1 The role of numerical competence in a specialized predatory strategy of an araneophagic 2 spider 3 4 5 Short Title: Spider numeracy 6 7 Ximena J Nelson1* 8 Robert R Jackson1,2 9 10 1 11 Zealand. 12 2 13 Mbita Point, Kenya. School of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New International Centre of Insect Physiology and Ecology, Thomas Odhiambo Campus, P.0. Box 30 14 15 *Correspondence: Ximena Nelson, School of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury, 16 Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealand. 17 Email: ximena.nelson@canterbury.ac.nz 18 Phone: 64-3-3642987 extn 4050 19 Fax: 64-3-3642590 20 21 22 23 1 24 25 Although a wide range of vertebrates have been considered in research on numerical competence, 26 little is known about the role of number-related decisions in the predatory strategies of 27 invertebrates. Here we investigate how numerical competence is expressed in a highly specialized 28 predatory strategy adopted by the small juveniles of Portia africana when practicing communal 29 predation, with the prey being another spider, Oecobius amboseli. Two or more P. africana 30 juveniles sometimes settle by the same oecobiid nest and then share the meal after one individual 31 captures the oecobiid. Experiments were designed to clarify how these predators use number-related 32 cues in conjunction with non-numerical cues when deciding whether to settle at a nest. We used 33 lures (dead spiders positioned in lifelike posture) arranged in a series of 24 different scenes defined 34 by the type, configuration and especially number of lures. On the whole, our findings suggest that 35 P. africana juveniles base settling decisions on the specific number of already settled conspecific 36 juveniles at the nest and express a preference for settling when the number is one instead of zero, 37 two or three. By varying the size of the already settled juveniles and their positions around the nest, 38 we show that factors related to continuous variables and stimulus configuration are unlikely 39 explanations for our findings. 40 41 Keywords: numerosity discrimination, numerical ability, prototype matching, communal predation, 42 araneophagy, Salticidae 43 2 44 Introduction 45 Understanding the nature and origin of numerical competence is a primary objective in human and 46 animal cognitive science (Carey 1998, 2001; Giaquinto 2001; Feigenson et al. 2004; Shettleworth 47 2009). Primates have been prominent in the literature on animal numerical competence, this trend 48 being consistent with a tradition of associating numerical competence with advanced human 49 language-based cognition (Dehaene 2001; Haun et al. 2010). Yet the once widely accepted view 50 that numerical competence is uniquely human and fundamentally dependent on verbal language has 51 now been supplanted by a rapidly expanding literature (Wesley 1961; Davis and Memmot 1982; 52 Brannon 2002; Cantlon and Brannon 2007; Slaughter et al. 2011) on the numerical competence of 53 pre-verbal human infants (hereafter, simply ‘infants’) and on non-human animals (hereafter, simply 54 ‘animals’), including a wide range of vertebrate species as well as examples from insects (Chittka 55 and Geiger 1995; Dacke and Srinivason 2008; Gross et al. 2009; Rezinikova and Ryabko 2011). 56 Although research on infants has been based on spontaneous response to number-related 57 cues (Wynn 1992, 1998), much of the early animal work depended on extensive training (e.g., 58 Matsuzawa 1985; Boysen and Bernston 1995; Pepperberg 2006) and this encouraged an impression 59 that, for animals, numerical competence is a hard-earned ability that is adopted only as a last resort 60 (Breukelaar and Dalrymple-Alford 1998; Seron and Pesenti 2001). However, there has been a shift 61 toward considering spontaneous expression of numerical competence by animals (Uller et al. 2001; 62 Flombaum et al. 2005; Hanus and Call 2007; Wood et al. 2007; Agrillo et al. 2008; Dadda et al. 63 2009) and this has, in turn, increased interest in understanding how numerical competence functions 64 for particular animals in their natural environments (Hager and Helfman 1991; McComb et al. 65 1994; Lyon 2003; Kitchen 2006; Hunt et al. 2008; Benson-Amram et al. 2011). 66 In the context of animal-centred research on the expression of numerical competence, 67 optimal foraging theory (OFT) has an interesting history. OFT began in a way that could be 68 described as decidedly uninterested in cognition (Pyke et al. 1977; Stephens and Krebs 1986). For 69 example, terms such as ‘strategy’ have always been prevalent in the OFT literature, but often with 3 70 disclaimers to the effect that nothing cognitive was intended. Yet, from the beginning, OFT was 71 relevant to hypotheses about cognitive decision-making and especially numerical cognition 72 (Shettleworth 2009). In particular, predictions from OFT (Charnov 1976) often imply considerable 73 capacity on the part of the animal to make relative number and relative quantity judgments (see 74 Church and Broadbent 1990; Boysen and Bernston 1995; Uller et al. 2003; Brannon 2006; Castelli 75 et al. 2006). Animals may sometimes express a go-for-less strategy (e.g., Hoare et al. 2004). 76 However, there are also many examples of a go-for-more strategy (e.g., Boysen and Bernston 1995; 77 Beran 2008; Beran et al. 2008). These results imply that optimal foraging includes judgments of 78 relative number (see Shettleworth 2009). 79 Less is known about animals adopting strategies in which specific numbers are innately 80 salient and responded to spontaneously (see Tardif et al. 2002). Yet, the distinction between relative 81 number judgment and choosing a specific number becomes important when specifying categories of 82 numerical competence (Gelman and Gallistel 1978; Dehaene 1992; Gallistel and Gelman 2000). 83 Kaufman et al. (1949), for example, distinguished between counting, subitizing and estimating. 84 True counting is an iterative process based on assigning a distinct tag to each item, with tags being 85 symbols, tokens or physical referents. Counting achieves more than simply detecting a difference 86 between two numbers, as it implies that the individual doing the counting has access to 87 representations of ordinal and cardinal numbers (Gelman and Gallistel 1978), a phenomenon which 88 has been demonstrated especially clearly in primates after varying degrees of training with Arabic 89 numerals (Boysen and Bernston 1989, 1995). 90 Subitizing is a mechanism by which an individual can, without having access to a 91 representation of a specific number, nonetheless make decisions corresponding to specific numbers 92 (Trick and Pylyshyn 1994; Railo et al. 2008), with the maximum typically being 3-4 (Cowan 1998, 93 2000, 2010). The mechanisms underlying subitizing appear to be related to individuating objects 94 (Trick 2005) and assigning them to a limited number of object files (Kahneman et al. 1992) in 95 working memory (Baddeley 1986, 2003; Owen 2004; Awh et al. 2007; Anderson et al. 2011; 4 96 Fougnie and Marois 2011). The individual is assumed to form a representation of occupied object 97 files, but does not necessarily use these object files as representations of numbers (Trick and 98 Pylyshyn 1994; Xu and Spelke 2000; Hauser and Carey 2003). 99 When ‘estimating’, an individual might represent an analogue magnitude, or a characteristic 100 called the ‘numerousness’ of a set, or the individual might derive a relative number judgment by 101 making comparisons (Kobayashi et al. 2004; McCrink and Wynn 2004). This system, known as the 102 ‘accumulator model’ or ‘analogue-magnitude system’, is used for estimating how different large 103 numbers compare to each other (Meck and Church 1983; Gallistel and Gelman 2000) and does not 104 require that the individual have access to either a representation of a specific number or to any 105 particular information pertaining to a specific number (Le Corre and Carey 2008). This model is 106 based on the premise that memory is an inherently noisy medium. According to the accumulator 107 model, when non-verbal representations of numerosity are coded into memory, variability increases 108 in proportion to the square root of the number targeted for memory (Weber’s Law), with this 109 accounting for the seeming discontinuity (≤ 4) in the capacity of animals to perceive number 110 directly. However, the accumulator model does not distinguish between the ability to count and 111 summation of variables that covary with the counted objects, such as volume or area (Beran 2001). 112 Although there has been research on numerical competence in auditory (Starkey et al. 1983), 113 tactile (Davis et al. 1989; Riggs et al. 2006) and chemical (Carazo et al. 2009; Thomas and 114 Simmons 2010) channels, most of the literature on numerical competence pertains to vision 115 (Dehaene 1992; Burr and Ross 2008; Brady et al. 2011). It is striking that the literature on animal 116 numerical competence does not include salticid spiders, as these small predators have intricate 117 vision-based behaviour and the capacity to classify prey by sight into multiple categories (Nelson 118 and Jackson 2011; Harland and Jackson 2001). Salticids belonging to the genus Portia are of 119 particular interest because they have often been subjects in research related to animal cognition 120 (Jackson and Cross 2011; Jakob et al. 2011). 5 121 Here we consider the role of number-related cues in an especially intricate predatory 122 strategy adopted by the small juveniles (body length 1.5-3.0 mm) of P. africana in Kenya (hereafter 123 ‘Portia’) when preying on Oecobius amboseli (hereafter ‘oecobiid’), a small spider species (adult 124 body length, 2-3 mm) that builds tent-like silk nests on boulders, tree trunks and the walls of 125 buildings (Jackson et al. 2008). 126 A typical oecobiid nest is sparsely woven and only about 5 mm in diameter, with the 127 resident oecobiid inside usually remaining visible, although less distinct than when outside. When 128 disturbed by a potential predator, the oecobiid runs away from its nest in a straight line and then 129 suddenly freezes, but eventually walks back and re-enters the nest. Portia juveniles exploit this 130 behaviour by first settling near to the nest and eventually, with its palps and forelegs, initiating 131 intermittent probing and striking of the nest silk. Portia may capture the oecobiid as it comes out of 132 the nest in an attempt to flee. Alternatively, the oecobiid may escape, but Portia waits at the nest 133 and captures it upon its return (Jackson et al. 2008). 134 Although most salticids are solitary predators, Portia often practises communal predation, 135 especially when the prey is an oecobiid. In typical sequences, two Portia settle alongside each other 136 at an oecobiid’s nest and when one Portia captures an oecobiid, it is joined by the other to feed 137 alongside it. For the critical decision of whether to settle at an oecobiid nest, variables known to 138 matter include whether a nest is present and occupied by an oecobiid instead of a similar-sized 139 Portia, whether another Portia is already settled near the nest and is facing toward or away from the 140 nest (Jackson and Nelson 2012). 141 That number-related cues might be salient is suggested because more than two Portia 142 feeding together or settled together alongside the same oecobiid nest are rarely seen (Jackson et al. 143 2008). Taking into consideration the non-numerical cues already known to be important (Jackson 144 and Nelson 2012), the hypothesis we consider is that Portia bases decisions on the number of 145 Portia settled at a nest inside which there is an oecobiid, with the settled Portia being in a particular 146 orientation to the nest and within a specific distance range from the nest. 6 147 As in any study designed to consider the role of number-related cues (Clearfield and Mix 148 1999; Hurewitz et al. 2006; Franks et al. 2008), we need to consider possibility that non-numerical 149 variables alone suffice to explain our findings. Here we consider three alternative hypotheses, each 150 of which specifies particular confounding variables: continuous variables such as the area in a scene 151 filled by salient already settled Portia (‘Portia-stuff hypothesis’); the amount of free space 152 remaining alongside a nest (‘limited-space hypothesis’); the configuration in which already settled 153 Portia are arranged at the nest (‘canonical-configuration hypothesis’). 154 155 Methods 156 All test subjects were F2-generation juveniles (body length, 2.5 mm) of Portia africana from 157 laboratory culture (standard rearing procedures; for details, see Jackson et al. 2008). Here we focus 158 on the details that pertain specifically to numerical cues, with only brief descriptions being provided 159 for experimental detail shared with the previous study (Jackson Nelson 2012). 160 As our goal was to investigate Portia’s innate, spontaneous decisions, we ensured that 161 neither the test spiders nor their parents had prior experience with oecobiids. By basing our 162 experiments on the reactions of test spiders (living Portia) to stationary lures (dead spiders 163 positioned in lifelike posture in ‘scenes’), we avoided confounding variables that can arise when 164 testing predators with living prey (see Nelson and Jackson 2011). We minimized the possibility of 165 residual odour cues by making lures from spiders that had been kept in ethanol. After being 166 positioned in a scene, the lures were sprayed with aerosol plastic for preservation. 167 Portia lures were made from juveniles belonging to three size categories (body length: small 168 1.5 mm, medium 2.5 mm, and large 3.0 mm). Test spiders were always medium and, unless stated 169 otherwise, Portia lures were also medium. The oecobiids used for making lures were field-collected 170 adult females (body length 2.5 mm). 171 172 In experiments, each treatment was based on presenting test spiders with a scene. For each scene, lures were centred in a different arrangement on the flat surface of a 50 mm-wide rubber 7 173 stopper (hereafter referred to simply as the ‘disc’). Scenes were defined by: the number of lures 174 present; the type, configuration and orientation of the lures; whether Portia lures were small, 175 medium or large; whether there was a spider present in an artificial nest, and the distance between 176 lures and the nest (see Table 1). To create artificial nests (diameter 5.0 mm), silk was taken from a 177 nest built by an oecobiid that had been kept in the laboratory without prey for the previous 7 days 178 and then draped over a lure. No individual test spider, lure or nest was used more than once (N for 179 each scene was 100 or 200; see Table 1) and all tests were conducted between 0800 and 1200 hours 180 (laboratory photoperiod, 12L-12D, lights on 0700 hours). 181 For making a test chamber (for details see Jackson and Nelson 2012), the disc was first 182 inserted into a hole centred on bottom of a Petri dish so that it protruded 2 mm into the dish. The lid 183 was then put on the Petri dish, creating the test chamber with the scene enclosed. The dish was then 184 turned vertical and held in place by clamping the distal end of the disc to a retort stand (lowest point 185 of Petri dish 200 mm above bench). After a 5 min settling period within a glass tube, a test spider 186 was allowed access to the test chamber through an entry hole directly across from the centre of the 187 scene (i.e., through the ‘lid’ of the Petri dish). Tests began when the test spider walked into the test 188 chamber. 189 When a scene included one or more lures positioned outside the nest, with or without a lure 190 inside, the ‘already settled Portia’ lures outside were referred to simply as ‘Portia’, with ‘test 191 spiders’ being used to differentiate between dead (lures) and live Portia. Depending on the scene, 192 three distances of Portia lures from the nest were applicable. These were defined by distance from 193 the centre of the already settled spider’s body and the centre of the nest: close (8 mm), far (16 mm) 194 and very far (24 mm). ‘Evenly spread around a nest’ refers to scenes in which Portia were 195 positioned so as to make an equilateral triangle with the nest at the centre (e.g., Scene 8, Table 1). 196 Side by side ‘on an arc’ refers to scenes in which two or more Portia were positioned on an arc with 197 each Portia equidistant from the centre of the nest. When there were three Portia on an arc, the 198 Portia in the centre was directly below the nest, on an imaginary line drawn straight down from the 8 199 centre of the nest, one of the other two spiders was on an imaginary line running through the centre 200 of the nest 45o to the left and the other was on an imaginary line running through the nest 45o to the 201 right (e.g., Scene 9, Table 1). There was no Portia in the middle position when only two Portia 202 were present, but the left and the right individuals were positioned the same as when three were 203 present. ‘In a horizontal line’ refers to scenes similar to placement on an arc except that the Portia 204 formed an imaginary straight line alongside the nest instead of an arc centred on the nest (e.g., 205 Scene 16, Table 1). ‘Lined up one behind the other’ refers to scenes in which two or three Portia 206 were positioned so that the first individual faced the nest, the second faced the first from the rear 207 and the third faced the second Portia, with the first Portia being close to the nest, the second being 208 far from nest and, if present, the third very far from nest (e.g., Scene 10, Table 1). 209 Tests ended and the test outcome was recorded when the test spider settled (i.e., when the 210 test spider walked on to the disc and, without first attacking a lure or a nest, became quiescent for 2 211 min), attacked (i.e., without first settling, the test spider leapt, lunged at or struck at a lure or a nest), 212 or else neither settled nor attacked within the 60 min test period. Whenever an attack was seen, the 213 target was recorded. 214 We carried out pairwise comparisons between different scenes using 2 x 2 contingency 215 tables (chi-square and Fisher exact tests (FET) of independence). Bonferroni adjustments for 216 multiple comparisons were applied and, unless stated otherwise, all results that were significant 217 remained so after adjusting alpha. In all comparisons between scenes there was no significant 218 difference for the number of attacks, so while we have left the data in Table 1, we have omitted 219 attacks from further discussion. We used Kruskal-Wallis tests to analyse the latency to settle in 220 different scenes (Dunn’s multiple comparisons used for pairwise comparisons). 221 222 Results and Discussion 223 9 224 Are test spider decisions influenced by whether the number of Portia already settled close to the 225 nest is one or two and, when two, by how the two are spaced with respect to the nest? 226 Significantly more test spiders settled when there was only one Portia close to the oecobiid- 227 occupied nest (Scene 2) instead of one at each end (Scene 3) (χ2 = 88.62, P < 0.001), two on an arc 228 at one end (Scene 4, χ2 = 48.20, P < 0.001) or two one behind the other at one end (Scene 5, χ2 = 229 41.70, P < 0.001). Instead of a go-for-more rule, test spiders appear to be adopting a go-for-one 230 rule. 231 However, when only one Portia was present (Scene 2), the available space near the nest was 232 greater than when two Portia juveniles were present equidistant from the nest (Scenes 3 & 4). This 233 suggests that our findings might be explained by the limited-space hypothesis (i.e., when deciding 234 whether to settle, test spiders attend to available space instead of the number of already settled 235 conspecific juveniles present). 236 Using the maximum possible distance at which a test spider might position itself from the 237 nearest Portia as an index of available space, the predicted trend from the limited-space hypothesis 238 is that more test spiders will settle with Scene 4 or Scene 5 than with Scene 3. Additionally, the 239 number of test spiders that will settle with Scene 5 will not be significantly different from the 240 number that settle with Scene 2, and more test spiders should settle with Scene 1 than with Scene 2. 241 Yet none of these predictions were corroborated by our findings. The number of test spiders that 242 settled when two Portia were at opposite ends of a nest (Scene 3) was not significantly different 243 from that when the two Portia were on an arc (Scene 4) (χ2 = 0.66, P = 0.417) or lined up at one end 244 (Scene 5) (χ2 = 1.95, P = 0.163). The number that settled with Scene 5, however, was significantly 245 less than the number that settled with Scene 2. Moreover, as shown in the previous study (Jackson 246 and Nelson 2012), more test spiders settled with Scene 2 (one Portia present) than with Scene 1 247 (Portia absent), despite Scene 1 offering more space near the nest. This combination of findings 248 suggests that, when confronted with Scenes 1-5, test spiders did not attend to available space near 10 249 the nest and instead made settling decisions on the basis of the specific number (0, 1 or 2) of Portia 250 at the nest, with one being the favoured number. 251 252 Are test spider decisions influenced by whether the number of Portia close to the nest is two or three 253 and, when three, by how the Portia are spaced with respect to the nest? 254 Significantly fewer test spiders settled when three Portia were evenly spaced around the nest 255 (Scene 8) than when two Portia were spaced maximally apart (Scene 3) (P < 0.001, FET). Fewer 256 test spiders settled when there were three (Scene 9) instead of two (Scene 4) Portia positioned on an 257 arc at one end of the nest (χ2 = 30.54, P < 0.001). However, contrary to the limited-space 258 hypothesis, the number of test spiders that settled when three Portia were evenly spaced around the 259 nest (Scene 8) was not significantly different from how many settled when three Portia were on an 260 arc at one end of the nest (Scene 9) (P = 0.053, FET). 261 When Portia were lined up at one end of the nest, the number of test spiders that settled 262 when two were present (Scene 5) was not significantly different from the number that settled when 263 three were present (Scene 10) (χ2 = 0.40, P = 0.529) (i.e., in this instance, response to two Portia 264 was comparable to response to three). Significantly more test spiders settled when three Portia were 265 lined up (Scene 10) rather than evenly spaced around the nest (Scene 8) (P < 0.001, FET) or on an 266 arc at one end of the nest (Scene 9) (χ2 = 46.02, P < 0.001), despite the number of Portia in each of 267 these scenes being three. 268 These findings are as predicted by the limited-space hypothesis because there is more free 269 space near the nest in Scene 10 than in Scenes 8 or 9. Yet we know from the previous study 270 (Jackson and Nelson 2012) that distance from the nest is important. In Scenes 8 and 9, each Portia 271 was close to the nest, but each Portia in Scene 10 was at a different distance from the nest. If test 272 spiders ignore Portia that are very far from the nest, then Scene 10 would have been perceived as a 273 scene with two, not three, Portia. Other comparisons (see below) allow us to consider this 274 hypothesis. 11 275 276 Are test-spider decisions influenced by the distance between Portia and the nest? 277 Significantly more test spiders settled when two Portia that were at opposite ends of a nest were 278 very far from (Scene 11), instead of close to (Scene 3), the nest (χ2 = 19.48, P < 0.001). 279 Significantly more test spiders settled when three Portia that were spaced evenly around a nest were 280 very far from (Scene 12), instead of close to (Scene 8), the nest (P < 0.001, FET). Likewise, more 281 test spiders settled when three Portia that were on an arc at one end of the nest were very far from 282 (Scene 13), instead of close to, the nest (Scene 9) (χ2 = 55.44, P < 0.001). 283 Evidently, when some Portia are very far from the nest and others are close, only those that 284 are close influence the test spiders’ settling decisions. When three Portia were present, significantly 285 fewer test spiders settled when all three were close to the nest (Scene 9) than when the centrally 286 placed Portia was very far from the nest and the other two Portia were close to the nest (Scene 14) 287 (χ2 = 22.34, P < 0.001). When the Portia on the left and right were very far from nest and the 288 centrally placed Portia was close to the nest (Scene 15), significantly more test spiders settled than 289 when all three were close to the nest (Scene 9) (χ2 =154.60). Similarly, significantly more test 290 spiders settled (χ2 =47.21, P < 0.001) when two of the three Portia were very far away (Scene 15), 291 instead of only one being far away (Scene 14). 292 The number of test spiders that settled when there were no Portia present at a nest (Scene 1) 293 was not different from when three Portia were surrounding, but very far from, the nest (Scene 12) 294 (χ2 = 3.28, P = 0.070) and compared with when there were three Portia on an arc at one end, but 295 very far from, the nest (Scene 13) (χ2 = 0.35, P = 0.552). 296 When there were three Portia, two of which were very far from the nest (Scene 15), test 297 spider decisions were not significantly different from when there was a single Portia close to a nest 298 (Scene 2) (χ2 = 0.87, P = 0.351). Additionally, when there were two Portia close to a nest and a 299 third present but very far from the nest (Scene 14), test spider decisions were not different from 300 when there were only two Portia close to the nest (Scene 4) (χ2 = 0.48, P = 0.487). 12 301 302 This combination of findings implies that, when test spiders are making settling decisions, Portia that are very far from the nest are ignored. 303 304 For test spiders, is distinguishing three from two Portia based on discriminating a triangle from a 305 straight line? 306 When test spiders distinguished Scene 3 from Scene 8 and Scene 4 from Scene 9, they distinguished 307 scenes containing two from scenes containing three Portia. Here we consider whether this might be 308 a distinction between a straight line (defined by two objects) and a triangle (defined by three 309 objects). This more or less corresponds to a hypothesis that, when subitizing, individuals pre- 310 attentively discriminate two from three objects not by representing number per se but instead by 311 identifying canonical configurations, one object corresponding to a dot, two to a straight line and 312 three to a triangle (Mandler and Shebo 1982). This hypothesis suggests that three objects on a 313 horizontal line will not be distinguished from two objects (see Trick and Pylyshyn 1994), yet the 314 number of test spiders that settled was significantly more when there were only two Portia (Scene 315 4) rather than three Portia in a horizontal line (Scene 16) (P < 0.001, FET). Additionally, test-spider 316 settling decisions when three Portia were in a horizontal line (Scene 16) were not significantly 317 different from test-spider decisions when the scene was otherwise identical except that the three 318 Portia were positioned on an arc, thereby defining three points on a virtual triangle (Scene 9) (P = 319 0.204, FET). 320 Perhaps the canonical-configuration hypothesis can be saved by proposing that, while not 321 defining an actual triangle, three objects in a straight line at least defines a potential for a triangle 322 (see Dehaene 1992; Trick and Pylyshyn 1994), but the more straightforward conclusion is that the 323 mechanism underlying Portia’s discrimination between scenes with three versus two already settled 324 Portia is based on information pertaining to numbers. 325 13 326 Are the test-spider decisions influenced by the orientation of the Portia that is already settled at the 327 nest? 328 In the previous study (Jackson and Nelson 2012), where there was never more than one Portia at 329 the nest, test spider settling decisions varied depending on the orientation of the Portia at the nest. 330 Attending to orientation appears to be an additional level of information to process when making 331 settling decisions. Here we investigated whether test spiders use this additional information when 332 confronted by a scene in which more than one Portia is present. 333 Test-spider decisions when two (Scene 17) or three (Scene 18) Portia were facing away 334 from the nest were not significantly different from when two (Scene 3) or three (Scene 8) Portia 335 were facing the nest (two: χ2 =1.13, P = 0.287; three: P = 0.667, FET). These findings suggest that 336 orientation, which matters when only one Portia is seen at a nest, is ignored when there are two 337 Portia at a nest. We might account for this difference by proposing that Portia is subject to capacity 338 limitations that prevent processing information about orientation at the same time as processing 339 information about number, but other comparisons (see below) raise doubts about capacity 340 limitations being this severe. An alternative hypothesis, which seems more likely, is that test spiders 341 are more strongly motivated to process information about the orientation of already settled Portia 342 when the preferred number of them (one) is present and less strongly motivated when the number is 343 two. 344 345 Besides being influenced by seeing Portia at a nest, are test spiders influenced by seeing and 346 identifying the nest inhabitant? 347 Test spiders are more inclined to settle when they see specifically an oecobiid in the nest (Jackson 348 and Nelson 2012). Under the added load of representing object identity, we might expect numerical 349 competence to suffer (see Schmitt and Fischer 2011), yet we found that the identity of the spider in 350 the nest influenced settling decisions even when two Portia were present. Significantly more test 351 spiders settled when the nest was occupied by an oecobiid (Scene 3) than when the nest was 14 352 unoccupied (Scene 6) (χ2 = 9.00, P =0.003) or occupied by a Portia juvenile instead of an oecobiid 353 (Scene 7) (P < 0.001, FET). 354 355 Are test spider decisions influenced by the size of the already settled Portia? 356 Animals have considerable ability to make decisions on the basis of perceived quantities expressed 357 as continuous variables instead of numbers (e.g., Rodríguez and Gamboa 2000; Beran 2010; 358 Krusche et al. 2010; Rodríguez and Gloudeman 2011), and one of the challenges in research on 359 numerical competence is to minimize the influence of confounding continuous variables (Lipton 360 and Speke 2003; Brannon et al. 2004; Hurewitz et al. 2006; Beran et al. 2008). Communal 361 predation on oecobiids occurs only among Portia juveniles within a specific size range (Jackson et 362 al. 2008), enabling us to test for the influence of a continuous variable (Portia stuff) on spider 363 settling decisions by varying the size of already settled Portia over this range. 364 We found that, when there was one Portia in a scene, the number of test spiders that settled 365 were not significantly different depending on whether the Portia was large (Scene 19) or small 366 (Scene 20) (χ2 = 0.51, P =0.476). This was also the case when there were two (Scene 21 vs. Scene 367 22: χ2 = 0.11, P = 0.744) or three (Scene 23 vs. Scene 24: none settled) Portia at the scene. These 368 data suggest that the number of Portia at a scene, not continuous variables, affected decisions. 369 370 Does Portia estimate or subitize? 371 According to the accumulator model we would predict that the number of already settled Portia at a 372 scene affect the latency to settle. In fact, we did find a significant positive relationship between the 373 number of already settled spiders at a scene and latency to settle (H20 = 37.24, P = 0.011), but 374 where we would predict specific effects, for example between Scenes 2, 4 and 9, we found instead 375 that the median times to settle (Table 1) did not differ. 376 377 Being accurate within the range 0-3, Portia’s settling decisions might suggest subitizing, as this mechanism typically has an upper limit of 3 or 4 (Cowan 2010). However, Portia’s settling 15 378 decisions are also based on considerable classifying of objects in addition to number, such as the 379 distinction between oecobiids and conspecific individuals (even when seen through nest silk), 380 orientation and distance of the already settled Portia with respect to the nest, and nest occupancy. It 381 has been argued that the attention required for extensive classifying is incompatible with subitizing 382 (Treisman and Gelade 1980; Tomonaga 2008; Xu and Chun 2009), which seems to operate at mid- 383 level visual processing (Butterworth 1999, 2008) that functions primarily in individuating items and 384 assigning them to object files (Pylyshyn 1989, 2001) pre-attentively or without making heavy 385 demands on attention (Piazza et al. 2002, 2011; Railo et al. 2008; Burr et al. 2010). 386 387 Does Portia count? 388 When orientation to and distance from the nest were most effectively positioned, as previously 389 determined (Jackson and Nelson 2012), we found different numbers of Portia to be ranked by test 390 spiders when eliciting settling. The strongest stimulus was a single Portia, there being a sharp drop 391 in the number of test spiders that settled when no Portia were present (zero) and in the presence of 392 one more Portia (two). The fewest test spiders settled when there were three Portia present, 393 although it might be more accurate to say ‘more than two’ instead of ‘three’ as we have no evidence 394 to consider whether three is distinguished from four or any other number larger than three. 395 The rationale for the design of our experiments was an understanding of a specific context in 396 which a particular predator deploys a specialized communal predatory strategy. Each individual 397 Portia was tested only once, with no individual having prior experience with the prey or the 398 experimental set up. These methods were appropriate for our objective of determining whether 399 specific numbers are innately salient to this predator based on its specific predatory strategy. This is 400 rather different from typical objectives in the literature on the numerical competence of animals, 401 which aim to test more general-purpose number-based decisions, such as go-for-more rules (Beran 402 2008; Beran et al. 2008). 16 403 Number appears to be a salient cue when Portia makes settling decisions, but there is no 404 basis for proposing that Portia’s decisions are based on anything especially close to true counting. 405 For example, true counting is based on representing numbers as properties of sets that stay the same 406 even when the identity of the particular objects in a set change, but the expression of numerical 407 competence we have investigated seems to be linked tightly to objects with highly specific 408 characteristics (i.e., other Portia already settled at an oecobiid nest). 409 410 Does Portia use prototype-matching when making number-related decisions? 411 Although numerical competence appears to be important in Portia’s communal predatory strategy, 412 variables related to number appear to be one of an assortment of variables that influence settling 413 decisions. Objects belonging to three categories seem to be identified, a nest and two spider 414 categories: the prey (the oecobiid) and other Portia, and how these objects are arranged with respect 415 to each other is also pertinent. We can not rule out that Portia’s numerical competence is expressed 416 in the context of classifying scenes, with Portia’s settling decisions being based on how closely the 417 scene being viewed matches a prototype scene. 418 Prototype-matching is better known as a mechanism by which individuals assign items to 419 non-numerical categories (Rosch 1975; Rosch et al. 1976) and it is in the context of predatory 420 versatility (Curio 1976) that non-numerical categories have been considered in salticid research. 421 Versatile predators adopt conditional predatory strategies, with each individual deploying different 422 capture tactics depending on prey type. By adopting different behaviour in encounters with different 423 prey, these predators reveal the salient prey categories underlying their classification schemes. 424 Species from the genus Portia have a large repertoire of innate prey-specific capture tactics (Nelson 425 and Jackson 2011) possibly based on matching prey exemplars against a collection of prototypes 426 (Jackson and Cross 2011). 427 428 In the context of communal predation by Portia on oecobiids, we may have evidence of an innately salient category, but in this instance the most relevant exemplars and prototype may be 17 429 scenes instead of individual prey items or individual prey categories. That Portia will decide to 430 settle may become increasingly more likely as exemplar-prototype resemblance becomes closer, 431 with the prototype being defined in part by a number-related factor. 432 Prototype-matching as a mechanism underlying numerical competence has been proposed 433 before (Terrell and Thomas 1990), yet is rarely considered. Like subitizing and estimating, this 434 mechanism does not require that the animal has access to representations of specific numbers in the 435 full abstract meaning of true numbers, but it does imply that information specific to a number rather 436 than, for example, a continuous variable, is salient. For Portia, numerical competence in the context 437 of preying on oecobiids may be a consequence of this predator adopting, as part of a conditional 438 predatory strategy, a highly specialized prototype-matching routine at the level of scenes instead of 439 only at the level of individual prey items. 440 18 441 Acknowledgements 442 Godfrey Otieno Sune, Stephen Aluoch and Jane Atieno Obonyo provided invaluable technical 443 assistance. We acknowledge financial support from the National Geographic Society (RRJ), the 444 Royal Society of New Zealand (Marsden Fund; RRJ; James Cook Fellowship, RRJ). 445 446 References 447 Agrillo C, Dadda M, Serena G (2008) Do fish count? Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in 448 449 450 451 452 female mosquitofish. Anim Cogn 11:495-503 Anderson DE, Vogel EK, Awh E (2011) Precision in visual working memory reaches a stable plateau when individual item limits are exceeded. J Neurosci 31:1128-1138 Awh E, Barton B, Vogel EK (2007) Visual working memory represents a fixed number of items regardless of complexity. Psychol Sci 18:622-628 453 Baddeley A (1986) Working memory. Oxford University Press, Oxford 454 Baddeley A (2003) Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:829- 455 456 839 Benson-Amram S, Heinen VK, Dryer SL, Holekamp KE (2011) Numerical assessment and 457 individual call discrimination by wild spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Anim Behav 458 82:743-752 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 Beran MJ (2001) Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115:181-191 Beran MJ (2008) Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) succeed in a test of quantity conservation. Anim Cogn 11:109-116 Beran MJ (2010) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) accurately compare poured liquid quantities. Anim Cogn 13:641-649 Beran MJ, Evans TA, Harris EH (2008) Perception of food amounts by chimpanzees based on the number, size, contour length and visibility of items. Anim Behav 75:1793-1802 19 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 Boysen ST, Berntson GG (1989) Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 103:23-31 Boysen ST, Berntson GG (1995) Responses to quantity: Perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process 21:82-86 Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA (2011) A review of visual memory capacity: beyond individual items and toward structured representations. J Vis 11:1-34 Brannon EM (2002) The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy. Cognition 83:223-240 475 Brannon EM (2006) The representation of numerical magnitude. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:222-229 476 Brannon EM, Van de Walle G (2001) The development of ordinal numerical competence in young 477 478 479 480 481 children. Cogn Psychol 43:53-81 Brannon EM, Abbott S, Lutz DJ (2004) Number bias for the discrimination of large visual sets in infancy. Cognition 93:B59-B68 Breukelaar JWC, Dalrymple-Alford JC (1998) Timing ability and numerical competence in rats. J Exp Psychol 24:84-97 482 Burr DC, Ross J (2008) A visual sense of number. Curr Biol 18:425-428 483 Burr DC, Turi M, Anobile G (2010) Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional 484 resources. J Vis 10(6):1-10 485 Butterworth B (1999) A head for figures. Science 284:928-929 486 Butterworth B (2008) Numerosity perception: how many speckles on the hen? Curr Biol 16:R388- 487 389 488 Cantlon JF, Brannon EM (2007) Basic math in monkeys and college students. PLoS ONE 5:e328 489 Carazo P, Font E, Forteza-Behrendt E (2009) Quantity discrimination in Tenebrio molitor: evidence 490 of numerosity discrimination in an invertebrate? Anim Cogn 12:463-470 491 Carey S (1998) Knowledge of number: its evolution and ontogeny. Science 282:641-642 20 492 493 Carey S (2001) Cognitive foundations of arithmetic: evolution and ontogenesis. Mind Lang 16:3755 494 Castelli F, Glaser DE, Butterworth B (2006) Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the 495 parietal lobe: a functional MRI study. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 103:4693-4698 496 Charnov EL (1976) Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theor Popul Biol 9:129-136 497 Chittka L, Geiger K (1995) Can honey-bees count landmarks? Anim Behav 49:159-164 498 Church RM, Broadbent HA (1990) Alternative representations of time, number, and rate. Cognition 499 500 501 37:55-81 Clearfield MW, Mix KS (1999) Number versus contour length in infants’ discrimination of small visual sets. Psychol Sci 10:408-411 502 Cowan N (1998) Visual and auditory working memory capacity. Trends Cogn Sci 2:77-78 503 Cowan N (2000) The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage 504 505 506 capacity. Behav Brain Sci 24:87-185 Cowan N (2010) The magical mystery four: how is working memory capacity limited, and why? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19:51-57 507 Curio E (1976) The Ethology of Predation. Springer-Verlag, Berlin 508 Dacke M, Srinivasan MV (2008) Evidence for counting in insects. Anim Cogn 11:683-689 509 Dadda M, Piffer L, Agrillo C, Biazza A (2009) Spontaneous number representation in mosquitofish. 510 511 512 513 514 Cognition 112:343-348 Davis H, Memmott J (1982) Counting behavior in animals: a critical evaluation. Psychol Bull 92:547-571 Davis H, MacKenzie KA, Morrison S (1989) Numerical discrimination by rats (Rattus norvegicus) using body and vibrissal touch. J Comp Psychol 103:45-53 515 Dehaene S (1992) Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition 44:1-42 516 Dehaene S (2001) Precis of the number sense. Mind Lang 16: 16-36 517 Feigenson L, Dehaene S, Spelke E (2004) Core systems of number. Trends Cogn Sci 8:307-314 21 518 519 520 521 522 Flombaum JI, Junge JA, Hauser MD (2005) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers. Cognition 97:315-325 Fougnie D, Marois R (2011) Distinct capacity limits for attention and working memory. Psychol Sci 17:526-534 Franks NR, Dornhaus A, Metherell BG, Nelson TR, Lanfear SAJ, Symes WS (2006) Not 523 everything that counts can be counted: ants use multiple metrics for a single nest trait. Proc 524 R Soc B 273:165-169 525 526 527 528 Gallistel CR, Gelman R (2000) Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends Cogn Sci 4:59-65 Gelman R, Gallistel CR (1978) The child’s understanding of number. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA 529 Giaquinto M (2001) What cognitive systems underlie arithmetical abilities? Mind Lang 16:56-68 530 Gross HJ, Pahl M, Si A, Zhu H, Tautz J, Zhang S (2009) Number-based visual generalisation in the 531 532 533 534 honeybee. PLoS ONE 4(1): e4263 Hager MC, Helfman GS (1991) Safety in numbers: shoal size choice by minnows under predatory threat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 29:271-276 Hanus D, Call J (2007) Discrete quantity judgments in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan 535 troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus): the effect of presenting whole sets versus 536 item-by-item. J Comp Psychol 121:241-249 537 Harland DP, Jackson RR (2001) Prey classification by Portia fimbriata, a salticid spider that 538 specializes at preying on other salticids: species that elicit cryptic stalking. J Zool 255:445- 539 460 540 541 Haun DBM, Jordan FM, Vallortigara G, Clayton NS (2010) Origins of spatial, temporal and numerical cognition: Insights from comparative psychology. Trends Cog Sci 14:552-560 22 542 Hauser MD, Carey S (1999) Building a cognitive creature from a set of primitives: evolutionary and 543 developmental insights. In: Allen C, Cummins D (eds.) The Evolution of Mind. Oxford 544 University Press, Oxford 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 Hauser MD, Carey S (2003) Spontaneous representations of small numbers of objects by rhesus macaques: examination of content and format. Cognit Psychol 47:367-491 Hoare DJ, Couzin ID, Godin GJ, Krause J (2004) Context-dependent group size choice in fish. Anim Behav 67:155-164 Hunt S, Low J, Burns KC (2008) Adaptive numerical competency in a food-hoarding songbird. Proc R Soc B 275:2373-2379 Hurewitz F, Gelman R, Schnitzer B (2006) Sometimes area counts more than number. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 103:19599-19604 553 Jackson RR, Cross FR (2011) Spider cognition. Adv Insect Physiol 41:115-174 554 Jackson RR & Nelson XJ (2012) Attending to detail by communal spider-eating spiders. Anim 555 556 557 558 Cogn In Press. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0469-y Jackson RR, Pollard SD, Salm K (2008) Observations of Portia africana, an araneophagic jumping spider, living together and sharing prey. N Z J Zool 35:237-242 Jakob EM, Christa D, Skow CD, Long S (2011) Plasticity, learning and cognition. In: Herberstein 559 ME (ed) Spider behaviour: flexibility and versatility. Cambridge University Press, New York, 560 pp. 307-347 561 562 563 564 Kahneman D, Treisman A, Gibbs BJ (1992) The reviewing of object files: object-specific integration of information. Cognit Psychol 24:175-219 Kaufman EL, Lord MW, Reese TW, Volkmann J (1949) The discrimination of visual number. Am J Psychol 62:498-525 565 Kitchen DM (2006) Alpha male black howler monkey responses to loud calls: effect of numeric 566 odds, male companion behaviour and reproductive investment. Anim Behav 67:125-139 23 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 Kobayashi T, Hiraki K, Mugitani R, Hasegawa T (2004) Baby arithmetic: one object plus one tone. Cognition 91:B23-B34 Krusche P, Uller C, Dicke U (2010) Quantity discrimination in salamanders. J Exp Biol 213:18221828 Le Corre M, Carey S (2008) Why the verbal counting principles are constructed out of representations of small sets of individuals: a reply to Gallistel. Cognition 107:650-662 Lipton JS, Spelke ES (2003) Origins of number sense: large-number discrimination in human infants. Psychol Sci 14:396-401 Lyon BE (2003) Egg recognition and counting reduce costs of avian conspecific brood parasitism. Nature 422:495-499 Mandler G, Shebo BJ (1982) Subitizing: an analysis of its conceptual processes. J Exp Psychol 111:1-22 579 Matsuzawa T (1985) Use of numbers by a chimpanzee. Nature 315:57-59 580 McComb K, Packer C, Pusey A (1994) Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between 581 582 583 584 585 586 groups of lions, Panthera leo. Anim Behav 47:379-387 McCrink K, Wynn K (2004) Large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants. Psychol Sci 15:776-781 Meck WH, Church RM (1983) A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:320-334 Nelson XJ, Jackson RR (2011) Flexibility in the foraging strategies of spiders In: Herberstein ME 587 (ed) Spider behaviour: flexibility and versatility. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 588 31-56 589 Owen AM (2004) Working memory: imaging the magic number four. Curr Biol 14:R573-R574 590 Pepperberg IM (2006) Grey parrot numerical competence: a review. Anim Cogn 9:377-391 591 Piazza M, Fumarola A, Chinello A, Melcher D (2011) Subitizing reflects visuo-spatial object 592 individuation capacity. Cognition 121:147-153 24 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 Piazza M, Mechelli A, Butterworth B, Price CJ (2002) Are subitizing and counting implemented as separate or functionally overlapping processes? NeuroImage 15:435-446 Pylyshyn ZW (1989) The role of location indexes in spatial perception: a sketch of the FINST spatial-index model. Cognition 32:65-97 Pylyshyn ZW (2001) Visual indexes, preconceptual objects, and situated vision. Cognition 80:127158 Pyke GH, Pulliam HR, Charnov EL (1977) Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests. Q Rev Biol 52:137-154 Railo H, Koivisto M, Revonsuo A, Hannula MM (2008) The role of attention in subitizing. Cognition 107:82-104 Reznikova Z, Ryabko B (2011) Numerical competence in animals, with an insight from ants. Behaviour 148:405-434 Riggs KJ, Ferrand L, Lancelin D, Fryziel L, Dumur G (2006) Subitizing in tactile perception. Psychol Sci 17:271-272 Rodríguez RL, Gamboa E (2000) Memory of captured prey in three web spiders (Araneae: Araneidae, Linyphiidae, Tetragnathidae). Anim Cogn 3:91-97 609 Rodríguez RL, Gloudeman MD (2011) Estimating the repeatability of memories of captured prey 610 formed by Frontinella communis spiders (Araneae: Linyphiidae). Anim Cogn 14:675-682 611 Rosch E (1975) Cognitive representations of semantic categories. J Exp Psychol 104:192-233 612 Rosch E, Mervis CB, Greay WD, Jonhson DM, Boyes-Braem P (1976) Basic objects in natural 613 614 615 616 617 categories. Cognit Psychol 8:382-429 Rugani R, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2007) Rudimental numerical competence in 5-day-old domestic chicks (Gallus gallus): identification of ordinal position. J Exp Psychol 33:21-31 Rugani R, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2010) Imprinted numbers: newborn chicks’ sensitivity to number vs. continuous extent of objects they have been reared with. Dev Sci 13:790-797 25 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 Rugani R, Fontanari L, Simoni E, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2009) Arithmetic in newborn chicks Proc R Soc B 276:2451-2460 Schmitt, V., Fischer, J 2011. Representational format determines numerical competence in monkeys. Nature commun 2, 257. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1262 Shettleworth SJ (2009) Cognition, Evolution, and Behaviour. 2nd edition. Oxford University Press, New York Seron X, Pesenti M (2001) The number sense theory needs more empirical evidence. Mind Lang 16:76-88 Slaughter V, Itakura S, Kutsuki A, Siegal M (2011) Learning to count begins in infancy: evidence from 18 month olds’ visual preferences. Proc R Soc B 278:2979-2984 Starkey P, Spelke ES, Gelman R (1983) Detection of intermodal numerical correspondences by human infants. Science 222:179-181 630 Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1986) Foraging Theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton 631 Tardif SD, Layne DG, Smucny DA (2002) Can marmoset mothers count to three? Effect of litter 632 size on mother-infant interactions. Ethology 108:825-836 633 Terrell DF, Thomas RK (1990) Number-related discrimination and summation by squirrel monkeys 634 (Saimiri sciureus sciureus and S. boliviensus boliviensus) on the basis of the number of sides 635 of polygons. J Comp Psychol 104:238-247 636 Thomas RK, Simmons LW (2010) Cuticular hydrocarbons influence female attractiveness to males 637 in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus. J Evol Biol 23:707-714 638 Tomonaga M (2008) Relative numerosity discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): 639 evidence for approximate numerical representations. Anim Cogn 11:43-57 640 Treisman AM, Gelade G (1980) A feature integration theory of attention Cogn Psychol 12:97-136 641 Trick LM (2005) The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: patterns of selective 642 interference in subitizing and counting. Psychon Bull Rev 12:675-681 26 643 644 645 646 647 Trick LM, Pylyshyn ZW (1994) Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychol Rev 101:80-102 Uller C, Hauser MD, Carey S (2001) Spontaneous representation of number in cotton-top tamarins. J Comp Psychol 115:1-10 Uller C, Jaeger R, Guidry G, Martin C (2003) Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more: 648 rudiments of number in a species of basal vertebrate. Anim Cogn 6:105-112 649 Wesley F (1961) The number concept: a phylogenetic review. Psychol Bull 53: 420-428 650 Wood JN, Hauser MC, Glynn DD, Barner D (2007) Free-ranging rhesus monkeys spontaneously 651 individuate and enumerate small numbers of non-solid portions. Cognition 106:207-221 652 Wynn K (1992) Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature 358:749-750 653 Wynn K (1998) Psychological foundations of number: numerical competence in human infants. 654 655 656 657 Trends Cogn Sci 2:296-303 Xu Y, Chun MM (2009) Selecting and perceiving multiple visual objects. Trends Cogn Sci 13:167174 Xu Y, Spelke ES (2000) Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition 74:B1-11 658 659 660 27 Table 1 Data from testing Portia africana juveniles with ‘scenes’ made from lures (dead spiders mounted in lifelike posture) and silken nests. See text for details. Radiating lines: nest. Black polygon: lure made from adult Oecobius amboseli (Oa) female (always in nest). Grey polygon: lure made from Portia africana (Pa) juvenile (body length 2mm, unless stated otherwise). White circles (‘eyes’) represent direction in which lure faced. When more than one lure was in a scene the centre-to-centre distances of all lures was 8 mm unless stated otherwise. Q1 and Q3: 1st and 3rd quartile, respectively. 1 Data from Jackson and Nelson (In Press). *N = 200; all other N = 100. Scene Scene description No. Diagram Attacked Attacked Settled Median latency to settle nest (N) already settled (N) (min) (Q1, Q3) spider (N) 11* Oa in nest 9 na 75 22 (13, 31) 21* Pa and Oa facing each other 1 0 131 26 (15, 36) 3* Two Pa at opposite ends of nest, one facing Oa 5 2 38 25 (14.5, 29) head-on and other facing rear of Oa 28 4 Two Pa side-by-side in front of and facing Oa 4 0 23 20 (10, 40) 5 Two Pa lined up one behind the other at one end 1 2 26 28.5 (18.75, 47.5) 0 0 6 22.5 (16.25, 34.25) 0 0 4 23.5 (16.75, 30.25) 0 2 1 39 (39, 39) 3 0 6 21.5 (13.5, 26.25) of nest. Pa head-on to Oa 6 Two Pa. Each Pa at opposite end of nest. Both facing empty nest 7 Two Pa at opposite ends of a nest. Both facing nest. Pa in nest 8* Three Pa surrounding nest, forming triangle with Oa at centre. Each Pa facing Oa. Pa in lower position head-on to Oa 9* Three Pa side-by-side in front of and facing Oa 29 10 Three Pa lined up one behind the other. Pa head- 4 0 30 18 (11.5, 34.25) 2 0 43 28 (24, 36) 5 0 27 28 (21, 36) 5 0 34 28 (16.5, 39.25) on to Oa. 1st Pa juvenile close to, 2nd far & 3rd very far from nest 11 Oa and one Pa facing each other. Other Pa opposite, facing rear of Oa. Both Pa very far from nest 12 Three Pa surrounding nest, forming triangle with Oa at centre. Pa in lower position facing Oa head-on. All Pa very far from nest 13 Three Pa side-by-side facing Oa (one head-on). All Pa very far from nest 30 14 Three Pa side-by-side facing Oa. One Pa head- 1 0 19 31 (26, 40) 0 0 122 29 (22, 36.25) 1 2 1 43 (43, 43) 0 3 30 27 (18.5, 36.25) 2 2 0 na on to Oa, very far from nest and positioned between other two Pa. Each of other two Pa close to nest 15* Three Pa side-by-side facing Oa. One Pa headon to Oa, close to nest and positioned between other two Pa which are very far from nest 16 Three Pa directly to the side of another facing Oa (one head-on) 17* Two Pa at opposite end of nest, both facing away from Oa 18 Three Pa surrounding nest. Each Pa facing away from Oa 31 19 Pa (large) and Oa, facing each other 0 0 59 25 (16, 31) 20 Pa (small) and Oa, facing each other 2 0 54 24 (17, 33.75) 21 Two large Pa at opposite end of nest, one facing 0 0 24 27.5 (22.5, 35) 1 1 27 31 (19, 36) 0 1 0 na Oa head-on and other facing rear of Oa 22 Two small Pa at opposite end of nest, one facing Oa head-on and other facing rear of Oa 23 Three large Pa surrounding nest, forming triangle with Oa at centre. Pa in lower position head-on to Oa 32 24 Three small Pa surrounding nest, forming 0 2 0 na triangle with Oa at centre. Each Pa facing Oa. Pa in lower position head-on to Oa 33