12640143_Nelson & Jackson_R1

advertisement
1
The role of numerical competence in a specialized predatory strategy of an araneophagic
2
spider
3
4
5
Short Title: Spider numeracy
6
7
Ximena J Nelson1*
8
Robert R Jackson1,2
9
10
1
11
Zealand.
12
2
13
Mbita Point, Kenya.
School of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New
International Centre of Insect Physiology and Ecology, Thomas Odhiambo Campus, P.0. Box 30
14
15
*Correspondence: Ximena Nelson, School of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury,
16
Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealand.
17
Email: ximena.nelson@canterbury.ac.nz
18
Phone: 64-3-3642987 extn 4050
19
Fax: 64-3-3642590
20
21
22
23
1
24
25
Although a wide range of vertebrates have been considered in research on numerical competence,
26
little is known about the role of number-related decisions in the predatory strategies of
27
invertebrates. Here we investigate how numerical competence is expressed in a highly specialized
28
predatory strategy adopted by the small juveniles of Portia africana when practicing communal
29
predation, with the prey being another spider, Oecobius amboseli. Two or more P. africana
30
juveniles sometimes settle by the same oecobiid nest and then share the meal after one individual
31
captures the oecobiid. Experiments were designed to clarify how these predators use number-related
32
cues in conjunction with non-numerical cues when deciding whether to settle at a nest. We used
33
lures (dead spiders positioned in lifelike posture) arranged in a series of 24 different scenes defined
34
by the type, configuration and especially number of lures. On the whole, our findings suggest that
35
P. africana juveniles base settling decisions on the specific number of already settled conspecific
36
juveniles at the nest and express a preference for settling when the number is one instead of zero,
37
two or three. By varying the size of the already settled juveniles and their positions around the nest,
38
we show that factors related to continuous variables and stimulus configuration are unlikely
39
explanations for our findings.
40
41
Keywords: numerosity discrimination, numerical ability, prototype matching, communal predation,
42
araneophagy, Salticidae
43
2
44
Introduction
45
Understanding the nature and origin of numerical competence is a primary objective in human and
46
animal cognitive science (Carey 1998, 2001; Giaquinto 2001; Feigenson et al. 2004; Shettleworth
47
2009). Primates have been prominent in the literature on animal numerical competence, this trend
48
being consistent with a tradition of associating numerical competence with advanced human
49
language-based cognition (Dehaene 2001; Haun et al. 2010). Yet the once widely accepted view
50
that numerical competence is uniquely human and fundamentally dependent on verbal language has
51
now been supplanted by a rapidly expanding literature (Wesley 1961; Davis and Memmot 1982;
52
Brannon 2002; Cantlon and Brannon 2007; Slaughter et al. 2011) on the numerical competence of
53
pre-verbal human infants (hereafter, simply ‘infants’) and on non-human animals (hereafter, simply
54
‘animals’), including a wide range of vertebrate species as well as examples from insects (Chittka
55
and Geiger 1995; Dacke and Srinivason 2008; Gross et al. 2009; Rezinikova and Ryabko 2011).
56
Although research on infants has been based on spontaneous response to number-related
57
cues (Wynn 1992, 1998), much of the early animal work depended on extensive training (e.g.,
58
Matsuzawa 1985; Boysen and Bernston 1995; Pepperberg 2006) and this encouraged an impression
59
that, for animals, numerical competence is a hard-earned ability that is adopted only as a last resort
60
(Breukelaar and Dalrymple-Alford 1998; Seron and Pesenti 2001). However, there has been a shift
61
toward considering spontaneous expression of numerical competence by animals (Uller et al. 2001;
62
Flombaum et al. 2005; Hanus and Call 2007; Wood et al. 2007; Agrillo et al. 2008; Dadda et al.
63
2009) and this has, in turn, increased interest in understanding how numerical competence functions
64
for particular animals in their natural environments (Hager and Helfman 1991; McComb et al.
65
1994; Lyon 2003; Kitchen 2006; Hunt et al. 2008; Benson-Amram et al. 2011).
66
In the context of animal-centred research on the expression of numerical competence,
67
optimal foraging theory (OFT) has an interesting history. OFT began in a way that could be
68
described as decidedly uninterested in cognition (Pyke et al. 1977; Stephens and Krebs 1986). For
69
example, terms such as ‘strategy’ have always been prevalent in the OFT literature, but often with
3
70
disclaimers to the effect that nothing cognitive was intended. Yet, from the beginning, OFT was
71
relevant to hypotheses about cognitive decision-making and especially numerical cognition
72
(Shettleworth 2009). In particular, predictions from OFT (Charnov 1976) often imply considerable
73
capacity on the part of the animal to make relative number and relative quantity judgments (see
74
Church and Broadbent 1990; Boysen and Bernston 1995; Uller et al. 2003; Brannon 2006; Castelli
75
et al. 2006). Animals may sometimes express a go-for-less strategy (e.g., Hoare et al. 2004).
76
However, there are also many examples of a go-for-more strategy (e.g., Boysen and Bernston 1995;
77
Beran 2008; Beran et al. 2008). These results imply that optimal foraging includes judgments of
78
relative number (see Shettleworth 2009).
79
Less is known about animals adopting strategies in which specific numbers are innately
80
salient and responded to spontaneously (see Tardif et al. 2002). Yet, the distinction between relative
81
number judgment and choosing a specific number becomes important when specifying categories of
82
numerical competence (Gelman and Gallistel 1978; Dehaene 1992; Gallistel and Gelman 2000).
83
Kaufman et al. (1949), for example, distinguished between counting, subitizing and estimating.
84
True counting is an iterative process based on assigning a distinct tag to each item, with tags being
85
symbols, tokens or physical referents. Counting achieves more than simply detecting a difference
86
between two numbers, as it implies that the individual doing the counting has access to
87
representations of ordinal and cardinal numbers (Gelman and Gallistel 1978), a phenomenon which
88
has been demonstrated especially clearly in primates after varying degrees of training with Arabic
89
numerals (Boysen and Bernston 1989, 1995).
90
Subitizing is a mechanism by which an individual can, without having access to a
91
representation of a specific number, nonetheless make decisions corresponding to specific numbers
92
(Trick and Pylyshyn 1994; Railo et al. 2008), with the maximum typically being 3-4 (Cowan 1998,
93
2000, 2010). The mechanisms underlying subitizing appear to be related to individuating objects
94
(Trick 2005) and assigning them to a limited number of object files (Kahneman et al. 1992) in
95
working memory (Baddeley 1986, 2003; Owen 2004; Awh et al. 2007; Anderson et al. 2011;
4
96
Fougnie and Marois 2011). The individual is assumed to form a representation of occupied object
97
files, but does not necessarily use these object files as representations of numbers (Trick and
98
Pylyshyn 1994; Xu and Spelke 2000; Hauser and Carey 2003).
99
When ‘estimating’, an individual might represent an analogue magnitude, or a characteristic
100
called the ‘numerousness’ of a set, or the individual might derive a relative number judgment by
101
making comparisons (Kobayashi et al. 2004; McCrink and Wynn 2004). This system, known as the
102
‘accumulator model’ or ‘analogue-magnitude system’, is used for estimating how different large
103
numbers compare to each other (Meck and Church 1983; Gallistel and Gelman 2000) and does not
104
require that the individual have access to either a representation of a specific number or to any
105
particular information pertaining to a specific number (Le Corre and Carey 2008). This model is
106
based on the premise that memory is an inherently noisy medium. According to the accumulator
107
model, when non-verbal representations of numerosity are coded into memory, variability increases
108
in proportion to the square root of the number targeted for memory (Weber’s Law), with this
109
accounting for the seeming discontinuity (≤ 4) in the capacity of animals to perceive number
110
directly. However, the accumulator model does not distinguish between the ability to count and
111
summation of variables that covary with the counted objects, such as volume or area (Beran 2001).
112
Although there has been research on numerical competence in auditory (Starkey et al. 1983),
113
tactile (Davis et al. 1989; Riggs et al. 2006) and chemical (Carazo et al. 2009; Thomas and
114
Simmons 2010) channels, most of the literature on numerical competence pertains to vision
115
(Dehaene 1992; Burr and Ross 2008; Brady et al. 2011). It is striking that the literature on animal
116
numerical competence does not include salticid spiders, as these small predators have intricate
117
vision-based behaviour and the capacity to classify prey by sight into multiple categories (Nelson
118
and Jackson 2011; Harland and Jackson 2001). Salticids belonging to the genus Portia are of
119
particular interest because they have often been subjects in research related to animal cognition
120
(Jackson and Cross 2011; Jakob et al. 2011).
5
121
Here we consider the role of number-related cues in an especially intricate predatory
122
strategy adopted by the small juveniles (body length 1.5-3.0 mm) of P. africana in Kenya (hereafter
123
‘Portia’) when preying on Oecobius amboseli (hereafter ‘oecobiid’), a small spider species (adult
124
body length, 2-3 mm) that builds tent-like silk nests on boulders, tree trunks and the walls of
125
buildings (Jackson et al. 2008).
126
A typical oecobiid nest is sparsely woven and only about 5 mm in diameter, with the
127
resident oecobiid inside usually remaining visible, although less distinct than when outside. When
128
disturbed by a potential predator, the oecobiid runs away from its nest in a straight line and then
129
suddenly freezes, but eventually walks back and re-enters the nest. Portia juveniles exploit this
130
behaviour by first settling near to the nest and eventually, with its palps and forelegs, initiating
131
intermittent probing and striking of the nest silk. Portia may capture the oecobiid as it comes out of
132
the nest in an attempt to flee. Alternatively, the oecobiid may escape, but Portia waits at the nest
133
and captures it upon its return (Jackson et al. 2008).
134
Although most salticids are solitary predators, Portia often practises communal predation,
135
especially when the prey is an oecobiid. In typical sequences, two Portia settle alongside each other
136
at an oecobiid’s nest and when one Portia captures an oecobiid, it is joined by the other to feed
137
alongside it. For the critical decision of whether to settle at an oecobiid nest, variables known to
138
matter include whether a nest is present and occupied by an oecobiid instead of a similar-sized
139
Portia, whether another Portia is already settled near the nest and is facing toward or away from the
140
nest (Jackson and Nelson 2012).
141
That number-related cues might be salient is suggested because more than two Portia
142
feeding together or settled together alongside the same oecobiid nest are rarely seen (Jackson et al.
143
2008). Taking into consideration the non-numerical cues already known to be important (Jackson
144
and Nelson 2012), the hypothesis we consider is that Portia bases decisions on the number of
145
Portia settled at a nest inside which there is an oecobiid, with the settled Portia being in a particular
146
orientation to the nest and within a specific distance range from the nest.
6
147
As in any study designed to consider the role of number-related cues (Clearfield and Mix
148
1999; Hurewitz et al. 2006; Franks et al. 2008), we need to consider possibility that non-numerical
149
variables alone suffice to explain our findings. Here we consider three alternative hypotheses, each
150
of which specifies particular confounding variables: continuous variables such as the area in a scene
151
filled by salient already settled Portia (‘Portia-stuff hypothesis’); the amount of free space
152
remaining alongside a nest (‘limited-space hypothesis’); the configuration in which already settled
153
Portia are arranged at the nest (‘canonical-configuration hypothesis’).
154
155
Methods
156
All test subjects were F2-generation juveniles (body length, 2.5 mm) of Portia africana from
157
laboratory culture (standard rearing procedures; for details, see Jackson et al. 2008). Here we focus
158
on the details that pertain specifically to numerical cues, with only brief descriptions being provided
159
for experimental detail shared with the previous study (Jackson Nelson 2012).
160
As our goal was to investigate Portia’s innate, spontaneous decisions, we ensured that
161
neither the test spiders nor their parents had prior experience with oecobiids. By basing our
162
experiments on the reactions of test spiders (living Portia) to stationary lures (dead spiders
163
positioned in lifelike posture in ‘scenes’), we avoided confounding variables that can arise when
164
testing predators with living prey (see Nelson and Jackson 2011). We minimized the possibility of
165
residual odour cues by making lures from spiders that had been kept in ethanol. After being
166
positioned in a scene, the lures were sprayed with aerosol plastic for preservation.
167
Portia lures were made from juveniles belonging to three size categories (body length: small
168
1.5 mm, medium 2.5 mm, and large 3.0 mm). Test spiders were always medium and, unless stated
169
otherwise, Portia lures were also medium. The oecobiids used for making lures were field-collected
170
adult females (body length 2.5 mm).
171
172
In experiments, each treatment was based on presenting test spiders with a scene. For each
scene, lures were centred in a different arrangement on the flat surface of a 50 mm-wide rubber
7
173
stopper (hereafter referred to simply as the ‘disc’). Scenes were defined by: the number of lures
174
present; the type, configuration and orientation of the lures; whether Portia lures were small,
175
medium or large; whether there was a spider present in an artificial nest, and the distance between
176
lures and the nest (see Table 1). To create artificial nests (diameter 5.0 mm), silk was taken from a
177
nest built by an oecobiid that had been kept in the laboratory without prey for the previous 7 days
178
and then draped over a lure. No individual test spider, lure or nest was used more than once (N for
179
each scene was 100 or 200; see Table 1) and all tests were conducted between 0800 and 1200 hours
180
(laboratory photoperiod, 12L-12D, lights on 0700 hours).
181
For making a test chamber (for details see Jackson and Nelson 2012), the disc was first
182
inserted into a hole centred on bottom of a Petri dish so that it protruded 2 mm into the dish. The lid
183
was then put on the Petri dish, creating the test chamber with the scene enclosed. The dish was then
184
turned vertical and held in place by clamping the distal end of the disc to a retort stand (lowest point
185
of Petri dish 200 mm above bench). After a 5 min settling period within a glass tube, a test spider
186
was allowed access to the test chamber through an entry hole directly across from the centre of the
187
scene (i.e., through the ‘lid’ of the Petri dish). Tests began when the test spider walked into the test
188
chamber.
189
When a scene included one or more lures positioned outside the nest, with or without a lure
190
inside, the ‘already settled Portia’ lures outside were referred to simply as ‘Portia’, with ‘test
191
spiders’ being used to differentiate between dead (lures) and live Portia. Depending on the scene,
192
three distances of Portia lures from the nest were applicable. These were defined by distance from
193
the centre of the already settled spider’s body and the centre of the nest: close (8 mm), far (16 mm)
194
and very far (24 mm). ‘Evenly spread around a nest’ refers to scenes in which Portia were
195
positioned so as to make an equilateral triangle with the nest at the centre (e.g., Scene 8, Table 1).
196
Side by side ‘on an arc’ refers to scenes in which two or more Portia were positioned on an arc with
197
each Portia equidistant from the centre of the nest. When there were three Portia on an arc, the
198
Portia in the centre was directly below the nest, on an imaginary line drawn straight down from the
8
199
centre of the nest, one of the other two spiders was on an imaginary line running through the centre
200
of the nest 45o to the left and the other was on an imaginary line running through the nest 45o to the
201
right (e.g., Scene 9, Table 1). There was no Portia in the middle position when only two Portia
202
were present, but the left and the right individuals were positioned the same as when three were
203
present. ‘In a horizontal line’ refers to scenes similar to placement on an arc except that the Portia
204
formed an imaginary straight line alongside the nest instead of an arc centred on the nest (e.g.,
205
Scene 16, Table 1). ‘Lined up one behind the other’ refers to scenes in which two or three Portia
206
were positioned so that the first individual faced the nest, the second faced the first from the rear
207
and the third faced the second Portia, with the first Portia being close to the nest, the second being
208
far from nest and, if present, the third very far from nest (e.g., Scene 10, Table 1).
209
Tests ended and the test outcome was recorded when the test spider settled (i.e., when the
210
test spider walked on to the disc and, without first attacking a lure or a nest, became quiescent for 2
211
min), attacked (i.e., without first settling, the test spider leapt, lunged at or struck at a lure or a nest),
212
or else neither settled nor attacked within the 60 min test period. Whenever an attack was seen, the
213
target was recorded.
214
We carried out pairwise comparisons between different scenes using 2 x 2 contingency
215
tables (chi-square and Fisher exact tests (FET) of independence). Bonferroni adjustments for
216
multiple comparisons were applied and, unless stated otherwise, all results that were significant
217
remained so after adjusting alpha. In all comparisons between scenes there was no significant
218
difference for the number of attacks, so while we have left the data in Table 1, we have omitted
219
attacks from further discussion. We used Kruskal-Wallis tests to analyse the latency to settle in
220
different scenes (Dunn’s multiple comparisons used for pairwise comparisons).
221
222
Results and Discussion
223
9
224
Are test spider decisions influenced by whether the number of Portia already settled close to the
225
nest is one or two and, when two, by how the two are spaced with respect to the nest?
226
Significantly more test spiders settled when there was only one Portia close to the oecobiid-
227
occupied nest (Scene 2) instead of one at each end (Scene 3) (χ2 = 88.62, P < 0.001), two on an arc
228
at one end (Scene 4, χ2 = 48.20, P < 0.001) or two one behind the other at one end (Scene 5, χ2 =
229
41.70, P < 0.001). Instead of a go-for-more rule, test spiders appear to be adopting a go-for-one
230
rule.
231
However, when only one Portia was present (Scene 2), the available space near the nest was
232
greater than when two Portia juveniles were present equidistant from the nest (Scenes 3 & 4). This
233
suggests that our findings might be explained by the limited-space hypothesis (i.e., when deciding
234
whether to settle, test spiders attend to available space instead of the number of already settled
235
conspecific juveniles present).
236
Using the maximum possible distance at which a test spider might position itself from the
237
nearest Portia as an index of available space, the predicted trend from the limited-space hypothesis
238
is that more test spiders will settle with Scene 4 or Scene 5 than with Scene 3. Additionally, the
239
number of test spiders that will settle with Scene 5 will not be significantly different from the
240
number that settle with Scene 2, and more test spiders should settle with Scene 1 than with Scene 2.
241
Yet none of these predictions were corroborated by our findings. The number of test spiders that
242
settled when two Portia were at opposite ends of a nest (Scene 3) was not significantly different
243
from that when the two Portia were on an arc (Scene 4) (χ2 = 0.66, P = 0.417) or lined up at one end
244
(Scene 5) (χ2 = 1.95, P = 0.163). The number that settled with Scene 5, however, was significantly
245
less than the number that settled with Scene 2. Moreover, as shown in the previous study (Jackson
246
and Nelson 2012), more test spiders settled with Scene 2 (one Portia present) than with Scene 1
247
(Portia absent), despite Scene 1 offering more space near the nest. This combination of findings
248
suggests that, when confronted with Scenes 1-5, test spiders did not attend to available space near
10
249
the nest and instead made settling decisions on the basis of the specific number (0, 1 or 2) of Portia
250
at the nest, with one being the favoured number.
251
252
Are test spider decisions influenced by whether the number of Portia close to the nest is two or three
253
and, when three, by how the Portia are spaced with respect to the nest?
254
Significantly fewer test spiders settled when three Portia were evenly spaced around the nest
255
(Scene 8) than when two Portia were spaced maximally apart (Scene 3) (P < 0.001, FET). Fewer
256
test spiders settled when there were three (Scene 9) instead of two (Scene 4) Portia positioned on an
257
arc at one end of the nest (χ2 = 30.54, P < 0.001). However, contrary to the limited-space
258
hypothesis, the number of test spiders that settled when three Portia were evenly spaced around the
259
nest (Scene 8) was not significantly different from how many settled when three Portia were on an
260
arc at one end of the nest (Scene 9) (P = 0.053, FET).
261
When Portia were lined up at one end of the nest, the number of test spiders that settled
262
when two were present (Scene 5) was not significantly different from the number that settled when
263
three were present (Scene 10) (χ2 = 0.40, P = 0.529) (i.e., in this instance, response to two Portia
264
was comparable to response to three). Significantly more test spiders settled when three Portia were
265
lined up (Scene 10) rather than evenly spaced around the nest (Scene 8) (P < 0.001, FET) or on an
266
arc at one end of the nest (Scene 9) (χ2 = 46.02, P < 0.001), despite the number of Portia in each of
267
these scenes being three.
268
These findings are as predicted by the limited-space hypothesis because there is more free
269
space near the nest in Scene 10 than in Scenes 8 or 9. Yet we know from the previous study
270
(Jackson and Nelson 2012) that distance from the nest is important. In Scenes 8 and 9, each Portia
271
was close to the nest, but each Portia in Scene 10 was at a different distance from the nest. If test
272
spiders ignore Portia that are very far from the nest, then Scene 10 would have been perceived as a
273
scene with two, not three, Portia. Other comparisons (see below) allow us to consider this
274
hypothesis.
11
275
276
Are test-spider decisions influenced by the distance between Portia and the nest?
277
Significantly more test spiders settled when two Portia that were at opposite ends of a nest were
278
very far from (Scene 11), instead of close to (Scene 3), the nest (χ2 = 19.48, P < 0.001).
279
Significantly more test spiders settled when three Portia that were spaced evenly around a nest were
280
very far from (Scene 12), instead of close to (Scene 8), the nest (P < 0.001, FET). Likewise, more
281
test spiders settled when three Portia that were on an arc at one end of the nest were very far from
282
(Scene 13), instead of close to, the nest (Scene 9) (χ2 = 55.44, P < 0.001).
283
Evidently, when some Portia are very far from the nest and others are close, only those that
284
are close influence the test spiders’ settling decisions. When three Portia were present, significantly
285
fewer test spiders settled when all three were close to the nest (Scene 9) than when the centrally
286
placed Portia was very far from the nest and the other two Portia were close to the nest (Scene 14)
287
(χ2 = 22.34, P < 0.001). When the Portia on the left and right were very far from nest and the
288
centrally placed Portia was close to the nest (Scene 15), significantly more test spiders settled than
289
when all three were close to the nest (Scene 9) (χ2 =154.60). Similarly, significantly more test
290
spiders settled (χ2 =47.21, P < 0.001) when two of the three Portia were very far away (Scene 15),
291
instead of only one being far away (Scene 14).
292
The number of test spiders that settled when there were no Portia present at a nest (Scene 1)
293
was not different from when three Portia were surrounding, but very far from, the nest (Scene 12)
294
(χ2 = 3.28, P = 0.070) and compared with when there were three Portia on an arc at one end, but
295
very far from, the nest (Scene 13) (χ2 = 0.35, P = 0.552).
296
When there were three Portia, two of which were very far from the nest (Scene 15), test
297
spider decisions were not significantly different from when there was a single Portia close to a nest
298
(Scene 2) (χ2 = 0.87, P = 0.351). Additionally, when there were two Portia close to a nest and a
299
third present but very far from the nest (Scene 14), test spider decisions were not different from
300
when there were only two Portia close to the nest (Scene 4) (χ2 = 0.48, P = 0.487).
12
301
302
This combination of findings implies that, when test spiders are making settling decisions,
Portia that are very far from the nest are ignored.
303
304
For test spiders, is distinguishing three from two Portia based on discriminating a triangle from a
305
straight line?
306
When test spiders distinguished Scene 3 from Scene 8 and Scene 4 from Scene 9, they distinguished
307
scenes containing two from scenes containing three Portia. Here we consider whether this might be
308
a distinction between a straight line (defined by two objects) and a triangle (defined by three
309
objects). This more or less corresponds to a hypothesis that, when subitizing, individuals pre-
310
attentively discriminate two from three objects not by representing number per se but instead by
311
identifying canonical configurations, one object corresponding to a dot, two to a straight line and
312
three to a triangle (Mandler and Shebo 1982). This hypothesis suggests that three objects on a
313
horizontal line will not be distinguished from two objects (see Trick and Pylyshyn 1994), yet the
314
number of test spiders that settled was significantly more when there were only two Portia (Scene
315
4) rather than three Portia in a horizontal line (Scene 16) (P < 0.001, FET). Additionally, test-spider
316
settling decisions when three Portia were in a horizontal line (Scene 16) were not significantly
317
different from test-spider decisions when the scene was otherwise identical except that the three
318
Portia were positioned on an arc, thereby defining three points on a virtual triangle (Scene 9) (P =
319
0.204, FET).
320
Perhaps the canonical-configuration hypothesis can be saved by proposing that, while not
321
defining an actual triangle, three objects in a straight line at least defines a potential for a triangle
322
(see Dehaene 1992; Trick and Pylyshyn 1994), but the more straightforward conclusion is that the
323
mechanism underlying Portia’s discrimination between scenes with three versus two already settled
324
Portia is based on information pertaining to numbers.
325
13
326
Are the test-spider decisions influenced by the orientation of the Portia that is already settled at the
327
nest?
328
In the previous study (Jackson and Nelson 2012), where there was never more than one Portia at
329
the nest, test spider settling decisions varied depending on the orientation of the Portia at the nest.
330
Attending to orientation appears to be an additional level of information to process when making
331
settling decisions. Here we investigated whether test spiders use this additional information when
332
confronted by a scene in which more than one Portia is present.
333
Test-spider decisions when two (Scene 17) or three (Scene 18) Portia were facing away
334
from the nest were not significantly different from when two (Scene 3) or three (Scene 8) Portia
335
were facing the nest (two: χ2 =1.13, P = 0.287; three: P = 0.667, FET). These findings suggest that
336
orientation, which matters when only one Portia is seen at a nest, is ignored when there are two
337
Portia at a nest. We might account for this difference by proposing that Portia is subject to capacity
338
limitations that prevent processing information about orientation at the same time as processing
339
information about number, but other comparisons (see below) raise doubts about capacity
340
limitations being this severe. An alternative hypothesis, which seems more likely, is that test spiders
341
are more strongly motivated to process information about the orientation of already settled Portia
342
when the preferred number of them (one) is present and less strongly motivated when the number is
343
two.
344
345
Besides being influenced by seeing Portia at a nest, are test spiders influenced by seeing and
346
identifying the nest inhabitant?
347
Test spiders are more inclined to settle when they see specifically an oecobiid in the nest (Jackson
348
and Nelson 2012). Under the added load of representing object identity, we might expect numerical
349
competence to suffer (see Schmitt and Fischer 2011), yet we found that the identity of the spider in
350
the nest influenced settling decisions even when two Portia were present. Significantly more test
351
spiders settled when the nest was occupied by an oecobiid (Scene 3) than when the nest was
14
352
unoccupied (Scene 6) (χ2 = 9.00, P =0.003) or occupied by a Portia juvenile instead of an oecobiid
353
(Scene 7) (P < 0.001, FET).
354
355
Are test spider decisions influenced by the size of the already settled Portia?
356
Animals have considerable ability to make decisions on the basis of perceived quantities expressed
357
as continuous variables instead of numbers (e.g., Rodríguez and Gamboa 2000; Beran 2010;
358
Krusche et al. 2010; Rodríguez and Gloudeman 2011), and one of the challenges in research on
359
numerical competence is to minimize the influence of confounding continuous variables (Lipton
360
and Speke 2003; Brannon et al. 2004; Hurewitz et al. 2006; Beran et al. 2008). Communal
361
predation on oecobiids occurs only among Portia juveniles within a specific size range (Jackson et
362
al. 2008), enabling us to test for the influence of a continuous variable (Portia stuff) on spider
363
settling decisions by varying the size of already settled Portia over this range.
364
We found that, when there was one Portia in a scene, the number of test spiders that settled
365
were not significantly different depending on whether the Portia was large (Scene 19) or small
366
(Scene 20) (χ2 = 0.51, P =0.476). This was also the case when there were two (Scene 21 vs. Scene
367
22: χ2 = 0.11, P = 0.744) or three (Scene 23 vs. Scene 24: none settled) Portia at the scene. These
368
data suggest that the number of Portia at a scene, not continuous variables, affected decisions.
369
370
Does Portia estimate or subitize?
371
According to the accumulator model we would predict that the number of already settled Portia at a
372
scene affect the latency to settle. In fact, we did find a significant positive relationship between the
373
number of already settled spiders at a scene and latency to settle (H20 = 37.24, P = 0.011), but
374
where we would predict specific effects, for example between Scenes 2, 4 and 9, we found instead
375
that the median times to settle (Table 1) did not differ.
376
377
Being accurate within the range 0-3, Portia’s settling decisions might suggest subitizing, as
this mechanism typically has an upper limit of 3 or 4 (Cowan 2010). However, Portia’s settling
15
378
decisions are also based on considerable classifying of objects in addition to number, such as the
379
distinction between oecobiids and conspecific individuals (even when seen through nest silk),
380
orientation and distance of the already settled Portia with respect to the nest, and nest occupancy. It
381
has been argued that the attention required for extensive classifying is incompatible with subitizing
382
(Treisman and Gelade 1980; Tomonaga 2008; Xu and Chun 2009), which seems to operate at mid-
383
level visual processing (Butterworth 1999, 2008) that functions primarily in individuating items and
384
assigning them to object files (Pylyshyn 1989, 2001) pre-attentively or without making heavy
385
demands on attention (Piazza et al. 2002, 2011; Railo et al. 2008; Burr et al. 2010).
386
387
Does Portia count?
388
When orientation to and distance from the nest were most effectively positioned, as previously
389
determined (Jackson and Nelson 2012), we found different numbers of Portia to be ranked by test
390
spiders when eliciting settling. The strongest stimulus was a single Portia, there being a sharp drop
391
in the number of test spiders that settled when no Portia were present (zero) and in the presence of
392
one more Portia (two). The fewest test spiders settled when there were three Portia present,
393
although it might be more accurate to say ‘more than two’ instead of ‘three’ as we have no evidence
394
to consider whether three is distinguished from four or any other number larger than three.
395
The rationale for the design of our experiments was an understanding of a specific context in
396
which a particular predator deploys a specialized communal predatory strategy. Each individual
397
Portia was tested only once, with no individual having prior experience with the prey or the
398
experimental set up. These methods were appropriate for our objective of determining whether
399
specific numbers are innately salient to this predator based on its specific predatory strategy. This is
400
rather different from typical objectives in the literature on the numerical competence of animals,
401
which aim to test more general-purpose number-based decisions, such as go-for-more rules (Beran
402
2008; Beran et al. 2008).
16
403
Number appears to be a salient cue when Portia makes settling decisions, but there is no
404
basis for proposing that Portia’s decisions are based on anything especially close to true counting.
405
For example, true counting is based on representing numbers as properties of sets that stay the same
406
even when the identity of the particular objects in a set change, but the expression of numerical
407
competence we have investigated seems to be linked tightly to objects with highly specific
408
characteristics (i.e., other Portia already settled at an oecobiid nest).
409
410
Does Portia use prototype-matching when making number-related decisions?
411
Although numerical competence appears to be important in Portia’s communal predatory strategy,
412
variables related to number appear to be one of an assortment of variables that influence settling
413
decisions. Objects belonging to three categories seem to be identified, a nest and two spider
414
categories: the prey (the oecobiid) and other Portia, and how these objects are arranged with respect
415
to each other is also pertinent. We can not rule out that Portia’s numerical competence is expressed
416
in the context of classifying scenes, with Portia’s settling decisions being based on how closely the
417
scene being viewed matches a prototype scene.
418
Prototype-matching is better known as a mechanism by which individuals assign items to
419
non-numerical categories (Rosch 1975; Rosch et al. 1976) and it is in the context of predatory
420
versatility (Curio 1976) that non-numerical categories have been considered in salticid research.
421
Versatile predators adopt conditional predatory strategies, with each individual deploying different
422
capture tactics depending on prey type. By adopting different behaviour in encounters with different
423
prey, these predators reveal the salient prey categories underlying their classification schemes.
424
Species from the genus Portia have a large repertoire of innate prey-specific capture tactics (Nelson
425
and Jackson 2011) possibly based on matching prey exemplars against a collection of prototypes
426
(Jackson and Cross 2011).
427
428
In the context of communal predation by Portia on oecobiids, we may have evidence of an
innately salient category, but in this instance the most relevant exemplars and prototype may be
17
429
scenes instead of individual prey items or individual prey categories. That Portia will decide to
430
settle may become increasingly more likely as exemplar-prototype resemblance becomes closer,
431
with the prototype being defined in part by a number-related factor.
432
Prototype-matching as a mechanism underlying numerical competence has been proposed
433
before (Terrell and Thomas 1990), yet is rarely considered. Like subitizing and estimating, this
434
mechanism does not require that the animal has access to representations of specific numbers in the
435
full abstract meaning of true numbers, but it does imply that information specific to a number rather
436
than, for example, a continuous variable, is salient. For Portia, numerical competence in the context
437
of preying on oecobiids may be a consequence of this predator adopting, as part of a conditional
438
predatory strategy, a highly specialized prototype-matching routine at the level of scenes instead of
439
only at the level of individual prey items.
440
18
441
Acknowledgements
442
Godfrey Otieno Sune, Stephen Aluoch and Jane Atieno Obonyo provided invaluable technical
443
assistance. We acknowledge financial support from the National Geographic Society (RRJ), the
444
Royal Society of New Zealand (Marsden Fund; RRJ; James Cook Fellowship, RRJ).
445
446
References
447
Agrillo C, Dadda M, Serena G (2008) Do fish count? Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in
448
449
450
451
452
female mosquitofish. Anim Cogn 11:495-503
Anderson DE, Vogel EK, Awh E (2011) Precision in visual working memory reaches a stable
plateau when individual item limits are exceeded. J Neurosci 31:1128-1138
Awh E, Barton B, Vogel EK (2007) Visual working memory represents a fixed number of items
regardless of complexity. Psychol Sci 18:622-628
453
Baddeley A (1986) Working memory. Oxford University Press, Oxford
454
Baddeley A (2003) Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:829-
455
456
839
Benson-Amram S, Heinen VK, Dryer SL, Holekamp KE (2011) Numerical assessment and
457
individual call discrimination by wild spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Anim Behav
458
82:743-752
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
Beran MJ (2001) Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items
by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115:181-191
Beran MJ (2008) Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) succeed in a test of quantity conservation.
Anim Cogn 11:109-116
Beran MJ (2010) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) accurately compare poured liquid quantities.
Anim Cogn 13:641-649
Beran MJ, Evans TA, Harris EH (2008) Perception of food amounts by chimpanzees based on the
number, size, contour length and visibility of items. Anim Behav 75:1793-1802
19
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
Boysen ST, Berntson GG (1989) Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J
Comp Psychol 103:23-31
Boysen ST, Berntson GG (1995) Responses to quantity: Perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in
chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process 21:82-86
Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA (2011) A review of visual memory capacity: beyond individual
items and toward structured representations. J Vis 11:1-34
Brannon EM (2002) The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy. Cognition
83:223-240
475
Brannon EM (2006) The representation of numerical magnitude. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:222-229
476
Brannon EM, Van de Walle G (2001) The development of ordinal numerical competence in young
477
478
479
480
481
children. Cogn Psychol 43:53-81
Brannon EM, Abbott S, Lutz DJ (2004) Number bias for the discrimination of large visual sets in
infancy. Cognition 93:B59-B68
Breukelaar JWC, Dalrymple-Alford JC (1998) Timing ability and numerical competence in rats. J
Exp Psychol 24:84-97
482
Burr DC, Ross J (2008) A visual sense of number. Curr Biol 18:425-428
483
Burr DC, Turi M, Anobile G (2010) Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional
484
resources. J Vis 10(6):1-10
485
Butterworth B (1999) A head for figures. Science 284:928-929
486
Butterworth B (2008) Numerosity perception: how many speckles on the hen? Curr Biol 16:R388-
487
389
488
Cantlon JF, Brannon EM (2007) Basic math in monkeys and college students. PLoS ONE 5:e328
489
Carazo P, Font E, Forteza-Behrendt E (2009) Quantity discrimination in Tenebrio molitor: evidence
490
of numerosity discrimination in an invertebrate? Anim Cogn 12:463-470
491
Carey S (1998) Knowledge of number: its evolution and ontogeny. Science 282:641-642
20
492
493
Carey S (2001) Cognitive foundations of arithmetic: evolution and ontogenesis. Mind Lang 16:3755
494
Castelli F, Glaser DE, Butterworth B (2006) Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the
495
parietal lobe: a functional MRI study. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 103:4693-4698
496
Charnov EL (1976) Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theor Popul Biol 9:129-136
497
Chittka L, Geiger K (1995) Can honey-bees count landmarks? Anim Behav 49:159-164
498
Church RM, Broadbent HA (1990) Alternative representations of time, number, and rate. Cognition
499
500
501
37:55-81
Clearfield MW, Mix KS (1999) Number versus contour length in infants’ discrimination of small
visual sets. Psychol Sci 10:408-411
502
Cowan N (1998) Visual and auditory working memory capacity. Trends Cogn Sci 2:77-78
503
Cowan N (2000) The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage
504
505
506
capacity. Behav Brain Sci 24:87-185
Cowan N (2010) The magical mystery four: how is working memory capacity limited, and why?
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19:51-57
507
Curio E (1976) The Ethology of Predation. Springer-Verlag, Berlin
508
Dacke M, Srinivasan MV (2008) Evidence for counting in insects. Anim Cogn 11:683-689
509
Dadda M, Piffer L, Agrillo C, Biazza A (2009) Spontaneous number representation in mosquitofish.
510
511
512
513
514
Cognition 112:343-348
Davis H, Memmott J (1982) Counting behavior in animals: a critical evaluation. Psychol Bull
92:547-571
Davis H, MacKenzie KA, Morrison S (1989) Numerical discrimination by rats (Rattus norvegicus)
using body and vibrissal touch. J Comp Psychol 103:45-53
515
Dehaene S (1992) Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition 44:1-42
516
Dehaene S (2001) Precis of the number sense. Mind Lang 16: 16-36
517
Feigenson L, Dehaene S, Spelke E (2004) Core systems of number. Trends Cogn Sci 8:307-314
21
518
519
520
521
522
Flombaum JI, Junge JA, Hauser MD (2005) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously
compute addition operations over large numbers. Cognition 97:315-325
Fougnie D, Marois R (2011) Distinct capacity limits for attention and working memory. Psychol
Sci 17:526-534
Franks NR, Dornhaus A, Metherell BG, Nelson TR, Lanfear SAJ, Symes WS (2006) Not
523
everything that counts can be counted: ants use multiple metrics for a single nest trait. Proc
524
R Soc B 273:165-169
525
526
527
528
Gallistel CR, Gelman R (2000) Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends
Cogn Sci 4:59-65
Gelman R, Gallistel CR (1978) The child’s understanding of number. Harvard University Press,
Cambridge, MA
529
Giaquinto M (2001) What cognitive systems underlie arithmetical abilities? Mind Lang 16:56-68
530
Gross HJ, Pahl M, Si A, Zhu H, Tautz J, Zhang S (2009) Number-based visual generalisation in the
531
532
533
534
honeybee. PLoS ONE 4(1): e4263
Hager MC, Helfman GS (1991) Safety in numbers: shoal size choice by minnows under predatory
threat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 29:271-276
Hanus D, Call J (2007) Discrete quantity judgments in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan
535
troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus): the effect of presenting whole sets versus
536
item-by-item. J Comp Psychol 121:241-249
537
Harland DP, Jackson RR (2001) Prey classification by Portia fimbriata, a salticid spider that
538
specializes at preying on other salticids: species that elicit cryptic stalking. J Zool 255:445-
539
460
540
541
Haun DBM, Jordan FM, Vallortigara G, Clayton NS (2010) Origins of spatial, temporal and
numerical cognition: Insights from comparative psychology. Trends Cog Sci 14:552-560
22
542
Hauser MD, Carey S (1999) Building a cognitive creature from a set of primitives: evolutionary and
543
developmental insights. In: Allen C, Cummins D (eds.) The Evolution of Mind. Oxford
544
University Press, Oxford
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
Hauser MD, Carey S (2003) Spontaneous representations of small numbers of objects by rhesus
macaques: examination of content and format. Cognit Psychol 47:367-491
Hoare DJ, Couzin ID, Godin GJ, Krause J (2004) Context-dependent group size choice in fish.
Anim Behav 67:155-164
Hunt S, Low J, Burns KC (2008) Adaptive numerical competency in a food-hoarding songbird.
Proc R Soc B 275:2373-2379
Hurewitz F, Gelman R, Schnitzer B (2006) Sometimes area counts more than number. Proc Nat
Acad Sci USA 103:19599-19604
553
Jackson RR, Cross FR (2011) Spider cognition. Adv Insect Physiol 41:115-174
554
Jackson RR & Nelson XJ (2012) Attending to detail by communal spider-eating spiders. Anim
555
556
557
558
Cogn In Press. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0469-y
Jackson RR, Pollard SD, Salm K (2008) Observations of Portia africana, an araneophagic jumping
spider, living together and sharing prey. N Z J Zool 35:237-242
Jakob EM, Christa D, Skow CD, Long S (2011) Plasticity, learning and cognition. In: Herberstein
559
ME (ed) Spider behaviour: flexibility and versatility. Cambridge University Press, New York,
560
pp. 307-347
561
562
563
564
Kahneman D, Treisman A, Gibbs BJ (1992) The reviewing of object files: object-specific
integration of information. Cognit Psychol 24:175-219
Kaufman EL, Lord MW, Reese TW, Volkmann J (1949) The discrimination of visual number. Am
J Psychol 62:498-525
565
Kitchen DM (2006) Alpha male black howler monkey responses to loud calls: effect of numeric
566
odds, male companion behaviour and reproductive investment. Anim Behav 67:125-139
23
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
Kobayashi T, Hiraki K, Mugitani R, Hasegawa T (2004) Baby arithmetic: one object plus one tone.
Cognition 91:B23-B34
Krusche P, Uller C, Dicke U (2010) Quantity discrimination in salamanders. J Exp Biol 213:18221828
Le Corre M, Carey S (2008) Why the verbal counting principles are constructed out of
representations of small sets of individuals: a reply to Gallistel. Cognition 107:650-662
Lipton JS, Spelke ES (2003) Origins of number sense: large-number discrimination in human
infants. Psychol Sci 14:396-401
Lyon BE (2003) Egg recognition and counting reduce costs of avian conspecific brood parasitism.
Nature 422:495-499
Mandler G, Shebo BJ (1982) Subitizing: an analysis of its conceptual processes. J Exp Psychol
111:1-22
579
Matsuzawa T (1985) Use of numbers by a chimpanzee. Nature 315:57-59
580
McComb K, Packer C, Pusey A (1994) Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between
581
582
583
584
585
586
groups of lions, Panthera leo. Anim Behav 47:379-387
McCrink K, Wynn K (2004) Large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants.
Psychol Sci 15:776-781
Meck WH, Church RM (1983) A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J Exp
Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:320-334
Nelson XJ, Jackson RR (2011) Flexibility in the foraging strategies of spiders In: Herberstein ME
587
(ed) Spider behaviour: flexibility and versatility. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp.
588
31-56
589
Owen AM (2004) Working memory: imaging the magic number four. Curr Biol 14:R573-R574
590
Pepperberg IM (2006) Grey parrot numerical competence: a review. Anim Cogn 9:377-391
591
Piazza M, Fumarola A, Chinello A, Melcher D (2011) Subitizing reflects visuo-spatial object
592
individuation capacity. Cognition 121:147-153
24
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
Piazza M, Mechelli A, Butterworth B, Price CJ (2002) Are subitizing and counting implemented as
separate or functionally overlapping processes? NeuroImage 15:435-446
Pylyshyn ZW (1989) The role of location indexes in spatial perception: a sketch of the FINST
spatial-index model. Cognition 32:65-97
Pylyshyn ZW (2001) Visual indexes, preconceptual objects, and situated vision. Cognition 80:127158
Pyke GH, Pulliam HR, Charnov EL (1977) Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests.
Q Rev Biol 52:137-154
Railo H, Koivisto M, Revonsuo A, Hannula MM (2008) The role of attention in subitizing.
Cognition 107:82-104
Reznikova Z, Ryabko B (2011) Numerical competence in animals, with an insight from ants.
Behaviour 148:405-434
Riggs KJ, Ferrand L, Lancelin D, Fryziel L, Dumur G (2006) Subitizing in tactile perception.
Psychol Sci 17:271-272
Rodríguez RL, Gamboa E (2000) Memory of captured prey in three web spiders (Araneae:
Araneidae, Linyphiidae, Tetragnathidae). Anim Cogn 3:91-97
609
Rodríguez RL, Gloudeman MD (2011) Estimating the repeatability of memories of captured prey
610
formed by Frontinella communis spiders (Araneae: Linyphiidae). Anim Cogn 14:675-682
611
Rosch E (1975) Cognitive representations of semantic categories. J Exp Psychol 104:192-233
612
Rosch E, Mervis CB, Greay WD, Jonhson DM, Boyes-Braem P (1976) Basic objects in natural
613
614
615
616
617
categories. Cognit Psychol 8:382-429
Rugani R, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2007) Rudimental numerical competence in 5-day-old
domestic chicks (Gallus gallus): identification of ordinal position. J Exp Psychol 33:21-31
Rugani R, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2010) Imprinted numbers: newborn chicks’ sensitivity to
number vs. continuous extent of objects they have been reared with. Dev Sci 13:790-797
25
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
Rugani R, Fontanari L, Simoni E, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2009) Arithmetic in newborn chicks
Proc R Soc B 276:2451-2460
Schmitt, V., Fischer, J 2011. Representational format determines numerical competence in
monkeys. Nature commun 2, 257. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1262
Shettleworth SJ (2009) Cognition, Evolution, and Behaviour. 2nd edition. Oxford University Press,
New York
Seron X, Pesenti M (2001) The number sense theory needs more empirical evidence. Mind Lang
16:76-88
Slaughter V, Itakura S, Kutsuki A, Siegal M (2011) Learning to count begins in infancy: evidence
from 18 month olds’ visual preferences. Proc R Soc B 278:2979-2984
Starkey P, Spelke ES, Gelman R (1983) Detection of intermodal numerical correspondences by
human infants. Science 222:179-181
630
Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1986) Foraging Theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton
631
Tardif SD, Layne DG, Smucny DA (2002) Can marmoset mothers count to three? Effect of litter
632
size on mother-infant interactions. Ethology 108:825-836
633
Terrell DF, Thomas RK (1990) Number-related discrimination and summation by squirrel monkeys
634
(Saimiri sciureus sciureus and S. boliviensus boliviensus) on the basis of the number of sides
635
of polygons. J Comp Psychol 104:238-247
636
Thomas RK, Simmons LW (2010) Cuticular hydrocarbons influence female attractiveness to males
637
in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus. J Evol Biol 23:707-714
638
Tomonaga M (2008) Relative numerosity discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes):
639
evidence for approximate numerical representations. Anim Cogn 11:43-57
640
Treisman AM, Gelade G (1980) A feature integration theory of attention Cogn Psychol 12:97-136
641
Trick LM (2005) The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: patterns of selective
642
interference in subitizing and counting. Psychon Bull Rev 12:675-681
26
643
644
645
646
647
Trick LM, Pylyshyn ZW (1994) Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A
limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychol Rev 101:80-102
Uller C, Hauser MD, Carey S (2001) Spontaneous representation of number in cotton-top tamarins.
J Comp Psychol 115:1-10
Uller C, Jaeger R, Guidry G, Martin C (2003) Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more:
648
rudiments of number in a species of basal vertebrate. Anim Cogn 6:105-112
649
Wesley F (1961) The number concept: a phylogenetic review. Psychol Bull 53: 420-428
650
Wood JN, Hauser MC, Glynn DD, Barner D (2007) Free-ranging rhesus monkeys spontaneously
651
individuate and enumerate small numbers of non-solid portions. Cognition 106:207-221
652
Wynn K (1992) Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature 358:749-750
653
Wynn K (1998) Psychological foundations of number: numerical competence in human infants.
654
655
656
657
Trends Cogn Sci 2:296-303
Xu Y, Chun MM (2009) Selecting and perceiving multiple visual objects. Trends Cogn Sci 13:167174
Xu Y, Spelke ES (2000) Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition 74:B1-11
658
659
660
27
Table 1 Data from testing Portia africana juveniles with ‘scenes’ made from lures (dead spiders mounted in lifelike posture) and silken nests. See text
for details. Radiating lines: nest. Black polygon: lure made from adult Oecobius amboseli (Oa) female (always in nest). Grey polygon: lure made from
Portia africana (Pa) juvenile (body length 2mm, unless stated otherwise). White circles (‘eyes’) represent direction in which lure faced. When more
than one lure was in a scene the centre-to-centre distances of all lures was 8 mm unless stated otherwise. Q1 and Q3: 1st and 3rd quartile, respectively.
1
Data from Jackson and Nelson (In Press). *N = 200; all other N = 100.
Scene
Scene description
No.
Diagram
Attacked
Attacked
Settled
Median latency to settle
nest (N)
already settled
(N)
(min) (Q1, Q3)
spider (N)
11*
Oa in nest
9
na
75
22 (13, 31)
21*
Pa and Oa facing each other
1
0
131
26 (15, 36)
3*
Two Pa at opposite ends of nest, one facing Oa
5
2
38
25 (14.5, 29)
head-on and other facing rear of Oa
28
4
Two Pa side-by-side in front of and facing Oa
4
0
23
20 (10, 40)
5
Two Pa lined up one behind the other at one end
1
2
26
28.5 (18.75, 47.5)
0
0
6
22.5 (16.25, 34.25)
0
0
4
23.5 (16.75, 30.25)
0
2
1
39 (39, 39)
3
0
6
21.5 (13.5, 26.25)
of nest. Pa head-on to Oa
6
Two Pa. Each Pa at opposite end of nest. Both
facing empty nest
7
Two Pa at opposite ends of a nest. Both facing
nest. Pa in nest
8*
Three Pa surrounding nest, forming triangle with
Oa at centre. Each Pa facing Oa. Pa in lower
position head-on to Oa
9*
Three Pa side-by-side in front of and facing Oa
29
10
Three Pa lined up one behind the other. Pa head-
4
0
30
18 (11.5, 34.25)
2
0
43
28 (24, 36)
5
0
27
28 (21, 36)
5
0
34
28 (16.5, 39.25)
on to Oa. 1st Pa juvenile close to, 2nd far & 3rd
very far from nest
11
Oa and one Pa facing each other. Other Pa
opposite, facing rear of Oa. Both Pa very far
from nest
12
Three Pa surrounding nest, forming triangle with
Oa at centre. Pa in lower position facing Oa
head-on. All Pa very far from nest
13
Three Pa side-by-side facing Oa (one head-on).
All Pa very far from nest
30
14
Three Pa side-by-side facing Oa. One Pa head-
1
0
19
31 (26, 40)
0
0
122
29 (22, 36.25)
1
2
1
43 (43, 43)
0
3
30
27 (18.5, 36.25)
2
2
0
na
on to Oa, very far from nest and positioned
between other two Pa. Each of other two Pa
close to nest
15*
Three Pa side-by-side facing Oa. One Pa headon to Oa, close to nest and positioned between
other two Pa which are very far from nest
16
Three Pa directly to the side of another facing
Oa (one head-on)
17*
Two Pa at opposite end of nest, both facing away
from Oa
18
Three Pa surrounding nest. Each Pa facing away
from Oa
31
19
Pa (large) and Oa, facing each other
0
0
59
25 (16, 31)
20
Pa (small) and Oa, facing each other
2
0
54
24 (17, 33.75)
21
Two large Pa at opposite end of nest, one facing
0
0
24
27.5 (22.5, 35)
1
1
27
31 (19, 36)
0
1
0
na
Oa head-on and other facing rear of Oa
22
Two small Pa at opposite end of nest, one facing
Oa head-on and other facing rear of Oa
23
Three large Pa surrounding nest, forming
triangle with Oa at centre. Pa in lower position
head-on to Oa
32
24
Three small Pa surrounding nest, forming
0
2
0
na
triangle with Oa at centre. Each Pa facing Oa.
Pa in lower position head-on to Oa
33
Download