1 Running Head: Understanding Schemas Understanding Schemas: “Their Role in Attachment Insecurities, Disordered Eating, Marketing, Age-Related Reliance and Perception of Others” Erica Muse Georgia Southern University EDUF 7130 2 Understanding Schemas Abstract Schemas are pervasive in society and whether constructed accurately or inaccurately, they impact our viewpoints and actions. In order to gain a greater understanding of their applicability, this paper is an attempt to represent the role of schemas in several contexts through assessing published journal articles. Through related research and readings, I was able to gain knowledge and develop a framework for the role that schemas play in thoughts and decision making and find ways to apply the information to my daily life. 3 Understanding Schemas In choosing a topic to research for this paper, I reviewed the questions missed on the midterm exam and realized that I had misunderstood schemas and scripts. The purpose of this paper was to reframe my understanding by gaining knowledge about the role schemas play in our daily lives. The question on the exam was: “Schemas and scripts are best described by which of the following statements…” What I learned from my answer was that they are not “personal theories used to explain external phenomena”, but rather “a closely connected set of ideas related to a specific object or event”. Schemas encompass more than external phenomena and cannot be placed neatly into a box of understanding for they are pervasive in our conscious and subconscious and impact our daily thoughts and actions. Using the Georgia Southern University online library, I was able to obtain sources by completing a search within journals with the search term “schema”. Many articles were related to the topic, but after reviewing several I realized that I would like to choose five that approached schemas from different viewpoints and areas of research. The papers that were chosen reflect the concept through research completed on marketing, personality, attachment insecurities, disordered eating and age-related reliance. In the research project by Deas, Power, Collin, Yellowlees & Grierson (n.d), the goal of the authors was to better understand the role that perfectionist schemas and parental bonding play as related to Anorexia Nervosa. Using three groups, the hypotheses were threefold: perceptions of the parental bond would be more negative and perfectionist schemas would be more prevalent among the female Anorexia Nervosa group than two comparison groups --women diagnosed with depression and/or anxiety and women with no known diagnosis. Additionally, the authors speculated that perfectionist schema would act as a mediating factor between what they define as “negative parenting” and disordered eating. The 40 participants for the Anorexia Nervosa group were chosen from those receiving inpatient treatments at The Priory 4 Understanding Schemas Hospital, Glasgow and from an outpatient treatment facility, NHS Tayside’s Psychological Therapies Service (p.417). The 44 women chosen for the depression/anxiety group were also chosen from the NHS Tayside’s Psychological Therapies Service and the 78 women chosen for the “healthy” group were undergraduates pursuing psychology degrees from the University of Stirling. The quantitative, cross-sectional study examined disordered eating (dependent variable), perceived parenting (independent variable) and perfectionistic schemas (independent variable/mediator) (p.418). All participants were given the Eating Disorders Examination (EDE), The Parental Bonding Instrument-Short Form (PBI-S) and The Young Schema QuestionnaireShort Form (YSQ-S). The results showed that the women in the Anorexia Nervosa group scored significantly higher on the EDE, reported much less parental care/greater parental control and had scores that were significant on The Young Schema Questionnaire (relating specifically to perfectionism) when compared to the comparison groups. The authors found that more research would be needed with a larger group of Anorexia Nervosa participants before they could determine if perfectionism was a mediating factor. They did, however, conclude that women diagnosed with Anorexia Nervosa hold more negative views of their parents than the other comparison groups and more maladaptive schema. The author’s note that: “…the construct of negative perfectionism should not be overlooked when assessing, formulating and treating those with AN (p. 422).” Miesler, Leder & Herrmann (2011) focused their study on “baby schema” (focusing on baby faced shapes) as related to marketing and affective responses. The authors research was two-fold: “… to study whether innate affective responses to physical features of the baby schema were generalized to product designs (p.17)”, and to use facial electromyography (EMG) to measure affect non-verbally (p.18). The authors hypothesized “…that detecting facial features in 5 Understanding Schemas product designs rapidly triggers the adequate affective responses in consumers (p.19)”. Using Adobe Photoshop, researcher’s manipulated 16 car fronts (enlarging the headlights by 20%) and faces (eyes, nose and mouth) to reflect “baby schema” and participants were asked to evaluate perceived cuteness when given the original versus the adapted as choices. Thirty-five students participated in a pre-test with 19 rating pictures of cars and 16 rating pictures of faces on a Likert 7-point scale of cuteness (p.20). For the pre-test, there was a significant difference in ratings of cuteness between photos that were altered and originals; with those altered as baby faced perceived as cuter. The main study included data from undergraduate students: 28 car group participants and 21 face group participants. During the computerized rating, the EMG measured non-verbal affect by assessing two major facial muscles, the zygomaticus major and the corrugator supercilii (p.22). The results showed the following: the baby faced car fronts and faces were perceived as cuter than the original and the EMG showed that facial muscle activity was greater for both baby faced car fronts and faces when compared to the original. The authors make the suggestion for designers “…to increase the affective value of products by creating cute designs which can benefit from the human predisposition to feel attracted by baby-schema cues (p. 27)”. In their 2009 study, Ebner, Riediger & Lindenberger sought to show expectations of gains in childhood and early adulthood and losses in middle age and late adulthood. The authors also wanted to explore whether older adults “ rely on the schema when remembering ageassociated information (p.311)”. The first experiment consisted of individuals chosen from the Max Planck Institute for Human Development for a total of 48 participants, 24 younger (20-29 years) and 24 older (71-85 years). Each age group participated in one testing session which included: signing a consent form, completing the Digit Symbol Substitution Test and a socio- 6 Understanding Schemas demographic questionnaire, and finally rating 160 faces taken from the Productive Aging Laboratory Face Database in terms of attractiveness, likeability, distinctiveness, goal orientation, energy, mood and age (p.312). The results showed that both age groups classified faces using a life-span schema and gave younger faces more growth goals and older faces goals of lossprevention. Both age groups were shown to ascribe to the same/similar schema related to agegoal orientation. For the second part of the experiment, the goal was to examine both groups to see if there were differences in how much they rely on life-span schema using two testing sessions per group which were one week apart. There were 59 young participants (19-31 years) and 60 older participants (69-79 years); all participants were recruited through newspaper ads. Young and old participants were again tested in separate sessions which included: signing a consent form, generating three important personal goals, gaining instructions on how to use the computer programs, and then testing using the Spot-a-Word and Digit Symbol Substitution Test for the first session (p.314). The second session included the computer tasks and also a Paired-Associate Learning task. The researchers found that both young and older participant associated loss prevention with older adults and growth with younger. Additionally, between the first and second session, young participants recognized more faces and rejected more faces correctly than did older participants (p.316). The author’s discuss age related changes and their role in false memories and/or biases as well as episodic memory deficiencies which can cause decreases in older adult’s ability to create/ retrieve associations (p.320). Ein-Dor, Mikulincer & Shaver (2011) researched schema formed during interactions with unavailable/unresponsive attachment figures. The authors hypothesized that such schema influences the coping strategies employed by insecure people (p.78). The authors speculated that 7 Understanding Schemas insecure people experience sentinel schema: they remain vigilant to possible threats, react quickly to unclear situations, alert others to danger, seek to gain support of others and to be in close proximity of others when a threat is presented. They sought to find results supporting their hypothesis across six studies (p.79). For the first study, 80 Israeli undergraduates were participants across two sessions. In the first session, participants completed psychological assessments including the Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) scale which assessed feelings about close relationships on a 1-7 scale. Participants also completed The Big Five Inventory about their personality on a scale of 1-5 and the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale as well as a verbal measure using a Scholastic Aptitude Test. In the second session, participants viewed the photo of a threatening situation (a wolf) and were asked to write a story about what was happening currently and what they personally thought would happen next (p.82). The researchers found the results to support their predictions in that those participants that scored high on the measures of attachment anxiety also were more like to generate stories that were congruent with the sentinel schema. Avoidant participants were likely to generate stories that were consistent with what researchers deemed “fight-flight” schema (p.83). For the second study, 69 Israeli undergraduate students participated across two separate sessions. The first session consisted of participants taking the ECR. The second part consisted of being shown a video and told that they would be asked to remember the information presented. The video differed between randomly chosen participants and for some included answers that showed a person sensitive to danger while the other participants saw a video where the person interviewed was calm. Then participants had to identify via computer which sentences appeared in the video. Again, the findings were aligned with the author’s predictions; attachment was associated with threat related sentences being chosen and schema-biased memories (p.84). In the 8 Understanding Schemas third study, 57 Israeli undergraduates participated across two sessions. In the first session, they completed the ECR. The second session was similar to the second study, however, for this study the video varied and the responses of the person interviewed either showed that she “acts alone and without hesitation in times of danger” or in the control, that she “cooperates with others and waits to see what they are doing” (p. 85). The results of this study showed that avoidant attachment was associated with more false memories and faster information recognition congruent with fight-flight schema (p. 86). For the fourth study, there were 84 Israeli undergraduate participants that took part in one session that included: completing the ECR scale (anytime this test was given during the study the scores showed high levels of anxiety and avoidance), completing an unrelated personality test, reading a story about a 23-year old person in unfamiliar territory (the woods) and completing a memory task. The author’s hypotheses were supported; attachment anxiety led to more inferences related to sentinel schema. For the fifth session, 72 Israeli undergraduates participated in one session which included: completion of the ECR scale, completing an unrelated personality test, reading a story similar to the one presented in the fourth study followed by a memory task. The results showed that “avoidant attachment was associated with generating more inferences about the rapid fight-flight schema (p.88)”. The final study focused on whether behavior arose from the schemas and included 138 Israeli undergraduate participants across two sessions. In the first session, participants completed two scales and in the second session which was held two weeks later, they were videotaped with hidden cameras after being given an unrelated questionnaire to complete unsupervised. When the experimenter left the room, it began to fill with smoke as though there was a fire. The experiment ended when people left the room or tried to put out the mock fire; all participants believed it was real. The results for this study showed 9 Understanding Schemas that attachment anxiety was directly linked with quick detection---and ultimately, that the schema did lead to specific behavior (p.89). In the final research study that I read concerning schemas, in a three part study Kammrath (2011) sought to explore the relationship between personality and behavior schema. To do this, two domains for interpersonal interactions were considered; one being communion domain and the other agency domain. Communion domain refers to people’s preference in their relationships concerning warm (examples: kindness and friendliness) and cold (examples: distance in relationships and intimacy avoidance). Agency domain refers to people’s preference concerning assertive and unassertive behavior. Kammrath hypothesized that there would be associations between the domains and behavior schemas. The research was conducted across three studies; in the first there were 188 undergraduate students and the assessment consisted of an online background survey followed by two scenarios. One scenario was romantic and the other was academic however, both were related to decision-making. Imagining themselves as the protagonist, individuals read the scenarios(the orders varied depending on the participant) and wrote scripts based around what they felt the protagonist might say to the other person in the scenario if they were responding in four ways: assertively, submissively, warmly or coldly (p.756). After writing, participants answered questions related to their expectations about that response. The results of the first study confirmed the hypothesis that trait (agency) and communion domains were associated with expectations for trait-relevant behavior (p.758). For the second study, there were 169 undergraduate students that completed background questionnaires online and read the same scenarios. However, after reading them participants were asked to read different ways the protagonist could respond. These were unlabeled but corresponded to the same four as presented in the prior study. Again, participants were asked to 10 Understanding Schemas relate their expectations about the response but also answered questions that were related how they would feel if they were the person the protagonist was addressing and indicated how they would respond. The results of this study also showed that “trait communion moderated the extremity of people’s behavior schemas for warm and cold behavior and the trait agency moderated people’s behavior for assertive and submissive behavior (p.762)”. Study three focused on 78 MBA student’s that were asked to do the following: for six class meetings they were to work in pairs and provide feedback about the negotiation process, complete online personality surveys including the Good-Me-Bad-Me Questionnaire to assess the two domains. The results showed that there were correlations between if-then profiles and behavior schemas (p.766). The author suggests that rather than asking why people behavior certain ways to first consider viewing their behavior through a different lens. After reading the articles, it was clear that schemas have much potential to impact our daily lives both on a conscious and subconscious level. When making decisions, “snap judgments or bringing information from long-term memory to working memory, it is important to understand the role that schemas play and to remember that each person creates their own views of the world and their surroundings based on personal experience. Though I admittedly have no experience with the assessments used during research or the tools used for analysis, the overarching themes have much relevance. By recognizing where I may have flawed or inaccurate schema and choosing to evaluate my thought processes through various lenses, I can work more effectively to empathetically understand individual perspectives at work, in social settings and at home. Additionally, as a consumer, it is important to understand the “science of marketing” to be an informed consumer. 11 Understanding Schemas References Deas, S., Power, K., Collin, P., Yellowlees, A., & Grierson, D. (n.d). The Relationship between Disordered Eating, Perceived Parenting, and Perfectionistic Schemas. Cognitive Therapy And Research, 35(5), 414-424. Ebner, N. C., Riediger, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2009). Schema reliance for developmental goals increases from early to late adulthood: Improvement for the young, loss prevention for the old. Psychology And Aging, 24(2), 310-323. doi:10.1037/a0015430 Ein-Dor, T., Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2011). Attachment insecurities and the processing of threat-related information: Studying the schemas involved in insecure people's coping strategies. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 101(1), 78-93. doi:10.1037/a0022503 Kammrath, L. K. (2011). What we think we do (to each other): How personality can bias behavior schemas through the projection of if–then profiles. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 101(4), 754-770. doi:10.1037/a0023639 Miesler, L., Leder, H., & Herrmann, A. (2011). Isn't It Cute: An Evolutionary Perspective of Baby-Schema Effects in Visual Product Designs. International Journal Of Design, 5(3), 17-30.