Two Faces of Causality: A Small Case Study of the Admission of

advertisement
UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND
COLLEGE OF INFORMATION STUDIES
Two Faces of Causality: A Small Case Study of the Admission of
Scientific Evidence to Show Causality in a Bias and a Toxic Tort
Case in the 4th Circuit
Christina Kirk Pikas
LBSC 735: Legal Issues in Managing Information
Fall 2002
Due: December 11, 2002
Introduction
Over the past 10 years and many complaints of junk, voodoo, and bad science in court,
multiple efforts have been made to correct the way scientific evidence is handled in the federal
court system.1 Three significant Supreme Court rulings, new and revised federal rules of
evidence, and academic study have provided varied tools for triers of fact to employ to sort
through the evidence. First, the Daubert ruling in 1993 created a framework for trial judges to
use to evaluate the reliability, validity, and fit in accordance with the Federal Rules of Evidence.
Then, the Supreme Court ruled on two cases regarding the application of Daubert: Joiner
covered appeals and Kumho answered controversy about what types of expert testimony Daubert
covers.
The fourth circuit has heard about twenty-five cases in the past two years alone that hinged
on testimony included or excluded based on the Daubert criteria.2 These experts included
doctors, thermodynamics professors, maintenance supervisors, and several psychologists. The
judges are not expected to know all of these fields, but they must be able to assess the quality of
the evidence.
This paper reviews the efforts made to reform the handling of scientific evidence and several
areas of science and statistics that give judges the most difficulty: epidemiology, toxicology, and
multiple regression analysis. Specifically, this paper explores how the Daubert treatment of
scientific evidence influenced the resolution of two cases in the fourth circuit in which statistical
methods or scientific evidence were employed to show causality. Some of the scientific methods
used in court cases are discussed, causality is defined, and two diverse cases are studied in more
detail to demonstrate the application of the Daubert standards and statistical methods.
Background
Over the past century, scientific information has become more and more a part of the legal
system -- especially the civil tort system. Over this time, it has become clear that lawyers,
1
According to Robert Park in Voodoo Science: The Road from Foolishness to Fraud (New York: Oxford
University Press, 2000), 9, there are three types of Voodoo Science: pathological (scientists fool themselves), junk
(scientists craft arguments to fool or confuse), and pseudoscience (like magic and aliens – the language of science
without any actual science).
2
Peter Nordberg, “Fourth Circuit,” Daubert on the Web, November 20, 2002, available on the internet at
http://www.daubertontheweb.com/fourth_circuit.htm (accessed 11/21/02).
Pikas 2
judges, and juries for the most part do not have much science background and are therefore only
minimally able to determine good science from bad. This generated valid concerns that junk
science was winning large settlements. In addition to the increase in hard scientific evidence and
expert testimony in the courts, there is an increased introduction of social science, medical,
toxicological, and epidemiological experts and evidence. This information, too, relies on
complicated statistics and statistical methods. Some examples of cases relying on complicated
statistics dealt with employment discrimination, voter’s rights, product liability, census counting,
and patent infringement damages.
A series of decisions and rules since 1923 have combined to redefine the role of the judge as
gatekeeper and to define what considerations she must make prior to admitting scientific
evidence. The first of these decisions came from the Frye murder trial.
Frye
The first ruling that set a standard for admitting scientific evidence was the Frye decision in
1923. The discussion was over the admission of an early precursor to the lie detector test. Prior
to this ruling, experts were allowed to testify if they had knowledge beyond that of the average
juror and expertise was assumed if the expert was commercially successful in the field.3 The key
statement is
while courts will go a long way in admitting expert testimony deduced from a
well-recognized scientific principle or discovery, the thing from which the
deduction is made must be sufficiently established to have gained general
acceptance in the particular field in which it belongs.4
There are strengths and weaknesses to this approach. First, it places the responsibility for
ensuring good science back in the hands of the scientists perhaps best able to judge the
methodology and findings. Second, this criteria is fairly simple to apply and can be applied
without learning the relevant background material. Detractors point out that this excludes all
novel science and that general acceptance is less common in “rigorous fields with the healthiest
scientific discourse.”5
3
David L Faigman, David H. Kaye, and Michael J. Saks, "New Directions in Expert Testimony: Scientific,
Technical, and Other Specialized Knowledge Evidence in Federal and State Courts," American Law Institute American Bar Association Continuing Legal Education ALI-ABA Course of Study, April 26, 2001, Available via
Westlaw at http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com. (accessed 12/5/2002), §1-2.1
4
Daubert v Merrill54 App. C. C., at 47, 293 F., at 1014 as quoted in Kenneth R. Foster and Peter W.
Huber, Judging Science: Scientific Knowledge and the Federal Courts ( Cambridge MA: MIT Press, 1997), 280.
5
Faigman, Kaye, and Saks §1-2.4.
Pikas 3
Daubert
The Daubert case, William Daubert, et al. v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. (509 U.S.
579, 1993), involved the charge that the Merrell Dow product Benedictin was a human teratogen
responsible for the plaintiffs’ birth defects.6 Both sides submitted scientific evidence. The
plaintiff’s experts did a meta-analysis on published studies and also produced toxicological
evidence of the effect of large doses in laboratory animals. The defendant produced
epidemiological studies. The toxicological information was not admitted because the court
stated that only epidemiological studies showed causation. The judge did not admit the metaanalysis he because determined that it was not a “generally accepted” method and the specific
analysis had not received peer review.7
Certiorari was granted because of disagreements between courts “regarding the proper
standard for the admission of expert testimony.”8 Fifty years after the Frye decision discussed
above, the Federal Judicial Center issued the first edition of the Federal Rules of Evidence.
While very general, these provided general guidelines for admitting testimony and evidence.
Specifically, Rule 702, Testimony by Experts, allows testimony by a witness qualified as an
expert by “knowledge, skill, experience, training, or education” if the testimony will help the
court understand the evidence or determine a fact.9 Some courts in this time period applied the
Frye rule and others the rules of evidence.
Many amicus briefs were written including ones from the federal government, commercial
associations, legal foundations, and interested scientists, doctors, and researchers.10 One group
argued that junk science could lead to inappropriate liability others argued in turn that judges,
juries, and scientists do not understand scientific testimony and can’t be responsible for
determining its validity.11
6
See Foster and Huber , 277, for a reprint of the opinion.
7
Ibid, 278.
8
Ibid, 279.
9
Federal Rules of Evidence, 2001, http://www2.law.cornell.edu/cgibin/foliocgi.exe/fre/query=[jump!3A!27rule103!27]/doc/{t212}?, (12/4/2002), Rule 702.
Howard H. Kaufman, “The Expert Witness. Neither Frey [sic] nor Daubert Solved the Problem. What
Can Be Done?” International Review of Law Computers & Technology v15 n1 (March 2001), available via EBSCO
MasterFile Premier at http://www.sailor.lib.md.us/cgi-bin/ebsco, (accessed 10/15/2002), 87-8
10
11
Ibid, 88.
Pikas 4
The Daubert decision ushered in a new era in scientific evidence because it codified the role
of the judge as gatekeeper in lieu of other scientists. The assumption was that the judge is better
able to assess the reliability, validity, and helpfulness of scientific evidence than the jury and so
should review the scientific evidence in pre-trial hearings and admit only the experts and
evidence that might meet the following flexible guidelines:
1.
2.
3.
4.
whether it can be (and has been) tested
whether it has been subjected to peer review and publication
the known or potential rate of error
the existence and maintenance of standards controlling the
technique’s operation
5. general acceptance12
Joiner and Kumho
Soon after Daubert the Supreme court agreed to hear a case to determine the standard for an
appellate court to apply in reviewing a district court’s Daubert decision.13 In General Electric
Co. v. Joiner14 the plaintiff was a smoker with a history of cancer in his family. At his
workplace, he was exposed to PCBs that he claimed led to his cancer. The trial court excluded
the plaintiff’s experts after applying the Daubert criteria and granted a summary judgment for
the defendant. The Supreme Court reviewed the evidence and affirmed that the evidence was
properly excluded. The important results were first that abuse of discretion is what the appeals
court should examine in Daubert cases and second that the focus should be on the methodology
not the conclusions, which could conflict. According to Park in his book on Voodoo Science,
Joiner strengthened Daubert by making it so that “not only must evidence be obtained by
scientifically valid procedures, it must also be scientifically interpreted.”15
In the four years immediately following Daubert, controversy developed over exactly what
evidence the ruling covers. In Kumho Tire v. Carmichael,16 the plaintiff sued the tire company
for product liability because a blown minivan tire caused an accident in which there was a
fatality. The Supreme Court held that the gatekeeping requirement extends to all expert
12
Foster and Huber, 284-5.
13
Margaret A. Berger, “The Supreme Court’s Trilogy on the Admissibility of Expert Testimony,” chapter
in Federal Judicial Center, Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence, 2d ed, (St. Paul, MN: West Group, 2000), 13.
14
522 U.S.136-7, 1997.
15
Park, 170.
16
119 S. Ct. 1167, 1999.
Pikas 5
testimony including engineering, psychology, economics, etc. Additionally, no difference is
noted for the expert who relies on book science and one who relies on skills or experience. The
Daubert factors are to be applied flexibly, not all will apply to every situation.
Causality
Toxic Torts and Product Liability
In civil cases, especially toxic torts and product liability cases, the primary reason for the
introduction of an expert or scientific evidence is to prove causality. In other words, the plaintiff
introduces an expert to provide evidence that the defendant’s actions, products, etc., actually
caused the harm to the plaintiff.17 In civil torts relating to property damage caused by an
accident, this evidence is obtained easily. A witness testifies that he saw the accident and there
are pictures of the defendant’s car in the plaintiff’s living room. In toxic torts, on the other hand,
there is no direct evidence that the chemicals resulted in the injury. Likewise, in discrimination
cases there is no simple chain of events or consensus of the end state.
Proving causality is very complicated. In most of the toxic torts, no one really knows if a
given chemical, in a given quantity over a certain time actually caused the harm. Statistics in
conjunction with epidemiology and toxicology make it more or less likely but do not fully prove
the causality. First, the plaintiff has to prove general causality (“is the agent capable of causing
disease?”). 18 In other words, have scientifically valid and reliable studies shown the chemical
more often than not leads to the condition exhibited by the plaintiff? Then the plaintiff has to
prove specific causality (in this case, did the agent cause this particular harm?)
The two major fields used to show causality in toxic torts are epidemiology and toxicology.
Epidemiology is the study of “incidence, distribution, and etiology of disease in human
populations.”19 Controlled randomized trials are preferred because confounding factors and
errors can be minimized; however, since it is unethical to knowingly expose subjects to harm,
most epidemiological studies are observational in nature. Epidemiologists monitor groups over
time who have been exposed to the agent and similar groups who have not been exposed. Error
17
Per Bryan A. Garner, ed., Black's Law Dictionary, 7th ed., (St. Paul, MN: West Group, 1999), 213, to
cause is “to bring about or effect.”
Michael D. Green, D. Michael Freedman, and Leon Gordis, “Reference Guide of Epidemiology,” chapter
in Federal Judicial Center, Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence. 2d ed. (St. Paul, MN: West Group, 2000),
336.
18
19
Ibid, 335.
Pikas 6
occurs as a result of confounding factors, sample error, information bias, and statistical
problems.20 Even in the absence of the errors above, researchers are reluctant to infer causality
but apply the following factors to make a decision:









temporal relationship
strength of the association
doses-response relationship
replication of the findings
biological plausibility (coherence with existing knowledge)
consideration of alternative explanations
cessation of exposure
specificity of the association
consistency with other knowledge.21
Toxicology employs different principles to study the effects of foreign agents on the human
body. Per Goldstein and Henifin in the “Reference Guide on Toxicology,” there are three main
tenets of toxicology: all substances are hazardous to humans depending on dose, each agent
produces a “specific pattern of biological effects that can be used to establish disease causation,”
and the effects on laboratory animals are useful predictors of the effect on humans.22 The results
of toxicological studies are risk assessments and expected effects of given doses of agents. Per
Erica Beecher-Monas, courts are reluctant to accept toxicology studies on animals even though
they are well accepted in the scientific realm and a key problem is that most “animal toxicity
studies are not designed to demonstrate causation but to identify biological mechanisms of
toxicity.”23 The Reference Guide suggests the following criteria to judge specific causal
association between the agent and the plaintiff’s disease:



Was the plaintiff exposed to the substance so that the substance was
absorbed into the body?
Were there other factors present that affected the distribution within the
body?
What is known about the relationship between human metabolism and the
compound?
20
See Darrell Huff, How To Lie With Statistics (New York: Norton, 1982) chp. 1, for a discussion of
sample error and information bias (what subjects report).
21
Quoted from Green, Freedman, and Gordis, 375.
22
Bernard D. Goldstein and Mary Sue Henifin, “Reference Guide on Toxicology,” chapter in Federal
Judicial Center, Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence. 2d ed. (St. Paul, MN: West Group, 2000), 403.
23
Erica Beecher-Monas, "A Ray of Light For Judges Blinded By Science: Triers of Science and
Intellectual Due Process," Georgia Law Review v33 (Summer 1999): n12. Available via Lexis on the internet at
http://www.lexis.com. (accessed 12/7/2002).
Pikas 7



What excretory route does the compound take and how does this affect its
toxicity?
What was the temporal relationship?
Was the exposure at or above the known threshold level?24
Discrimination and Bias
Statistical methods originally developed to study physical phenomena in the hard sciences
are now commonly applied to the social sciences. Legendre and Gauss originally developed
regression methods in the early 1800s to fit astronomical data about the orbits of planets.25 The
scientists carefully measured and knew the errors in their equipment and models. In the social
sciences, the models tend to be somewhat arbitrary and the inclusion or omission of variables is
not always clearly reasoned. In fact, Klock suggests that social scientists sometimes go
relationship shopping—they run statistical tests using the same data with multiple hypotheses
and equations. Whichever fits best must explain the effect.26
In discrimination and cases as applied to hiring, admission, voter redistricting, or census
taking a statistical technique known as multiple regression is used to prove causality and extent.
In other words, the analysis is used to prove that one group was treated unfairly specifically due
to one and only one factor, their age, sex, ethnicity, etc. A multiple regression model is used to
control for variables not related to the factor being tested. An example from the Reference
Manual on Scientific Evidence is of a company trying to determine if there is sex discrimination
in employee salaries. To predict salaries (independent variable) three explanatory variables are
used: experience, education, and a dummy variable (stands for sex, can be either 0 or 1). The
researcher plugs the data into the equation and determines the coefficient for each variable. The
coefficient for the dummy variable should show the variation of the salary between genders.27
Once the multiple regression analysis is complete there are several methods employed to
determine if it is an appropriate model and if the results are practically and statistically
24
Goldstein and Henifin, 422-6.
D. A. Freedman, “From Association to Causation: Some Remarks on the History of Statistics.”
Statistical Science v14 i3 (Aug 1999): 247.
25
26
Mark Klock, “Finding Random Coincidences While Searching For The Holy Writ of Truth:
Specification Searches In Law And Public Policy or Cum Hoc Ergo Propter Hoc?” Wisconsin Law Review i4
(2001), available via Westlaw at http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com, (accessed 11/30/2002), 1010-1.
27
David H. Kaye and David A. Freedman, “Reference Guide on Statistics,” chapter in Federal Judicial
Center, Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence, 2d ed, (St. Paul, MN: West Group, 2000), 145-8.
Pikas 8
significant.28 A first measure to determine if outlying data points are affecting the fit is the
standard deviation for each of the variables. This measures the bell curve in which the majority
should fall; any more than one standard deviation away is noted. For example, if the salary
difference attributed to gender is $1,500 and the standard deviation is $1,600, the difference is
not significant and probably occurred by chance. A next measure is goodness of fit. Two related
calculations should be performed: standard error of regression (standard deviation of the
regression error) and R² (“the percentage of variation in the dependent variable that is accounted
for by all the explanatory variables”).29
Case 1: Nettles v. Proctor & Gamble
The case of Susan Q. Nettles v. Proctor & Gamble Manufacturing Company (33 Fed. Appx.
670; 2002 U.S. App. LEXIS 6953)30 illustrates the principles of causality and the application of
the Daubert trilogy rulings. Ms. Nettles alleged that Proctor & Gamble’s (P&G) Vicks Sinex
Nasal Spray caused her blindness. Specifically, she alleged that the primary ingredient,
oxymetazoline, caused anterior ischemic optic neuropathy. Her case was based on the expert
testimony of a neuro-opthalmologist.
The appeals court determined first that the district court had correctly applied the Daubert
criteria. In pre-trial hearings, the court analyzed the “reasoning and methodology underlying
[the expert’s] opinions.”31 The court determined that there were no toxicological or
epidemiological peer-reviewed articles associating the main ingredient with the plaintiff’s
blindness. Additionally, the court considered the plaintiff’s exposure to the product and stated
that it was minimal (dose-relationship test). The court did not take into account any
toxicological studies or other Daubert factors like testing or general acceptance. The plaintiff’s
only evidence was a temporal connection; therefore the statement in the opinion: “the medical
causation expert has ‘inferred causation’ from a situation-specific occurrence.”32
28
See Foster and Huber chapter 4 (69-109) for a general discussion of errors and Huff chapter 4 (53-9) for
a discussion of significance of error.
29
Rubinfeld, 212-5.
30
Available via Lexis on the internet at http://www.lexis.com. (accessed 9/24/2002).
31
Ibid, opinion.
32
Ibid.
Pikas 9
The court of appeals also cited the Kumho and Joiner decisions in this opinion. First, the
court stated that the lower court acted with sound discretion as required by Joiner and was not
capricious or arbitrary. Furthermore, the appeals court stated that the decision was not an abuse
of discretion as defined in Kumho. The defendant moved for a summary judgment and one was
granted. The appeals court affirmed the decision.
The Nettles decision is a good example of the concepts of the paper. Daubert was correctly
applied: the guidance is flexible so that courts may use a subset of the guidelines as appropriate
to the case. The Kumho and Joiner decisions were applied by the appeals court correctly to
reinforce the lower court’s application of Daubert. The plaintiff’s expert did not, in fact,
produce enough research or evidence to back up the claim.
Case 2: Smith, Degenaro, Belloni, Rimler, Rosenbaum, On behalf of
themselves and all others similarly situated v. Virginia Commonwealth
University
The case of Ted J. Smith, III; Guy J. Degenaro; Frank Belloni; George W. Rimler; Allan
Rosenbaum, On behalf of themselves and all others similarly situated v. Virginia Commonwealth
University (VCU) (84 F.3d 672)33 asks the question whether VCU’s effort to correct past sex
discrimination in pay was in error and trammeled the rights of the male professors. This case
demonstrates the difficulties of using multiple regression techniques to show causality in bias
cases and the potential benefit of applying Daubert criteria to the plaintiff’s expert testimony.
In the late 1980’s the VCU school newspaper printed the salaries of the professors. When
the professors studied these, they noticed that the male professors made more than the female
professors in the same position did. The administration formed a committee to study the
situation. Based on previous studies done at area universities, the committee decided to do a
multiple regression study to determine if there was a pay differential and if so, how much.
Without taking into account any other variables, the male professors’ pay was $10,000 more per
year than the female professors’ was. The multiple regression study controlled for national
salary average, degree, tenure, quick tenure, years of experience at VCU, academic experience,
experience as department chair, and gender.
33
Available via Westlaw on the internet at http://lawschool.westlaw.com, (accessed 11/11/2002).
Pikas 10
The multiple regression study showed a $1,354 pay difference between the genders. After
some time and a recalculation, administration constituted a committee to give salary increases to
qualified female professors. The female professors provided documentation to the committee
and received raises from 1-40% depending on the disparity in pay. No male professors were
permitted to request a review and increase.
The plaintiffs allege that the multiple regression analysis was faulty because it failed to take
into account major factors affecting pay, e.g., performance and prior administrator experience.
The appeals court noted that the VCU compensation system was based on merit. The annual
reviews consider teaching load, teaching quality, publications, research, and service to the
community. The department chair recommends salary increases to the dean who awards the
raise. According to the opinion, “salaries vary widely from department to department.”34
The district court granted the defendant’s request for a summary judgment based on the
belief that VCU did a thorough study and included all variables necessary for the multiple
regression analysis, the money was handed out fairly and on a individual basis, and this action
was completed to correct a previous inequity. The VCU statisticians contended that performance
factors were “inherently subjective and unquantifiable” and substituted the rough proxies of
tenure and experience.35
Because the original case was decided only months after Daubert, it is not surprising that the
district court judge did not use the factors to assess the admissibility of the scientific evidence.
The lower court judge mentioned in the opinion that published studies existed (no mention of
peer review) and that the form of analysis was generally accepted. 36 He appeared to approve of
the use of performance factors only in the remedy mechanism as other universities did. The
VCU statisticians assumed that performance factors were similar across the groups.
The dissenting judges in the appeals court did apply Daubert. They stated that the
assumptions made by the plaintiff’s expert were not backed by research and were “speculative;”
furthermore, that the Kent State study mentioned by the expert is very different from the VCU
study so does not provide a “scintilla of evidence” as required by Daubert.
34
Ibid, 675.
35
Ibid, 682.
36
Smith, et al. v. Virginia Commonwealth University (856 F. Supp. 1088; 1994 U.S. Dist. LEXIS 9277),
July 8, 1994, available via Lexis at http://www.lexis.com, (accessed 12/7/2002), n15.
Pikas 11
A careful application of the Daubert standards by the lower court may have cleared up the
primary issue, the inclusion of performance factors in the multiple regression analysis.
Specifically, both the plaintiff’s and the defendant’s experts should have provided error analysis
and some calculations to show that the gender differences were practically and statistically
significant based on the error of the study and the pay of the professors. If the plaintiff’s expert
had shown the actual impact of the performance factors or the impact of the administrator pay,
the case would have been more convincing. Without this information, the plaintiff’s expert did
not prove that the factors should be included and the summary judgment was justified.
Additionally, had Joiner been decided when this came to the appeals court, the appeals court
may have been convinced to affirm the summary judgment as the opinion was not capricious or
arbitrary but carefully reasoned.
As to the use of multiple regression analysis to decide this matter, it seems that the model
inadequately fit the situation. The intention of the analysis was to determine the impact gender
had on salary. A complete analysis would set the salary as the dependent variable, and all of the
other factors related to pay as explanatory variables with the addition of a dummy variable for
sex. Checks should show that the explanatory variables are not correlated. Other analysis
should examine error and fit.37 The variables selected by the VCU statisticians were convenient
and easily quantified, but did not adequately fit the actual situation or measure the factors
impacting the salary.
In sum, the legal issues and lack of support would lead to the summary judgment for the
defendants. If properly supported and adequately argued, however, the case should have yielded
a summary judgment for the plaintiff as suggested by Judge Luttig in his concurring opinion
because the initial analysis did not paint an accurate picture of the salary situation at VCU.38 In
other words, the equation used for the multiple regression analysis should have modeled the real
situation at the time of calculation; however, none of the explanatory variables described the
actual factors that determined the salary. The initial analysis was invalid.
Daniel L. Rubinfeld. “Reference Guide on Multiple Regression,” chapter in Federal Judicial Center,
Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence, 2d ed, (St. Paul, MN: West Group, 2000), 194-200.
37
38
Smith, Degenaro, Belloni, Rimler, Rosenbaum v. VCU, 681.
Pikas 12
Conclusion
The definition of cause is simple: to bring about or effect; yet, in civil cases where causality
is all-important, there are many definitions and ways to prove legal causality. In product liability
cases, the common way is to have an expert testify. However, if the plaintiff’s entire case rests
on an expert deemed unreliable or irrelevant by the judge, the case will never reach a jury and
will end by summary judgment. In discrimination and bias cases, the courts show much more
caution and less accuracy39, but still cases end in summary judgments.
Cases are becoming more complex, each involving several types of scientific evidence and
statistical analyses. Unfortunately, this can lead juries to commingle evidence, using “evidence
of one element of a legal claim to substitute for proof of another element.”40 This makes it
imperative for judges to serve as gatekeeper to review and strain out invalid, unreliable, or
irrelevant evidence prior to the start of the trial.
In the post-Daubert era where the judge sees the evidence at least 90 days prior to the trial
and decides on its merits before jury selection, the majority of the responsibility is on the judge
to understand complicated statistical analyses or scientific evidence. Her purpose is not to
decide what is the correct conclusion or which science best describes the real world, but to
determine if the evidence is scientifically valid, reliable, and applies to the case at hand.
Courses, studies, and books to assist the trier of fact in understanding these pieces of evidence
abound; but recent studies show that although judges agree with the gatekeeper role, they do not
necessarily understand the Daubert criteria.41 Justice Breyer, in his introduction to the Reference
Manual on Scientific Evidence, suggests judges employ neutral experts to help sift through the
evidence.42 This seems like a beneficial approach for all but the taxpayers who foot the high
bills.
Beecher-Monas suggests “Probabilistic attribution and statistical analysis frequently confound the
courts,” 1069.
39
40
Kaufman, 82.
41
Shirley A. Dobbin, Sophia I. Gatowski, James T. Richardson, Gerald P. Ginsburg, Mara L. Merline, and
Veronica Dahir, “Applying Daubert –How Well Do Judges Understand Science And Scientific Method?”
Judicature v85 i5 (Mar-Apr 2002): 244-7, available via Westlaw at http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com, (accessed
11/30/2002), 246-7.
42
Stephen Breyer, “Introduction,” chapter in Federal Judicial Center, Reference Manual on Scientific
Evidence, 2d ed, (St. Paul, MN: West Group, 2000), 7.
Pikas 13
We have seen here from the application of the Daubert factors to the sample fourth circuit
cases that the factors appear to aid in understanding the cases and coming to a just resolution in
the Nettles case. Likewise, the lack of the Daubert framework in the VCU case added confusion
and disguised the central issue of the validity of the original statistical analysis. Overall,
although not flawless, the Daubert framework with the Joiner and Kumho enhancements seems
to be a useful tool for judges to evaluate the merits of expert testimony. More training for the
judges would not be as helpful as the increase in use of neutral experts to help sort through the
evidence.
Pikas 14
Bibliography
Beecher-Monas, Erica. "A Ray of Light for Judges Blinded by Science: Triers of Science and
Intellectual Due Process." Georgia Law Review v33 (Summer 1999): 1047-111.
Dixon, Lloyd and Brian Gill. Changes in the Standards for Admitting Expert Evidence in
Federal Civil Cases Since the Daubert Decision. Santa Monica, CA: RAND, 2001.
Dixon, Lloyd and Brian Gill. “Changes in the Standards for Admitting Expert Evidence in
Federal Civil Cases Since the Daubert Decision.” Psychology, Public Policy, & Law v8
i3 (September 2002): 251-308. Available via Westlaw at
http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com. (accessed 11/30/2002).
Dobbin, Shirley A., Sophia I. Gatowski, James T. Richardson, Gerald P. Ginsburg, Mara L.
Merline, and Veronica Dahir. “Applying Daubert –How Well Do Judges Understand
Science And Scientific Method?” Judicature v85 i5 (Mar-Apr 2002): 244-7. Available
via Westlaw at http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com. (accessed 11/30/2002).
Faigman, David L. Legal Alchemy: The Use and Misuse of Science and the Law. New York:
W. H. Freeman, 1999.
Faigman, David L., David H. Kaye, and Michael J. Saks. "New Directions in Expert Testimony:
Scientific, Technical, and Other Specialized Knowledge Evidence in Federal and State
Courts." American Law Institute - American Bar Association Continuing Legal
Education ALI-ABA Course of Study. April 26, 2001. Available via Westlaw at
http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com. (accessed 12/5/2002).
Federal Judicial Center. Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence. 2d ed. St. Paul, MN: West
Group, 2000.
Federal Rules of Evidence. 2001. Available on the internet from the Legal Information Institute
at http://www.law.cornell.edu/rules/fre/overview.html. (accessed 12/4/2002).
Foster, Kenneth R. and Peter W. Huber. Judging Science: Scientific Knowledge and the Federal
Courts. Cambridge MA: MIT Press, 1997.
Freedman, D. A. “From Association to Causation: Some Remarks on the History of Statistics.”
Statistical Science v14 i3 (Aug 1999): 243–58.
Garner, Bryan A., ed. Black's Law Dictionary. 7th ed. St. Paul, MN: West Group, 1999.
Huber, Peter W. Galileo’s Revenge: Junk Science in the Courtroom. Basic Books: 1991.
Huff, Darrell. How To Lie With Statistics. New York: Norton, 1982.
Jasanoff, Sheila. Science at the Bar: Law, Science, and Technology in America. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press, 1995.
Johnson, Molly Treadway, Carol Krafka, and Joe S. Cecil. Expert Testimony in Federal Civil
Trials: A Preliminary Analysis. Washington: Federal Judicial Center, 2000.
Jones, Gregory Todd and Reidar Hagtvedt. "Sample Data as Evidence: Meeting the
Requirements of Daubert and the Recently Amended Federal Rules of Evidence."
Georgia State University Law Review v18 (Spring 2002): 721-48. Available via LEXIS
at http://www.lexis.com. (accessed 12/7/2002).
Pikas 15
Kaufman, Howard H. “The Expert Witness. Neither Frey [sic] nor Daubert Solved the Problem.
What Can Be Done?” International Review of Law Computers & Technology v15 n1
(March 2001): 79-101. Available via EBSCO MasterFile Premier at
http://www.sailor.lib.md.us/cgi-bin/ebsco. (accessed 10/15/2002).
Kaye, D H. “The Dynamics of Daubert: Methodology, Conclusions, and Fit in Statistical and
Econometric Studies.” Virginia Law Review v87 i8 (Dec 2001): 1933-2018. Available
via Westlaw at http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com. (accessed 11/30/2002).
Klock, Mark. “Finding Random Coincindences While Searching For The Holy Writ of Truth:
Specification Searches In Law And Public Policy or Cum Hoc Ergo Propter Hoc?”
Wisconsin Law Review i4 (2001): 1007-65. Available via Westlaw at
http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com. (accessed 11/30/2002).
Kovera, M B, M B Russano, and B D McAuliff. “Assessment of the Commonsense Psychology
Underlying Daubert – Legal Decision Makers’ abilities to Evaluate Expert Evidence in
Hostile Work Environment Cases.” Psychology, Public Policy, & Law v8 i2 (Jun 2002):
180-200. Available via Westlaw at http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com. (accessed
11/30/2002).
Krafka, Carol, Meghan A. Dunn, Molly Treadway Johnson, Joe S. Cecil, Dean Miletich. “Judge
and Attorney Experiences, Practices, and Concerns Regarding Expert Testimony in
Federal Civil Trials.” Psychology, Public Policy, and Law v8 n3 (September 2002):
309-32.
Nordberg, Peter. “Fourth Circuit.” Daubert on the Web. November 20, 2002. Available on the
internet at http://www.daubertontheweb.com/fourth_circuit.htm . (accessed 11/21/02).
Park, Robert. Voodoo Science: The Road from Foolishness to Fraud. New York: Oxford
University Press, 2000.
Sanders, Joseph, Shari S. Diamond, and Neil Vidmar. “Legal Perceptions of Science and Expert
Knowledge.” Psychology, Public Policy and Law v139 n8 (Jun 2002): 139-153.
Available via LEXIS at http://www.lexis.com. (accessed 10/8/2002).
Thomas, William A., ed. Scientists in the Legal System. Ann Arbor, MI: Ann Arbor Science,
1974.
Worthington, D L, M J Stallard, J M Price, and Pj Goss. “Hindsight Bias, Daubert, and The
Silicone Breast Implant Litigation – Making The Case For Court-Appointed Experts In
Complex Medical And Scientific Litigation.” Psychology, Public Policy, & Law v8 i2
(Jun 2002): 154-79. Available via Westlaw at http://www.lawschool.westlaw.com.
(accessed 11/30/2002).
Pikas 16
Download