NOTICE: this is the author’s version of a work that was accepted for publication in Law Enforcement Bulletin. Changes resulting from the publishing process, such as peer review, editing, corrections, structural formatting, and other quality control mechani sms may not be reflected in this document. Changes may have been made to this work since it was submitted for publication. A definitive version was subsequently published in FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 2009 78(5), 26-32 Brain Functioning as the Ground for Spiritual Experiences and Ethical Behavior Fred Travis, PhD Director, Center for Brain, Consciousness and Cognition Maharishi University of Management Fairfield, IA 52557 Abstract The brain is the interface between us and our world. Brain functioning changes sensations into perceptions and thoughts into actions. Reverberations in brain circuits lead to conscious experience; these reverberations also structurally change the brain. Stressful experiences change brain regions involved in memory and emotions--memory is impaired, and anxiety and aggression are increased. Spiritual experiences change frontal executive regions. Frontal brain areas become more coherent leading to more effective thinking and planning. Higher frontal coherence is correlated with higher moral reasoning, greater emotional stability, and decreased anxiety and toplevel athletes and managers have higher levels of frontal coherence. Spiritual experiences enliven executive brain circuits to buffer the impact of noxious experiences and so could contribute to higher well-being. 1 It is uncommon to have “brain” and “spiritual experiences” in the same sentence. It is even more uncommon to have “spirituality” and law enforcement together. This paper argues that brain functioning is fundamental to both spiritual experiences and the complex decisions that law enforcement officials must make every day. In addition, this paper explores how spiritual experiences can reverse the negative impact of noxious experiences on brain functioning and enhance individual well-being. The brain transforms sensations of outer objects into perceptions, and inner intentions into actions. You cannot see the mind think, but you can see the brain fire. Thus, brain patterns can serve as proxy variables for conscious functioning. Brain patterns can provide objective measures of growing subjectivity—even of spiritual experiences and ethical behavior. Different tasks activate specific brain regions. If you imagine a semi-truck in your mind’s eye, and then a postage stamp, you would see activation in the calcarine fissure—a deep fold in the visual system at the back of the brain. Larger items produce activation deeper in the fissure; small items produce activation closer to the surface (Korogi et al., 1997). Other brain areas are activated during esthetic judgments. When individuals look at sculptures, they usually pick the sculpture that corresponds to the Golden Mean as being more pleasing. Associated with that judgment is greater activation in the left back (parietal) brain area (Di Dio et al., 2007). Now, you are asked to make an ethical judgment: Is it right to steal a loaf of bread to feed someone who is starving? As you are thinking through this problem, areas in the front of the brain become active (Pujol et al., 2008). The frontal area is the boss of the brain; the CEO of the brain. Information from all brain areas converges on the front of the brain—emotions, memories, unconscious processes, concrete perceptual experiences, intentions and plans come together in frontal areas. Frontal areas then put ongoing experience into the larger context of past and future; right and wrong. 2 Frontal executive areas are responsible for planning, decision-making and judgment. Connections of other parts of the brain with frontal areas begin to gain a fatty layer of myelin around age 12. The process of myelination continues through age 25. When output fibers are myelinated, information travels along those fibers 20 times faster (Toga et al., 2006). Imagine the world of a teenager. The brain areas for seeing the world (back sensory areas) and responding to the world (motor areas) are fully myelinated. Connections with the frontal brain areas are not yet developed. Consequently, the output of sensory and motor processing is done first and the teenager acts before the input from the frontal areas can be added to their decision. Thus, the teenager’s world remains rooted in concrete experiences, and is not placed in relation to consequences (past and future), or overall plans and values. Teenagers are like cars with their accelerator intact, but the brakes are not yet installed. Effects of Ongoing Experiences on Brain Connections: Experience changes the brain The natural maturation of frontal areas is constantly tuned by experience. Experience flowing through the brain leaves its trace in the structure and function of the brain—when two cells fire together, they are wired together (Garagnani et al., 2008). For instance, violin players have larger cortical representations in the primary somatosensory cortex corresponding to the fingers of their left hand, the hand that forms the chords, than their right hand, which holds the bow (Elbert et al., 1995). London taxi cab drivers with more years of navigation experience have higher hippocampal gray matter volume, an area associated with spatial memory, than do novice taxi cab drivers (Maguire et al., 2006). The “phantom limb” experience in amputees results from cortical reorganization following the loss of the limb (Flor et al., 1995). Implications for Law Enforcement Officers. You have decided to join the police force to protect your community. You meet similar farsighted individuals, functioning at high levels of thinking, and displaying high levels of moral reasoning. Now you have weathered five or ten years 3 of service. What might have happened? Lieutenant Tejada, University of California Police Department, Berkeley, California comments: Police officers are exposed to the worst that life has to offer. They see the denizens of society at their very worst—when they have just been victimized or when they have just victimized someone else. Police officers see the perpetrators of evil and the results of their evil deeds. The constant contact with evil is corrosive, and those effects are cumulative. ((Feemster, 2007), pg, 22) Experiences change the brain. This is inevitable. The violence law enforcement officers see becomes part of the functioning of their brain and body. Neural imaging assessed activation of the areas of the brain that stop wrong behavior, called orbitofrontal cortex, after individuals watched violent movies. Activation in the orbitofrontal cortex decreased as participates watched more and more violent movies (Patrick, 2008). Bandura’s social learning research found that children who play violent video games act more violently in play and allow more aggressive behavior towards friends. Stress affects brain connections. Under stress, the brain downshifts to a more primitive response style. It is called “fight-or-flight.” Frontal executive brain areas are off line. Under stress one sees the world and responds to the world. Chronic stress—a small elevation of stress over the long term— and acute stressors such as the death of a spouse, produces lasting change on brain functioning. High stress causes brain regions involved in memory and emotions, such as hippocampus (Sapolsky, 1996), amygdala, and prefrontal cortex, to undergo structural remodeling-memory is impaired and anxiety (Bremner, 2006) and aggression are increased (McEwen, 1998; McEwen, 2006). This can lead to officers becoming distant from spouses, kids, family, and friends because they are too drained to give to anyone else (Hawkins, 2001; McCarty et al., 2007). Neural imaging of individuals who have been under extensive stress, and/or abused alcohol and drugs reveals “functional lesions” in their frontal executive areas (Amen, 1998). The brain matter is intact. However, it is not involved in planning and decision-making. 4 Need for a Fundamentally Different Experience Stressful experiences reduce connections with frontal executive areas and amplify stimulus-response circuits. An experience of wholeness is needed to balance the experience of negative, stressful experiences. Spiritual experiences are defined in this paper as the experience of wholeness. They are experiences of our universal nature, that part of us that is not tied to time, space, our individual body or personality. Spiritual experiences, as discussed in this paper, are not confined to religious practices. To understand this concept, we will model the mind as an ocean with constantly changing waves at the surface and silence at the depth. Thoughts and feelings are picked up at the surface of the mind. They are analogous to the changing waves of the ocean. This is the level of ordinary experiences. Intuition, vague feelings are picked up at more vague or subtle levels of the mind. They are analogous to the middle levels of the ocean. Spiritual experiences occur at the silent depths of the mind. This is analogous to the depths of the ocean. This silent depth of the mind has been called “pure consciousness” (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1969; Travis and Pearson, 2000). The adjective “pure” emphasizes that it is purely an experience of wakefulness, self-awareness, or consciousness. Selfawareness continues, even though bodily sensations, sensory perceptions, thoughts, and feelings are absent. Spiritual experiences activate widespread brain areas, as reflected in higher brainwave coherence (Travis et al., 2002). Coherence is a mathematical calculation of the degree of similarity of electrical activity between two areas on the scalp. If patterns of electrical activity are similar, then brain areas are considered to be structurally or functioning related (Thatcher et al., 1986). Peak experiences investigated by Maslow fit into this category of spiritual 5 experiences. Peak experiences occur spontaneous in many individuals. Peak experiences transform a person’s worldview. One becomes more self-reliant, self-sufficient, independent, and takes responsibility for one’s life and performance, rather than relying on others (Maslow, 1968). Spontaneous spiritual experiences, such as peak experiences occur across cultures. However, since they occur spontaneously, it is difficult to measure the associated brain and bodily functioning of these experiences. Our lab has measured spiritual experiences during Transcendental Meditation (TM) practice. In terms of the analogy of the mind as an ocean, TM practice takes attention from the active surface thinking level of the mind to the silent depths of the mind--pure consciousness devoid of mental content (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1969) A content analysis of pure consciousness experiences revealed three themes that characterized that state: the absence of time, space and body sense with an expanded sense of self-awareness (Travis and Pearson, 2000). This state is an intrinsic state that emerges in awareness when thought activity settles down. Brain patterns during pure consciousness experiences could suggest how spiritual experiences may change brain patterns and in turn help to reverse the toxic effects of stress and negativity. Brain patterns during pure consciousness experiences. Pure consciousness experiences, during TM practice, are characterized by higher alpha and beta1 coherence between left and right frontal cortices and front and back cortical areas (Travis et al., in press). Simultaneous recording of EEG and MEG during TM practice reported that higher frontal and central alpha EEG coherence is associated with MEG source location in medial frontal and anterior cingulate cortices—the executive centers of the brain (Yamamoto et al., 2006). Higher frontal coherence support direct modes of experience in which knower and known merge, and the personal self-sense is transcended. 6 TM experiences are also reported to change brain patterns during challenging cognitive tasks after the meditation session. EEG was recorded during simple and choice reaction time tasks in non-TM, short-term (7.1 yrs TM) and long-term TM participants (24.2 yrs TM). Three brain distinguished these three group: 1) higher broad-band frontal coherence (alpha: 8-12Hz, beta: 12.520Hz, and gamma: 20.5-50Hz), 2) higher alpha/beta power ratios, and 3) better match between task demands and brain preparatory response (Travis et al., 2000; Travis et al., 2002). These empirically identified measures were converted to z-scores and combined to form a scale. This scale was called a “Brain Integration Scale” (Travis et al., 2002). Brain Integration Scale scores in these participants positively correlated with emotional stability, moral reasoning, and inner directedness, and negatively correlated with anxiety (Travis et al., 2004). Construct Validation of the Brain Integration Scale. Brain Integration Scale (BIS) scores were higher in individuals with more years meditation practices. Two questions naturally arise from this finding. First, do spiritual experiences during TM practice increase BIS scores, or are people with higher BIS scores more likely to learn and continue TM practice? A second question is: Is the Brain Integration Scale just a “meditation” measure or are BIS scores related with outer success? Two studies suggest that TM practice does increase BIS scores. Also, two studies report that top level athletes and managers have higher BIS scores. These studies will first be presented and then the relation of BIS scores with law enforcement will be discussed. Longitudinal increases in frontal coherence (within groups). Fourteen students were assessed four times over their first year of TM practice—baseline, and after 2 months, 6 months and 12 months TM practice (Travis and Arenander, 2006). EEG was measured during TM practice, and during reaction-time tasks. Frontal broadband coherence significantly increased after twomonths TM practice to very highly levels. Coherence values reached these same high levels during 7 the six- and twelve-month recordings. In contrast, during the reaction-time tasks, frontal coherence continued to increase at each posttest recording. These findings suggest 1) the state gained during TM practice can be achieved successfully after only a few months practice, and 2) meditation practice has practical benefits for daily life. These data emphasize the point that spiritual experiences have real, practical effects on the functioning of one’s mind and body. Longitudinal increases in BIS scores (random assignment study). This randomized controlled trial investigated effects of TM practice on BIS scores, electrodermal habituation to 85dB tones, and sleepiness in 38 college students. After pretest, students were randomly assigned to learn TM immediately or learn after the posttest. At posttest, the TM group had significantly higher BIS scores, less incidence of sleepiness during the day, and faster habituation rates—they were less jumpy and reactive (Travis et al., 2007). Lower sleepiness and faster habituation rates were correlated with higher scores on the Brain Integration Scale. College is a time of great challenge. The academic, financial, and social demands of the college experience can be highly stressful (Arnedt et al., 2005; Zeigler et al., 2005). TM practice buffered effects of high stress—BIS scores increased; sleepiness decreased, and sympathetic reactivity was lower. These data suggest that spiritual experiences, such as those resulting from TM practice, may help an individual deal with the stress of life. Higher BIS scores in world-class athletes. This and the next study report BIS scores in world-class subjects, who did not practice a meditation technique. These two studies address the question: Are BIS scores related to outer success? The first study was done in collaboration with the National Olympic Training Center (Olympiatoppen) in Norway and the Norwegian University for Sports Sciences (Norges Idrettshøgskole). Thirty-three professional athletes were selected who met three criteria: 1) placing among the 10 best performers in major competitions (Olympic Games, World Championships, 8 World Cup, or similar) for at least three seasons; 2) being active on the top level within the last five years, and 3) being at least 25 years of age. Thirty-three control athletes were selected, who had been active in training and competition at the senior level for at least three seasons, but did not consistently place amongst the top 50 % in the Norwegian Championships. These subjects were matched on age, gender, and type of sport. The two groups of athletes, who differed significantly in level of competitive performance, also differed in physiological and psychological measures. The World-class performers had higher BIS scores; faster habituation rates to a loud tone; and higher scores on measures on selfdevelopment and moral reasoning (Travis et al., 2007). A similar finding is reported with top-level managers as reported by the next study. Higher BIS scores in top-level managers. Twenty top-level Norwegian managers were selected from the private and public sectors. The criteria used were 1) demonstrated excellence in management performance as evidenced by expanding their business many times or turning around failing businesses, and 2) reputation for being socially responsible, having sound ethics, and displaying a more human orientation in business. The comparison group consisted of twenty persons employed at lower and middle levels in organizations. The comparison individuals were matched for gender and type of organization (competitive/non-competitive). Top level managers were characterized by: higher BIS scores and higher moral reasoning. Both top-performing managers and world-class athletes exhibited high BIS scores and higher levels of moral reasoning. BIS scores include high frontal EEG coherence – more integrated functioning of the brain’s frontal executive centers, the “CEO of the brain” (Travis et al., in press). Frontal integration provides a coherent framework to unite localized processes into a larger picture. It allows the individual to rise above the demands of the moment and include more expanded 9 concepts in making decisions—for instance the impact of actions on society rather than only on one’s individual needs. Discussion Spiritual experiences could provide the inner armour to protect law enforcement officers from the noxious effects of negative experiences and stress. The data presented here indicate that spiritual experiences, during TM practice, led to increased frontal brain integration, faster habituation to stressful stimuli, and higher moral reasoning. Spiritual experiences enliven frontal coherence, which builds global circuits to place individual experiences in a larger framework. This appears to neutralize the effects of noxious experiences. This is more than just coping with the pain. Spiritual experiences add a new dimension to meaning-making to redefine the experience into a more positive light. Spiritual experiences should be seriously considered and included in the protective gear for every law enforcement officer. It will enable them to do their job and continue to thrive as human beings. 10 References Amen, D. G. (1998). Change Your Brain, Change Your Life: The Breakthrough Program for Conquering Anxiety, Depression, Obsessiveness, Anger, and Impulsiveness. New Yori: Three Rivers Press. Arnedt, J. T., Owens, J., Crouch, M., Stahl, J. & Carskadon, M. A. (2005). Neurobehavioral performance of residents after heavy night call vs after alcohol ingestion. JAMA, 294, 102533. Bremner, J. D. (2006). The relationship between cognitive and brain changes in posttraumatic stress disorder. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1071, 80-6. Di Dio, C., Macaluso, E. & Rizzolatti, G. (2007). The Golden Beauty: Brain Response to Classical and Renaissance Sculptures. PLoS ONE, 2, e1201. Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B. & Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270, 305-7. Feemster, S. L. (2007). Spirituality: The DNA of law enforcement practice. Federal Bureau of Investigation Law Enforcement Bulletin, 76, 22-23. Flor, H., Elbert, T., Knecht, S., Wienbruch, C., Pantev, C., Birbaumer, N., Larbig, W. & Taub, E. (1995). Phantom-limb pain as a perceptual correlate of cortical reorganization following arm amputation. Nature, 375, 482-4. Garagnani, M., Wennekers, T. & Pulvermuller, F. (2008). A neuroanatomically grounded Hebbianlearning model of attention-language interactions in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci, 27, 492-513. Hawkins, H. C. (2001). Police officer burnout: A partial replication of Maslach's Burnout Inventory. Police Quarterly, 4, 343-360. Korogi, Y., Takahashi, M., Hirai, T., Ikushima, I., Kitajima, M., Sugahara, T., Shigematsu, Y., Okajima, T. & Mukuno, K. (1997). Representation of the visual field in the striate cortex: comparison of MR findings with visual field deficits in organic mercury poisoning (Minamata disease). AJNR Am J Neuroradiol, 18, 1127-30. Maguire, E. A., Woollett, K. & Spiers, H. J. (2006). London taxi drivers and bus drivers: a structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus, 16, 1091-101. Maharishi Mahesh Yogi. (1969). Maharishi Mahesh Yogi on the Bhagavad Gita. New York: Penguin. Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of Being. New York: van Nostrand. McCarty, W. P., Zhao, J. & Garland, B. (2007). Occupational Stress and Burnout Between Male and Female Police Officers: Are there any Gender Differences? Annual meeting of the AMERICAN SOCIETY OF CRIMINOLOGY. McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 840, 33-44. McEwen, B. S. (2006). Sleep deprivation as a neurobiologic and physiologic stressor: Allostasis and allostatic load. Metabolism, 55, S20-3. Patrick, C. J. (2008). Review. Psychophysiological correlates of aggression and violence: an integrative review. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, Pujol, J., Reixach, J., Harrison, B. J., Timoneda-Gallart, C., Vilanova, J. C. & Perez-Alvarez, F. (2008). Posterior cingulate activation during moral dilemma in adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp, 29, 910-21. Sapolsky, R. M. (1996). Stress, glucocorticoids, and damage to the nervous system: The current state of confusion. Stress, 1, 1-19. 11 Thatcher, R. W., Krause, P. J. & Hrybyk, M. (1986). Cortico-cortical associations and EEG coherence: a two-compartmental model. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology, 64, 123-43. Toga, A. W., Thompson, P. M. & Sowell, E. R. (2006). Mapping brain maturation. Trends in Neuroscience, 29, 148-59. Travis, F. & Arenander, A. (2006). Cross-sectional and longitudinal study of effects of transcendental meditation practice on interhemispheric frontal asymmetry and frontal coherence. International Journal of Neuroscience, 116, 1519-38. Travis, F., Haaga, D., Hagelin, J., Tanner, M., Arenander, A., Nidich, S., Gaylord, C., Grosswald, S., Rainforth, M. & Schneider, R. (in press). A Self-Referential Default Brain State: Patterns of Coherence, Power, and eLORETA Sources during Eyes-Closed Rest and the Transcendental Meditation Practice. Cogitive Processig, Travis, F., Harung, H. & Blank, W. (in press). Higher development and leadership: Toward brain measures of managerial capacity. Journal of Business and Psychology, Travis, F. & Pearson, C. (2000). Pure consciousness: distinct phenomenological and physiological correlates of "consciousness itself". Int J Neurosci, 100(1-4), Travis, F., Tanner, M., Haaga, D. & Schneider, R. (2007). Cortical plasticity, college experience, and the Transcendental Meditation technique. Society for Psychophysiological Research, Savannah, GA. Travis, F., Tecce, J. J. & Guttman, J. (2000). Cortical plasticity, contingent negative variation, and transcendent experiences during practice of the Transcendental Meditation technique. Biol Psychol, 55(1), 41-55. Travis, F. T., Arenander, A. T. & DuBois, D. (2004). Psychological and physiological characteristics of a proposed Object-Referral/Self-Referral continuum of self-awareness. Conscious Cogn, 13, 401-420. Travis, F. T., Tecce, J., Arenander, A. & Wallace, R. K. (2002). Patterns of EEG coherence, power, and contingent negative variation characterize the integration of transcendental and waking states. Biol Psychol, 61, 293-319. Yamamoto, S., Kitamura, Y., Yamada, N., Nakashima, Y. & Kuroda, S. (2006). Medial profrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in the generation of alpha activity induced by transcendental meditation: a magnetoencephalographic study. Acta Medica Okayama, 60, 51-8. Zeigler, D. W., Wang, C. C., Yoast, R. A., Dickinson, B. D., McCaffree, M. A., Robinowitz, C. B. & Sterling, M. L. (2005). The neurocognitive effects of alcohol on adolescents and college students. Prev Med, 40, 23-32. 12