages and influences on decision-making (Hastie et al, 1983) Hastie et al carried out research in which mock-juries for a fictional murder trial were used. There were three conditions: P’s had to arrive at a 12-0 (unanimous) verdict P’s had to reach a 10-2 (majority) verdict P’s had to reach 8-4 (divided) verdict All the groups were videotaped and Hastie el al observed how the discussions developed. Based upon this research, Hastie et al argue that jury discussions go through the following stages in order to make their decision/verdict: Stages in jury decision-making Orientation Period Relaxed & open discussion Set the agenda Raise questions and explore facts Different opinions arise Open Confrontation Fierce debate Focus on detail Explore different interpretations Pressure on the minority to conform Support for the group decision is established Reconciliation Attempts to smooth over conflicts Tension released through humour Influences in [Jury] Decision Making Some of the influences include: cognitive processes such as belief in a “just world” or attribution errors, pre-trial publicity, ethnicity, gender, witness confidence, use of expert witness evidence, order of presentation of testimony etc. In the jury room factors such as leadership and the social processes which influence decision making such as majority and minority influence also have an impact on jury decision making. We will discuss minority and majority influence below. Minority vs Majority Influence Minority influence is the result of INFORMATIONAL SOCIAL INFLUENCE- the desire to be right. In contrast, majority influence might be due to public compliance (NORMATIVE SOCIAL INFLUENCE) that is just passively going along with the majority to fit in or it might be due to INFORMATIONAL SOCIAL INFLUENCE- the desire to be right, thus acceptance of the majority view. 88 Subsection = Majority Influence The power of majority influence and conformity (Asch, 1953) Solomon Asch conducted a classic experiment on conformity to find out whether people would still conform (comply) when the correct answer was obvious. Aim: The aim of this study was to find out how people would behave when given an unambiguous (clear) task. Would they be influenced by the behaviour of others, or would they stick firmly to what they knew to be right? How much conformity to majority influence would there be? Methodology: A laboratory experiment Participants: In total, 123 American male undergraduates (students) were tested. Procedure: Asch showed a series of lines (the ‘standard’ or ‘stimulus’ line and the possible answers) to participants seated around a table. They were asked ‘Which of three lines, A, B or C matches the stimulus line X?’ X A B C This happened in groups of 7-9 participants but only one was naïve (a real participant). The others were stooges or confederates (insiders) of the researcher. Asch was interested to see if even in a crystal-clear decision, an individual would defer to the majority. The confederates were told to give the same incorrect answer on 12 critical trials. In total there were 18 trials with each participant: In the first two trials, confederates answered correctly, and then 16 more trials, in 12 critical trials the confederates gave wrong answers and 4 more where confederates gave correct answers. 89 Findings: The crucial measure was how often naïve participants gave the same wrong answer as the confederates on the critical trials. This was a measure of conformity. Overall, there was a 32% conformity rate. In other words, participants agreed with the wrong answer on about a third of the critical trials. But there were important individual differences. For example, no one conformed on all the critical trials, and about 25% didn’t conform even once. About 75% conformed at least once. Participants appeared to be experiencing a lot of stress through the experiment (e.g. nervous laughter, fidgeting). Just to confirm that the lines were unambiguous, Asch conducted a control trial with no confederates giving wrong answers. Asch found that people do make mistakes about 1% of the time. Conclusion: Because the answers on the critical trials were obviously incorrect, Asch’s study shows the impact that a majority can have on an individual. It shows a surprisingly strong tendency to conform to group pressure. However, the majority does not have the same impact on every individual. For Asch the important finding was that there was any conformity at all. However, Asch also saw the fact that on two thirds of the trials his participants had remained independent as clear evidence of how people could resist the pressure to conform. Why did participants conform in Asch’s study? Asch interviewed some of his participants and found that they tended to give one of three reasons: Distortion of perception- they really did think their wrong answers were right Distortion of judgement- they felt doubt about the accuracy of their judgement and therefore yielded to the majority view Distortion of action- they didn’t want to be ridiculed and therefore went along with the group. Those who didn’t conform said that either they were confident in their answer or they thought that the majority was probably right but they still had to say what they saw. The need to belong is thought to be part of the socialisation process, which particularly influences those with poorer self-esteem who are insecure about their own worth and opinions. It has been linked to a conformist style of personality and it would be this type of person who would be one of the 32% who conform. The need to be right arises when any ambiguous situation occurs and we look to other people’s solutions to guide our own. Clearly in the Asch experiment we have an unambiguous stimulus and so it is more likely that the need to belong is prevalent. 90 In a jury, the task is far more complex and an individual juror may be for more influenced by the need to make the right decision and give the correct verdict for the evidence. Research with over 225 juries has shown that the majority guilty view (8 to 11 jurors) expressed as a vote early in the deliberations led to a guilty verdict 86% of the time, showing that the majority has the most powerful influence (Abbott and Batt, 1999). There is some informal evidence to suggest that the first thing many juries do is take a vote when they withdraw, as a starting point for their discussions. Obviously the suggestion of this research is that conformity effects might be better resisted if the vote was delayed until discussion about the evidence has taken place. Subsection = Minority Influence So far we have considered majority influence- the pressures on the minority to conform to the views and behaviour of the majority. However, what about the minority influencing the majority? Minority influence occurs when a minority rejects the established norm of the majority of group members and gets the majority to move to the position of the minority. Minority influence is the result of INFORMATIONAL SOCIAL INFLUENCE- the desire to be right. In contrast, majority influence might be due to public compliance (NORMATIVE SOCIAL INFLUENCE) that is just passively going along with the majority to fit in or it might be due to INFORMATIONAL SOCIAL INFLUENCE- the desire to be right, thus acceptance of the majority view. Moscovici (1969) carried out a set of experiments to test the minority influence. He got groups of six participants (four naive, two stooges) to make colour perception judgements about a series of slides which were all blue. He set up the conditions: Condition 1 Stooges all said slides were green Condition 2 2/3 stooges said slides were green Condition 3 Stooges were inconsistent in slide colours The results of his study showed that there was 32% conformity only in condition 1. Moscovici’s experiment suggests that minorities can influence majorities as long as they are consistent in their views and that this consistency will create conflict in the others, leading them to question and possibly change their views. 91 Why is consistency important? According to Moscovici when the minority are consistent they speak with a single voice and give the impression they are convinced they are right. As a result they appear confident and thus are taken seriously by the majority. Thus, minority influence is based on informational social influence- providing the majority with new ideas that cause them to re-examine their views. In the following study, the concepts of autonomy or internal locus is combined with consistency to find its effect on minority influence. Investigation of the influence of perceived autonomy on minority influence (Nemeth and Wachtler, ) Aim: To investigate the influence of perceived autonomy (choosing where to sit at a table) and consistency on minority influence. Methodology: A laboratory experiment as a mock trial. Participants: Groups of five participants (one is a stooge) drawn from an adult sample of students Procedure: The group of five has to deliberate on the amount of compensation due for a victim of an injury. After hearing the facts, everyone makes an individual verdict and is then taken to another room where there is a rectangular table with two seats at either of the long sides of the table and one at the head of the table. Condition 1: In half the groups the participants are asked to sit at the table with the confederate/stooge choosing the seat at the head or the top of the table. Condition 2: In the other groups the experimenter tells everyone where to sit (including the confederate/stooge). Both groups then deliberate on the case. During the discussion, the confederate consistently adopts a deviant position, suggesting a figure of $3,000 in compensation instead of $10,000-$25,000 which was the view of the rest. Results: The confederate exerts influence when he is consistent and when he is perceived as autonomous because he has chosen his seat, whereas when seated by the experimenter he has little influence (measured by the difference between the original individual verdicts and the later group ones). Moreover, when he has been influential, this effect continues into a second case. When he sits at the head of the table is seen as more consistent and confident. 92 Discussion: This has interesting repercussions for the jury room where people sit around a long table. Could the effect be replicated in the classroom? Many examples exist where minorities have influenced the majority into adopting their views by being consistent over time, for example gay civil partnerships (homosexuality used to be considered a mental illness), belief in global warming (this was originally an extreme view held by a few scientist), and cult music groups or programmes which become mainstream. However, it is possible that in the jury room in real life this effect is weakened by the need for unanimity in a limited time. Summary: Reaching a verdict Once out in the jury room, many influences come into play in reaching a verdict and only a few have been covered in this section. Majority and minority influence are probably the best known, but there is much more research on decision-making which can be applied here. 93 REACHING A VERDICT June 2010 1. Describe how attractiveness of a defendant can influence courtroom behaviour. (10) 2. Evaluate the methodology used in witness appeal. (15) January 2011 1. What is the effect on a jury of evidence being ruled inadmissible in court? (10) 2. Evaluate the usefulness of research into persuading a jury. (15) June 2011 1. How might the view of the majority influence a jury when reaching a verdict? (10) 2. Evaluate the application of research into what influences reaching a verdict in court. (15) 1. What effect does the order in which testimony if presented have on persuading a jury? (10) 2. Discuss the limitations of research into persuading a jury. (15) January 2012 1. Describe research into the effect of shields and videotape on children giving evidence. (10) 2. Discuss difficulties which may be encountered when researching witness appeal. (15) June 2012 1. Describe the stages of jury decision-making when reaching a verdict. (10) 2. Discuss ethical issues when researching influences on reaching a verdict in court. (15) January 2013 1. Describe how persuasion may be used in a courtroom. (10) 2. To what extent is research into persuading a jury useful? (15) June 2013 1. Describe research into the effect of witness confidence in the courtroom. (10) 2. Assess the usefulness of research into witness appeal in the courtroom. (15) 94 Topic 4 – After a Guilty Verdict 1. Imprisonment Planned behaviours once freed from jail Depression/ suicide risk The prison situation and roles 2. Alternatives to imprisonment Probation Restorative justice ‘Looking Death worthy’ 3. Treatment programmes Cognitive skill programmes Anger management Using ear acupuncture with a drug rehabilitation programme 95 Topic 4 – After a guilty verdict Once a guilty verdict has been announced the defendant becomes part of the penal system and is an offender. Depending on the severity of the crime and whether or not this is a first offence, the offender will be sentenced to a punishment. This could be community service, being tagged, an ASBO (anti-social behaviour order), a fine, a probation order or a prison sentence. Psychologists have been interested in whether any of these punishments work and if so which are most effective. Effectiveness is measured by looking at recidivism rates, this means re-offending rates. The penal system has two aims, first to punish and second to rehabilitate the offender. These two often come into conflict in the public mind, especially when a particularly offensive crime has been committed. It is hard for the victim’s relatives and the public generally to see criminals getting help and support which is costly when the victim generally gets nothing or may have lost their life. The idea of rehabilitation is that offenders who are safe to let out into the community can become useful citizens who contribute to society through working and paying taxes, instead of being a drain on resources. If the punishment and rehabilitation has been successful the offender should not reoffend. 96 Theories of Imprisonment The Theories/Studies 1. The impact of community-based employment on offender reintegration (Gillis, 2005) 2. Depression/Suicide Risk in Jail: Palmer and Connelly 3. The Prison Situation and Roles (Haney,Banks & Zimbardo, 1973) The main approaches within this area are Individual Differences, Social and Cognitive psychology. This is because all of these factors will influence the thoughts, feelings and behaviours of the offenders once remanded within the prison institution. In the summer of 2007 the UK prison population reached an all time high of 81,000, despite having an official capacity of only 78,000, leading to serious overcrowding. What are the implications of this? In order for prison to work, prisoners must be educated and made more employable, to ensure they are more likely to remain out of prison and instead contribute to society. Otherwise, prisons become simply ‘Universities of crime’. The Prison Reform Trust (2007) has found that many prisoners have not reached the levels of literacy and numeracy expected of average 11 year old; 50% in reading, 66% in numeracy and 80% in reading. 50% do not have the skills required by 96% of all jobs and 50% have been excluded from school. These statistics make prisoners, along with their criminal records, virtually unemployable without successful educational intervention within the prison system. Many people are critical of the parole system (the fact that prisoners are often released early and rarely serve their full sentence); however, this is an important incentive, crucial to the smooth operation of the prison system. Applications for parole are allowed after a minimum term (set by the judge) has been served. Success will depend on the nature of the offence, the judge’s comments on sentencing and crucially the inmate’s behaviour in prison. This gives the prisoner an incentive to behave and comply with prison rules, without this incentive many inmates would be unmanageable. 97 Subsection = Planned behaviours once freed from jail The theory of planned behaviour applied to an offender’s likelihood of ‘going straight’ Azjen (1988) Attitude to ‘going straight’ Subjective norm ‘Crime is not normal’ Intention (not to reoffend) Behaviour (staying out of prison) Perceived behavioural control Azjen’s theory is that a prisoner’s positive intention to stay out of prison is key to success. This intention will be influenced by the prisoner’s personality, their beliefs about the value of their life outside of prison and their sense of control over their destiny. In reality it is not that easy. The impact of community-based employment on offender reintegration (Gillis & Nafekh, 2005) Aim: To investigate the effect on recidivism rates of a community-based employment scheme. Procedure: A content analysis of data from Canada’s Offender Management System was completed on 23,525 individuals released between January 1998 and January 2005 (95% were male). A matched pairs design was used were offenders were matched for gender, risk 98 level, release year, sentence length, family/marital relations, substance abuse, emotional orientation, community functioning and attitudes. The two groups that were compared on outcomes were those employed prior to release on a special programme for offenders and those that were unemployed. The programme focused on job-search techniques, individual psychometric assessments and on-the-job placements. Findings: Those on the employment programme were more likely to remain on conditional release (ie. not sent back to prison) and less likely to return to custody with a new offence. At the end of the study period 70% of the employed group remained out of prison, compared to just 55% of the unemployed group. Median return time (to jail) was also longer for the employed group (37 months, compared to 11 months). Conclusions: Employment-based programmes play an important role in the last few months of an offender’s sentence, giving them some of the skills they need and helping them to integrate into the community. Furthermore, by planning the offender’s return to the community addresses some of the points raised in Azjen’s model and increases the likelihood of success. Subsection = Depression/suicide risk in prison According to the Howard League for Penal Reform there were 92 unnatural deaths in prisons in 2007, with a further 100 resuscitated. At any one time there are 1500 prisoners on suicide watch. Hanging is the most likely form of suicide in prison, although great care is taken to prevent it. The suicide rate in the prison population is about four times that of the suicide rate in the general population. In addition to recorded suicides are other unnatural deaths, including deaths by “consciously self-inflicted injury” (CSI), and if these are added to official statistics the suicide rates in prisons would be much higher. Dr Enda Dooley describes one such case in his (1990) study (see below) Why? Lonely Uncertainty Isolation from family Fear Overcrowding 99 Subsection = The Prison Situation and Role You will remember studying Reicher and Haslam in Year 12 and discussing the infamous Stanford Prison Experiment (SPE) by Haney, Banks and Zimbardo (1973). In this study the behaviour of the prisoners and guards resembled that of real prisoners and guards in real prisons and this led the researchers to conclude that the reason for this was that the brutal set up of the prison situation impacted on the participants: "Despite the fact that guards and prisoners were essentially free to engage in any form of interaction . . . the characteristic nature of their encounters tended to be negative, hostile, affrontive and dehumanising" (Haney et al., 1973) The researchers concluded that the level of brutality and distress they observed was caused by the situation and did not result from the nature of the individuals who took part (remember they selected conforming, normal, average middle-class students to take part in their study. It had to be stopped on day 6 because of the distress of the “prisoners” under the “guards” cruel regime) Zimbardo et al had hoped that the SPE would lead to an improvement in the American Penal system and the treatment of prisoners. Politically, there was a will to move towards alternatives to prison, no prisons were being built and the prevailing belief of The Supreme Court was that humane prisons are a sign of a cultured society. There was a belief that (at the time) prisons were too harsh and there was not enough focus on rehabilitation. It is worth noting here that American jails remain much tougher than UK prisons. And, of course, many states still have the death penalty. In their paper Zimbardo et al summarise the changes that actually happened from 1973-1998 and make “a series of reform-oriented proposals drawn from [the SPE] and related studies on the power of social situations and institutional settings that can be applied to the current crisis in American corrections” (Haney and Zimbardo, Page 1) Haney & Zimbardo (1998) – The Past and Future of U.S. Prison Policy Twenty-Five Years After the Stanford Prison Experiment Part One – Summarising changes over the last 25 years Haney and Zimbardo cite the following changes to the American penal system since the SPE –they refer to these as: “groundwork for the mean season of corrections that the nation has now entered… The criminal justice system not only has become increasingly harsh and punitive but also has obscured many of the psychological insights on which the SPE and numerous other empirical studies were based–insights about the power of social situations and contexts to influence and control behaviour”. President Ronald Reagan’s Republican ‘War on Drugs’ (1980s) led to political pressure to put more criminals behind bars. The concept of rehabilitation was discredited – criminals deserved punishment. Haney and Zimbardo refer to this as the “death of rehabilitation” Determinate sentencing was introduced – whatever sentence was given had to be served in full with no chance for parole Many new prisons were consequently built, USA imprisons more people than any other modern nation (2008 prison population = 2 million). The building rate has never kept up with demand and Haney and Zimbardo note that for the last 25 years the prison system has been considerd to be “in crisis” Racial bias in prison population 48% African-Caribbean men although they only represent 6% of the general population. Many for drug offences. Hispanics and women were also over represented compared with white men. Introduction of the ‘Supermax’ prisons : example of a ‘Supermax’ cell. The Supermax cell is designed for ultra-secure, long term segregated confinement and the offender lives in extreme isolation and with little 102 stimulation and reduced privileges. They are locked in 23 out of 24 hours. These cells are used to house “violent or disruptive” prisoners in free standing cell blocks in isolation from the other prisoners. In 1998 36 states already had Supermax units and work was underway to increase capacity by 25%. Haney and Zimbardo argue that the USA is perpetuating discrimination against black people and encouraging dispositional explanations of criminal behaviour. The ‘Supermax’ cell is another example of a dispositional explanation being used to categorise some prisoners as ‘problem prisoners’. The implication is that nothing can be done with or for these “violent” types – and consequently they are often “ineligible for prison jobs, vocational training programs, and, in many states, education” (p.8). [Key term – Dispositional Explanations: A dispositional explanation sees the cause of criminal behaviour as being entirely due to an individual’s personality, rather than considering the effect on them of the environment, which would be a situational explanation]. Finally, The Supreme Court had shown an unsympathetic stance on appeals from prisoners who used the Eight Amendment, which states that “no cruel and unusual punishments” should be imposed on a U.S. citizen, thus setting a punitive tone which reflected current attitudes to crime. Part Two – Suggestions for Improvements, using lessons learned from the SPE and other psychological studies 1) Prisons should be used sparingly as they are psychologically damaging: resources should be directed into alternatives to prison. 2) Taking a lesson from the SPE where psychologically healthy young men were affected by the prison, prisons should take account of individual differences, in particular how a person is likely to react to confinement in a crowded, violent environment. 3) The conditions that exist when someone comes out of prison are as important as those on the inside. Rehabilitation programmes are needed to teach prisoners the skills to cope once they are released. “Otherwise, prisoners who were illprepared for job and social situations before they entered prison become more so over time, and the longer they have been imprisoned, the more likely it is that rapid technological and social change will have dramatically transformed the world to which they return.” Haney and Zimbardo p.13). Rehabilitation is necessary to reduce recidivism rates. 4) Both the SPE and Milgram’s obedience studies showed how difficult it is to predict or even explain behaviour in terms of individual characteristics, so in prison, explanations of disciplinary infractions and violence should focus more on the context in which they happened and less on the prisoners who engaged in 103 them. In addition, predicting reoffenders would be best done by considering the context in which they will be placed rather than their individual characteristics 5) Haney and Zimbardo acknowledge that they got caught up in the SPE and lost their balanced perspective as objective experimenters. Therefore they suggest that reform needs to come from people outside of the prison system who are empowered to act on it, since those within the system are not impartial… “to depend exclusively on those whose perspectives have been created and maintained by these powerful situations to, in turn, transform or control them is shortsighted and psychologically naive. This task must fall to those with a different logic and point of view, independent of and free from the forces of the situation itself. To be sure, the current legal retreat to hands-off policies in which the courts defer to the presumably greater expertise of correctional officials ignores the potency of prison settings to alter the judgments of those charged with the responsibility of running them. The SPE and much other research on these powerful environments teach that this retreat is terribly illadvised.” (Haney and Zimbardo, p. 15) Psychological knowledge should be used to improve the conditions within prisons. “…psychology can be made relevant to the broad and pressing national problems of crime and justice, that the discipline can assist in stimulating badly needed social and legal change, and that scholars and practitioners can improve these policies with sound data and creative ideas” Haney and Zimbardo, p16. 104 Summary: Imprisonment This section has perhaps presented a depressing picture of the penal system. We have seen that despite much effort and real commitment by researchers and prison staff, re-conviction rates after imprisonment stubbornly remain at around 60%. The picture in the USA is one of a movement to a more extreme regime which at present prioritises containment and control above rehabilitation. Overcrowding has led to more suicides and deaths both in the USA and in the UK, increasing the stress on prisoners and staff. Prison can have negative psychological effects upon those incarcerated. Prison and treatment programmes have to be seen within the legal and political frameworks of the country in which they exist Alternatives to Imprisonment The Theories/Studies 1. Investigation of the experiences of offenders on probation orders (Mair & May, 1997) 2. Review of restorative justice and its effectiveness in preventing reoffending (Sherman & Strang, 2007) 3. Investigation of the relation between stereotypically black features in offenders and the likelihood of receiving the death sentence (Eberhardt et al, 2006) The main approaches within this area are Individual Differences, Social, Cognitive and Physiological Psychology. This is because this section focuses upon the factors that can influence the success of alternatives to imprisonment in order to reduce crime. Subsection = Probation [Key term: probation order (or “community rehabilitation order”, as it is now known in the UK) = In 1896 the temperance society of the Church of England started the probation service by giving alcoholics a gift of 5 shillings to help them reform. At that time alcohol was seen as the ‘root of all evil’. The recipients of the gift had to sign the pledge (to give up alcohol) and follow the teachings of the bible to keep them away from temptation and crime. The modern probation service supervises over 200,000 offenders on four main types of community sentences: Community rehabilitation orders Community punishment orders Combined orders (rehabilitation & punishment) Drug treatment and testing orders Approximately 90% are male and 25% aged between 16-20 years old. 70% of clients are on community sentences, the remaining 30% are on licence after being released early from prison sentences. Any breach of probation is taken very seriously and can lead to a custodial sentence. 106 Probation Order You Must Stay our of trouble Immediately report any change of address or phone number Be on time for appointments Let staff visit your home You Must Not Miss an appointment or be late Turn up under the influence of alcohol or drugs Upset or threaten people Make racist, sexist or other offensive remarks Keep to court requirements Keep to your licence if you have been released from prison Take part in any activities your probation officer asks you to Investigation of the experiences of offenders on probation orders (Mair & May, 1997) Aim: To investigate the experience of offenders on probation orders across England & Wales Procedure: 3299 offenders were chosen at random from 22 different probation offices, these represented all age groups and offences. There was a 40% drop out rate, with a disproportionate number of these coming from London. Interviews were conducted by independent researchers, questions were mainly closed, likert scale and multiple choice. Results: 88% felt probation was useful. 60% thought there probation officer would help them individually. Having someone independent to talk to seems to be the most useful function of the probation officer, and the topics discussed included: things to do, problems with accommodation, problems with money, employment, family problems, why they committed the offence, drugs and alcohol problems an their health generally. However, only 37% said probation would stop them re-offending. Conclusion: Probation is seen as useful by offenders. However, over a third on offenders went on to re-offend. Other factors need to be considered such as socio-economic status, unemployment, family, peers etc 107 Subsection = Restorative Justice Historically, the offender is seen as having committed a crime against the state, eg. Smith Vs Regina (Lawsuits are give the name of the defendant and the Queen). However, crimes have victims and victims often feel that their experiences and expectations are not taken into account by the justice system. Restorative justice aims to redress this balance, crime is seen as being against a person or organisation and victims are allowed to be part of what happens. Restorative justice has to be voluntary for all parties and seeks a positive outcome. It is respectful and not degrading for either offender or victim. Restorative justice aims to provide victims with: An opportunity to explain the impact of the crime (impact statements) An acknowledgement of the harm caused A chance to ask questions Some control and choice Peace of mind about the future. Restorative justice has a long history, although in the past it was often more about revenge and compensation. Sharia law (Muslim law) which is based on the Koran is dispensed by elders who decided on the amount of compensation a victim or victim’s family is entitled to. They suggest that this system works better than the UK system as, “Here you have the fullest prisons in the world because you feed the criminals and they become fatter and do the crimes again”, "Our experience may be something we can offer to this country's penal system." Restorative justice is tough both for victims and offenders. For offenders they have to face up to the consequences of their actions, but for victims they may be forced to relive frightening and upsetting experiences. There are 4 types of RJ: Victim Offender Mediation Group Conferencing Restorative Conferencing Indirect Mediation/Reparation 108 Review of restorative justice and its effectiveness in preventing reoffending (Sherman & Strang, 2007) Aim: To look at restorative justice in practice and measure its effectiveness in terms of reoffending. Procedure: A content analysis was carried out on 424 academics papers on restorative justice (secondary data). From these 36 studies were found that compared re-offending rates for those who were part of restorative justice programmes and those that were not. Results: Restorative justice is more effective when there is a personal victim and tends to work for violence and property crime. However, restorative justice is not effective in all cases. The clearest benefits are for the victim, it reducing post traumatic shock syndrome and helps them come to terms with the crime. Conclusion: There is strong evidence to suggest that restorative justice is effective in some cases. There is support for its increased use, especially with young, first time offenders. Subsection = ‘Looking Death Worthy’ Eberhardt et al (2006) suggest that in the USA the more stereotypically ‘Black’ your features are the more likely you are to be given a death sentence for your crime. This may seem an alarming idea, but think back to early theories by Lombroso which suggested that criminal shared certain physical characteristics. It is a fact that the Criminal justice system is dominated by white people and that many of the states which still have the death penalty are Southern states where discrimination and prejudice were historically worse. Investigation of the relation between stereotypically black features in offenders and the likelihood of receiving the death sentence (Eberhardt, 2006) Aim: To investigate whether there was support for the hypothesis that black offenders with stereotypically black features were more likely to get the death sentence than white offenders. 109 Procedure: An analysis of the database of death-eligible cases in Philadelphia between 1979 and 1999. In 44 cases a Black man had killed a white victim. Black and white photographs of these 44 were shown to naïve raters (one at a time for 4 seconds each) who were asked to rate their facial features for stereotypical Black features between 1-11, 11 was very stereotypical. The 51 raters from Stanford University included 32 white, 15 Asian and four of other ethnicities. Results: It was found that the most stereotypically Black defenders were 57.5% more likely to receive the death penalty, than the less stereotypical defendants at 24.4%. In a second study where the victim was also Black, no significant effect was found, suggesting Black victims are seen as less important. Conclusion: This suggests that stereotypically looking Black men are seen as some how more ‘Death Worthy’. 110 Summary: Alternatives to prison Probation offers a chance to integrate back into society if it happens as intended. Unfortunately, pressures on the system mean that insufficient time is spent with offenders pre- and post-release and so the offenders often slip back into their old patterns of behaviour. Courses of significant treatment are not usually available to short-term prisoners because there is not much time for impact. There are sometimes indications in presentencing reports that a longer sentence will mean that a prisoner will get some appropriate and effective treatment. Prisoners must consent to drug and alcohol rehabilitation orders because these come under the category of medical treatment. They are not good at coming to weekly blood tests an there is a high attrition rate on these programmes. A positive note comes from the research on restorative justice, which seems in the right cases to offer victims of crime a way to come to terms with what has happened to them and to increase an offender’s awareness of their personal responsibilities. Being black is still likely to create negative stereotypical responses from the largely white members of the jurisdiction in the USA, leading to stiffer sentences and a greater likelihood of the death sentence. 111 Theories of Treatment programmes The Theories/Studies 1. Impact of cognitive skills programmes in reducing re-conviction (Cann, 2006) 2. Investigation into whether anger-management courses work (Ireland, 2000) 3. Use of acupuncture to treat drug addiction in prisoners (Wheatley, 2007) The main approaches within this area are Individual Differences, Cognitive, Social and Physiological Psychology. This is because this section focuses upon how these factors can increase/reduce the success of treatment programmes for offenders. The recidivism rate in the UK is approximately 64%. In order to break the ‘revolving door’ phenomenon of repeat offending effective treatment and rehabilitation programmes are needed. It is clear that at present these are too few and ineffective. Unfortunately, spending money on prisoners is not seen as a priority by governments and tax payers. But, this is short-sighted; breaking the cycle of reoffending is a major step towards reducing crime. Subsection = Cognitive Skills programmes Psychologists believe that one way to stop the cycle is to break down the faulty patterns which they believe underlie criminal behaviour. Before a criminal act can occur, it must be preceded by a criminal thought. Therefore, changing the way offenders think can prevent re-offending. This idea is the rationale behind cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), in other words if you change the way a person thinks you will change the way they act. When I get out, I’ll just do one more big job Friendship (2002) reported that re-offending rates for prisoners who received CBT were 14% lower than those who had not. However, a later study by Falshaw & Friendship (2003) failed to find a significant difference. 112 A major issue is that in order for CBT to be effective it must be well delivered by competent therapists and the prisoners must have a reasonably high IQ (at least 80), good literacy skills and be highly motivated. Cognitive skills programmes were first introduced in England and Wales in 1992, when two programmes were introduced: Reasoning and Rehabilitation (R & R) – the aim was to develop prisoners’ cognitive skills, targeting thinking style; self control or impulsivity; interpersonal problemsolving; critical reasoning; moral reasoning; social perspective training. The programme was made up of 36 sessions each of 2.5hrs, with assignments for prisoners to do between the sessions and after the programme was complete. This was designed in North America for use with prisoners at high risk of reoffending. Those on the programme had served sentences of varying lengths, from a few months to over 4 years, and had to have basic literacy skills and an IQ of at least 80 to qualify for inclusion on the programme. To study the effectiveness of the programme prisoners were classified into 4 different risk of reoffending categories: low risk, moderate low risk, moderate high risk and high risk. They were also categorised in terms of the reason for their sentence: violent, sexual, acquisitive or “other”. After a Home office review R & R was discontinued in England and Wales in 2004. Enhanced Thinking Skills (ETS) - ETS is a relatively short programme (20 sessions), similar to R&R, which addresses thinking and behaviour associated with offending. This includes impulse control, flexible thinking, social perspective taking, values/moral reasoning, reasoning, and inter-personal problem solving. It was designed for the prison population in England and Wales who are at medium to high risk of reoffending and is currently the programme most frequently delivered, with over 40,000 offenders having completed this course within HMPS over the past 12 years Do Cognitive Skills programmes work? In terms of the effectiveness of cognitive skills programmes, the two major studies of their effectiveness came up with conflicting results. The first of these, by Caroline Friendship and colleagues, was published in 2003 and reviewed a sample of 600+ male offenders comparing those who had been on R&R or ETS programmes between 1992 and 1996 with matched controls who had not. They had found that in all risk of offending categories those in the cognitive skills programme were less likely to reoffend, with an overall reduction in recidivism rates of 14%. This first major study suggested that cognitive skills programmes offered a promising method of rehabilitation of offenders. However, in a second major study, carried out on a similar scale by Louise Fanshaw, Caroline Friendship, Rosie Travers and Francis Nugent in 2003, there was found to be no significant difference in reoffending rates in a sample of 600+ male offenders on ETS or R&R from 1996-98 and matched controls, contradicting the earlier study. Fanshaw et al, however, suggest that their failure to find significant results might be 113 explained by other factors, such as the expansion and possible compromising of quality of the programmes (they note that in 1995-6, 30 prisons supported cognitive skills programmes and 746 completers were reported, whereas in 1998-9 70 institutions offered the programmes with 2837 completions). They also suggest that higher levels of motivation in both those delivering the programme in the period reported by the earlier study and also those receiving the programmes could explain in part why the second study failed to find a significant reduction in reoffending after cognitive behavioural therapy. The studies illustrate the methodological difficulties inherent in evaluating the effects of intervention or treatment programmes in general and don’t mean, Fanshaw et al argue, that cognitive skills programmes don’t work. The challenge, they conclude, is for the government to work out not whether the programmes work in general, but specifically to carry out investigations on what works with whom. Whilst there was a plan to carry out research with specific types of offender, the first major study of the effect of cognitive skills programmes on reoffending with female offenders was not carried out until 2006. This study was carried out by Jenny Cann and is described below. Impact of cognitive skills programmes in reducing re-conviction (Cann, 2006) Aim: To find out if cognitive skills programmes were effective in terms of lower reoffending rates for a sample of women prisoners. Procedure: Between 1996 & 2000, 180 female offenders were given one of two types of CBT: Enhanced Thinking Skills (ETS) or Reasoning and Rehabilitation (R & R). These were compared with a group of 540 female prisoners who did not receive therapy. Reconviction rates were examined two years after release. Results: No significant differences were discovered between the groups. Although the R & R group did slightly worse and were re-convicted earlier! Conclusion: Cann suggests that the programmes were ineffective for the following reasons: Women offend for different reasons to men; they may have cognitive skills deficits. Drug abuse, relationship problems, emotional factors and financial hardship are major causes of female criminality. The Cognitive therapies had been developed with male prisoners in mind and were therefore inappropriate for women. The therapy was not effectively delivered. 114 Interventions designed specifically for female prisoners: CARE In 2001, the Home Office reviewed the government’s strategy for women offenders and out of this came the intervention for female prisoners known as CARE: Choices, Actions, Relationships and Emotions. This is a programme designed for female offenders classed as having a medium to high risk of reconviction for violent and/or substance abuse related offending. This is s multi-disciplinary programme involving mental health workers, substance misuse workers for prisoners as well as non-statutory agencies in mentoring and advocacy, The aim is to target the areas of emotional well-being, relationships, substance misuse, and lifestyle and associates. These are areas that research has identified as being important factors in female offending and reoffending. The course aims to help participants identify and label emotions and develop skills for managing emotion. In addition, the course aims to foster a positive self-identity that will enable participants to live the kind of life they would like to on release. The CARE programme was piloted in two public sector prisons and an accreditation submission will be made to Correctional Services Accreditation Panel, (CSAP), in summer 2009. Subsection = Anger Management Anger is a strong emotion and many prisoners have problems controlling their anger which leads to violent behaviour. Anger needs to be controlled within a prison environment for the safety of staff and inmates. One way of achieving this aim is through anger management programmes. Anger management is a therapeutic programme used with a range of clients from convicted violent criminals to aggressive school children. It assumes that violence is caused by anger, and that if violent individuals learn to control their anger, their violent behaviour will decrease. The programme differentiates between two types of anger, instrumental aggression where the aggression is planned and is used to get something, and hostile aggression which is uncontrolled, unplanned aggression in response to a situation or person, for example road rage. Instrumental aggression is by definition aggression that is already controlled so this is not the target of anger management course. Instead the courses target hostile aggression and aim to help offenders gain control over their aggressive outbursts. Anger management programmes are based largely on cognitive-behavioural therapy techniques, and are usually delivered through group sessions. A specific anger management programme called the National Anger Management Package was devised for use in prisons in the 1980s, becoming widely used in the 1990s. The initial need for anger management in prisons arose from the rising levels of aggression and violence 115 within the prison setting itself, so the main focus of the National Anger Management Package was to try to reduce the amount of violence within prisons, but there was also with the hope that improved self-control would continue once offenders are released and lessen their chances of reoffending. Aims of the National Anger Management Package course for prisoners Course members should: - Increase their understanding of how and why they become angry - Realise the importance of monitoring their own behaviour so that they notice the first signs of becoming angry - Develop strategies for controlling their anger - Use role play to practise coping with provocation - Consider how their lives will improve if they learn to control their anger and aggression Typically such a course will involve around 8 two-hour sessions, spread over a couple of weeks. As well as helping the offenders to recognise and manage their responses to provocation they are also encouraged to consider avoiding situations where they may experience such provocation, such as night clubs or football matches. This lifestyle change is difficult to achieve but some offenders manage this through a determination to avoid situations where they realise they can’t control their behaviour and have a strong will not to reoffend. The main form of Anger management in prisons is a programme called CALM. CALM CALM is a Cognitive Behavioural Programme aimed at prisoners who have problems in managing their emotions and for whom this had been an important part of their offending behaviour. The programme encompasses six modules: 1. Primarily focused on enhancing a prisoner's motivation to participate in the programme and to change their behaviour. 2. Group members are introduced to the concept of physiological arousal and the relationship between arousal and performance. They are taught how to identify physiological changes that they experience when becoming angry and are provided with a range of arousal management techniques which they practice and receive feedback on from peers and facilitators. 3. Facilitators work with participants to understand how their irrational, hurtful and unhelpful thoughts contribute to their feelings and actions (anger and aggression). Participants learn to recognise irrational thinking, to argue against it and replace it with a more rational way of thinking. This in turn leads to more helpful and less disturbing feelings and behaviour. 4. Participants learn a number of skills to enable them to more effectively communicate with others, for example, in response to provocation from others. They also learn how to communicate and take responsibility for how they feel about a situation without blaming others and remaining in control of their emotions. 116 5. Provides a framework for applying the skills learned so far to other emotions which can lead to problematic or offending behaviour. These include jealousy, depression, anxiety (social threats and fear of losing control), and ‘superman feelings' (feelings of unlimited power). 6. Requires participants to identify the situations which are likely to most high risk for them in the future in terms of a return to anger and aggression. Facilitators then support participants in developing relapse prevention plans which incorporate skills learned on the programme and other skills as appropriate to the situation. Included in this module is preparation for what happens if they do relapse and exploration of ‘healthy' and ‘unhealthy' ways of coping following a relapse. CALM is currently offered in 24 establishments. Approximately 500 prisoners completed the programme in 2006/7. Investigation of whether anger-management courses work (Ireland,2000) Aim: To assess whether anger management programmes work with young male offenders. Procedure: A natural experiment compared a group of 50 prisoners who had completed CALM and a group of 37 who were assessed as suitable, but had not actually taken the course. The prisoners were given a cognitive behavioural interview. Prison officers completed a Wing Behavioural Checklist (WBC) rating 29 angry behaviour with scores of 0, 1 or 2 for the week before the interview. Prisoners also completed a self-report questionnaire on anger management with 53 questions. Results: 92% of prisoners belonging to the experimental group showed improvement on at least one of the measures. (48% of these showed improvement on both the AMA and the WBC, 35% showed improvement solely on the AMA, and 17% showed improvement solely on the WBC. 8% of all prisoners showed no improvement or a deterioration on both measures following course completion. Prisoners who improved on both measures appeared to contain the highest proportion of violent offenders, with those showing no improvement or a deterioration containing the smallest. Conclusions: Anger management was found to be successful in the short term for youth offenders within a prison setting. Ireland points out that o conclusions can be drawn about the long term effects of this programme as no reoffending data was gathered, but suggests that long-term evaluation of programmes should be included in future research, provided that the aims of the programme delivered were to extend the effectiveness 117 beyond the prison setting and have the effect of reducing reoffending, which was not the specific aim of the programme used in this study. Subsection = Using Ear Acupuncture With A Drug Rehabilitation Program Among the offender behaviour programmes delivered in HMPS are those specifically aimed at drug rehabilitation, helping offenders to give up substance abuse. These use a variety of techniques. One such programme, FOCUS, is a high-intensity drug and alcohol treatment programme operating in five of our high security prisons. The FOCUS programme looks to enhance motivation, change thinking associated with drug misuse, improve social and emotion management skills and develop relapse prevention skills using role-play. So far no big surprises, but what you may be surprised to know is that in these high security prisons, most of the prisoners who go through the programme also use ear acupuncture. This alternative treatment has been used in prisons for 5 years and is popular as it is cheap, easily taught (prison officers can be taught in a 4-day course and two officers can administer the treatment to 10-15 inmates at a time in a 40 minute session). Its proponents, including Michael Wheatley, (Senior Manager for the Drug Strategy & Services Directorate of High Security Prisons in England and Wales), argue that it is a useful addition to the drug rehabilitation programme and does not require the prisoner to be highly motivated to participate. But does it work? Is the FOCUS programme more effective where auricular (ear) acupuncture is administered as part of the programme? Wheatley presented the following paper in 2005 to argue that this is indeed the case. Use of acupuncture to treat drug addiction in prisoners (Wheatley, 2007) Aim: To evaluate the effectiveness of ear acupuncture. Procedure: 350 prisoners in six high security prisons received acupuncture and the standard care programme FOCUS; these were compared with a control group who just received FOCUS. Two trained practitioners worked with groups of 10-15 prisoners in a relaxed setting, needles were inserted into five acupuncture points in the ear and prisoners relaxed for 40 minutes. 118 Results: Qualitative Data: Prisoners reported better sleep, improved relaxation, better able to cope, reduced nicotine cravings, cognitive and health improvements. Staff reported better communication with staff and families, improved attendance at classes, calmer atmosphere and less use of healthcare facilities. Quantitative Data: 70% reduction in drug related incidents in the 6 months after treatment. 41% reduction in serious incident reports. 42% reduction in positive drug tests (mandatory). 33% reduction in positive drug test (voluntary) Conclusion: Wheatley believes that there is enough evidence to expand the delivery of acupuncture throughout the prison system. He believes it works best as a complimentary therapy alongside other programmes. What makes this a psychological study? Well, if you take the view that ear acupuncture “does something” biologically to the patient, then this is a biomedical study not a psychological one. However, what lends this study a psychological angle is that the effects of ear acupuncture as reported by Wheatley could not be replicated by Gates et al in 2006. Reviewing a total of seven controlled studies of acupuncture that compared “real” with “sham” acupuncture (needles stuck anywhere rather then the five “official” points for drug users) Gates et al found no difference in outcome between the “real” and “sham” acupuncture participants. With such evidence disputing the effect of ear acupuncture Wheatley’s study lacks reliability. This and other factors give us reason to question Wheatley’s confidence in the positive effect of including ear acupuncture in drug rehabilitation programmes for prisoners. There are other challenges to the validity of his work: If there is no effect of ear acupuncture on drug users, what can explain the positive outcomes that Wheatley reports in his study? What other variables might account for the positive change in the prisoners’ attitudes and behaviours? Is there an expectancy effect? Can we use the placebo effect to explain the “success” of ear acupuncture in Wheatley’s study? 119 120 Summary: Treatment programmes Treatment programmes are a real problem to assess. It seems that when a programme is well conducted, completed in full and properly targeted on the right offender, it works, but when extended to larger groups and often incomplete because of the overcrowding in the system, the positive effect disappears. However, both cognitive skills programmes and anger-management programmes rely on self-reports and observation to evaluate their effectiveness. This means that a prisoner could show high demand characteristics and present a favourable impression of their effectiveness to secure an early release. Psychologists and prison staff are willing to try alternative therapies such as ear acupuncture with drug offenders. This is itself is not psychological unless one views it as a placebo, but in practice it is used with so-called “talking cures” or more standard therapeutic approaches. In the end, all concerned look for “whatever works” and put into practice whatever they can. Unfortunately the present overcrowded system works against the prison staff and psychologists, meaning that prisoners rarely get to finish programmes or that sufficient time and expertise are put into them. 121 AFTER A GUILTY VERDICT January 2010 1. (a) Identify the link between imprisonment and suicide. (10). (b) Evaluate the usefulness of research into the psychological effects of imprisonment. June 2010 2. (a) Describe anger management as a treatment programme for offenders. (10) (b) Assess the effectiveness of offender treatment programmes. (15) June 2011 3. (a) Describe one piece of research into ‘looking death worthy.’ (10) (b) To what extent can research into alternatives to imprisonment be considered ethnocentric. (15) January 2012 4. (a) How has ear acupuncture been used as a treatment programme. (10) (b) Compare the strengths of different offender treatment programmes. (15) June 2012 5. (a) How can probation serve as an alternative to imprisonment? (10) (b) Evaluate the limitations of research into alternatives to imprisonment. (15) January 2013 6. (a) Describe research into planned behaviours once freed from jail. (10) (b) Evaluate the use of qualitative and quantitative data when researching imprisonment. (15) June 2013 1. (a) Describe research into the prison situation and roles. (10) (b) Evaluate the methodology used in research into imprisonment. (15) (a) How is restorative justice used after a guilty verdict? (10) (b) To what extent are alternatives to imprisonment effective? (15) June 2014 1. (a) Describe a cognitive skills programme used with offenders. (10) (b) Assess the usefulness of offender treatment programmes. (15) 2. (a) Outline research into ‘Looking Deathworthy.’ (10) (b) Assess the validity of research into alternatives to imprisonment. (15) G543 Checklist: Indicate Yes or No to show level of confidence 1 – Turning to Crime Yes/No Study Yes/No Social Explanations 1.1 Disrupted Families Farrington 1.2 Learning from Others Sutherland/Akers (EXT) 1.3 Poverty Wilkstrom (Peterborough) Cognitive Explanations 2.1 Criminal Thinking Patterns 2.2 Moral Development Yochelson & Samenow Kohlberg/Chen and Howitt(EXT) Gudjohnsson 2.3 Social Cognition Biological Explanations 3.1 Brain Dysfunction Raine/Raine 3.2 Genes & Serotonin Brunner/Caspi (EXT) 3.3 Gender Daly & Wilson 2 - Making a Case Yes/No Study Interviewing Witnesses 1.1 Recognising Faces Bruce 1.2 Factors Influencing Identification 1.3 Cognitive Interview Loftus (weapon focus)/Pickel (EXT) Fisher Interviewing Suspects 2.1 Detecting Lies Mann 2.2 Interrogation Techniques 2.3 False Confessions Inbau/Granhag (EXT) Gudjohnsson Creating a Profile 3.1 Top Down 3.2 Bottom up Theory (Ressler and Hazlewood) Study Canter Canter 3.3 Case Study Canter: John Duffy Yes/No 3 – Reaching a Verdict Yes/No Study Yes/No Persuading a Jury 1.1 Effect of Order of Testimony 1.2 Persuasion Pennington & Hastie Loftus 1.3 Effect of Evidence Ruled Inadmissible Pickel Witness Appeal 2.1 Attractiveness of Defendant 2.2 Witness Confidence Castellow/ Theory Dion Penrod 2.3 Effects of Shields and Video on Children giving Evidence Reaching a Verdict Ross 3.1 Stages in Decision Making Hastie 3.2 Majority Influence Asch 3.3 Minority Influence Nemeth/Mosco vici(EXT) 124 4 – After a Guilty Verdict Yes/No Study Yes/No Imprisonment 1.1 Planned Behaviours when free from Jail 1.2 Depression & Suicide Risk Gillis & Nafekh 1.3 Prison Situation and Roles Zimbardo Palmer and Connelly Alternatives to Imprisonment 2.1 Probation Mair & May 2.2 Restorative Justice Sherman & Strang 2.3 Looking ‘Deathworthy’ Eberhardt Treatment Programmes 3.1 Cognitive Skills Programme Cann 3.2 Anger Management Ireland 3.3 Ear Acupuncture Wheatley/Margolin (EXT) 125