1 Child-Parent Attachment and Response to Threat: A Move from the

advertisement
1
Child-Parent Attachment and Response to Threat:
A Move from the Level of Representation1
Jude Cassidy
Katherine B. Ehrlich
Laura J. Sherman
University of Maryland at College Park
“We are here concerned with nothing less than the nature of love and its origins in the attachment
of a baby to his mother. According to this viewpoint, attachment originates in a few specific
patterns of behavior, some of which are manifest at birth and some of which develop shortly
afterward. Attachment is not present at birth, however; it emerges gradually through a course of
development, and it is perhaps a matter of more or less arbitrary definition to identify the point at
which it could be said to have finally emerged. Attachment is manifested through these patterns
of behavior but the patterns do not themselves constitute the attachment. Attachment is internal.
We can conceive of attachment as somehow being built into the nervous system, in the course of
and as a result of the infant’s experience of his transactions with his mother and with other
people. This internalized something that we call attachment has aspects of feelings, memories,
wishes, expectancies, and intentions, all of which constitute an inner program acquired through
experience and somehow built into a flexible yet retentive inner mechanism (which we identify
with central nervous system functions) which serves as a kind of filter for the reception and
interpretation of interpersonal experience and as a kind of template shaping the nature of
outwardly observable response.”
Mary Ainsworth, 1967
Infancy in Uganda
During the past 30 years, the study of individual differences in the quality of infant-parent
attachment has focused on the representations that infants form during their first year of their
caregivers, of themselves, and of relationships. John Bowlby, the developer of attachment
theory, sometimes called these “representational models” (Bowlby, 1973) and sometimes
“internal working models” (the latter to emphasize the active nature of these representations;
Bowlby, 1973, 1988). These representational models are thought to center on the extent to which
the parent serves as a secure base from which the infant can explore and a safe haven to which
the infant can return when distressed or threatened. The pioneering empirical work of Mary
2
Ainsworth (e.g., Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978), and later of Mary Main (e.g., Main,
Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985), led to an explosion of research related to these representations (see
also Bretherton & Munholland, 2008). Above all else, the notion that representations derive from
experiences (as opposed to Freud’s, 1905/1953, 1917/1953, claim that representations were
based on fantasy and drives) was central to Bowlby’s conceptualizations.
Yet at the same time that these experiences with caregivers are contributing to the
formation of infant representations, these experiences are also likely to set into motion a variety
of non-representational, physiological regulatory processes that have important links to children’s
developing attachment systems. The fact that Bowlby paid relatively little attention to these
physiological processes is surprising given that Bowlby had begun his studies as a biologist, and
that the education about ethology that he had received largely from Robert Hinde was so central
to his early theorizing about why humans become attached to their caregivers. Yet Bowlby’s
focus on representations may be understood considering that he was initially writing during a
time when many branches of science were focused on the “cognitive revolution.” Importantly,
most of the research related to physiology within the mother-infant relationship was not then
available.
In this chapter, the aspect of infant and child functioning on which we focus is response
to threat. Understanding children’s response to threat is fundamental to understanding virtually
all aspects of children’s well being, including social, emotional, cognitive, and physiological
functioning, as well as the development of psychopathology and physical disease. One of the core
propositions of attachment theory is that attachment serves to reduce fearfulness in the face of
possible or actual threat, and that a central mechanism explaining this link is children’s
experience-based representations of the availability of the attachment figure. Moreover, it is
individual differences in the nature of these representations that are thought to predict individual
3
differences in children’s response to threat. Yet in species that do not possess the representational
capacities that humans possess, the link between attachment and response to threat nonetheless
clearly exists. The goal of this chapter is to consider what is known about this link that does not
involve representational processes.
We begin this chapter with Bowlby’s (1969/1982, 1973) thinking about the link between
attachment and response to threat, which is best understood within the context of what Bowlby
referred to as the secure base/safe haven construct. Bowlby drew on the research available at the
time to focus on behavioral response to threat. We then describe briefly the nature of
attachment-related representations and how they are thought to mediate the link between childparent attachment and child behavioral response to threat in humans. We then focus on the ways
in which the same experiences with caregivers that contribute to infant representations are also
likely to contribute to non-representational, physiological regulatory processes that reflect
response to threat at the biological level. In this way, we discuss how early experiences within
the attachment relationship can contribute directly to infant and child response to threat, without
involving representational processes, as is surely the case in some nonhuman mammals. Next, we
discuss the ways in which representational and nonrepresentational processes may interact to
influence development in humans. We conclude with a brief summary and consideration of
additional topics that could be examined within a broader model.
The Secure Base/Safe Haven Construct:
Understanding the Link between Attachment and Children’s Response to Threat
Attachment theory contains key propositions about the link between attachment and
children’s response to threat. In fact, much of the early development of attachment theory grew
from observations that children appeared to view separations from their mothers as threats to
which they responded with considerable distress (see Kobak & Madsen, 2008). Bowlby (1988),
4
along with his colleague Mary Ainsworth (1963), proposed what has come to be known as the
secure base/safe haven construct, and it is this thinking that is at the center of understanding the
link between attachment and response to threat.
The secure base/safe haven construct contains the notion that the two principle roles of an
attachment figure are serving as a secure base from which the child can explore, and as a haven
of safety to which the child can return in times of threat or distress (haven of safety is a term
originally used by Harlow, 1958). As such, this thinking rests on the conceptualization of the
interrelations among three behavioral systems: the attachment, exploration, and fear systems.
Bowlby (1969/1982) borrowed the behavioral system concept from ethologists to describe a
species-specific system of behaviors that leads to certain predictable outcomes, at least one of
which contributes to reproductive fitness.
When formulating attachment theory, one of Bowlby’s (1958) initial observations was
that infants engage in a series of behaviors, which he called attachment behaviors (e.g., crying,
following, signaling), that have the predictable outcome of gaining or maintaining proximity to
caregivers. Bowlby (1969/1982) argued that attachment behaviors are organized into an
attachment behavioral system that evolved because it enhanced the child’s survival to
reproductive age by reducing the threat of predators and other dangers. According to Bowlby, the
exploratory system gives survival advantage to the child in a different way – by providing
important information about the workings of the environment. Yet unbridled exploration with no
attention to potential hazards can be dangerous. The complementary yet mutually inhibiting
nature of the exploratory and attachment systems is thought to have evolved to ensure that while
the child is protected by maintaining proximity to attachment figures, he or she nonetheless
gradually learns about the environment through exploration. According to Ainsworth (1972),
5
“the dynamic equilibrium between these two behavioral systems is even more significant for
development (and for survival) than either in isolation” (p. 118).
The fear behavioral system, with its close links to both exploration and attachment, is
another behavioral system particularly important to understanding children’s responses to threat.
For Bowlby (1973), the biological function of the fear system, like that of the attachment system,
is protection. The evolutionary advantage of the fear system stems from the fact that it is
biologically adaptive for children to be frightened of certain stimuli. Without such fear, survival
and reproduction would be reduced. Bowlby described “natural clues to danger” — stimuli that
are not inherently dangerous but that increase the likelihood of danger; these include darkness,
loud noises, aloneness, and sudden looming movements. Because the attachment and fear
systems are intertwined so that frightened infants increase their attachment behavior, infants who
find these stimuli frightening are considered more likely to seek protection from caregivers and
thus to survive to pass on their genes.
Most infants balance these three behavioral systems, responding flexibly to a specific
situation after assessing both the environment’s characteristics and the caregiver’s availability
and likely behavior. For the purposes of this chapter, it is most important to note that experiences
related to the attachment figure’s availability and responsiveness are thought to play an important
role in the activation of an infant’s fear system, such that an available and responsive attachment
figure makes the infant much less susceptible to fear2. According to Bowlby (1973), “the degree
to which each of us is susceptible to fear turns in great part on whether our attachment figures are
present or absent” (p. 201). Bowlby (1973) described as important not only the physical presence
of an attachment figure, but also the infant’s confidence that the attachment figure will be
available if needed. The evolutionary advantage derived from this linkage is that it is at times
when the child is under threat and therefore most vulnerable that the child has most to gain from
6
contact with and protection from the attachment figure. A converging body of empirical work, in
which maternal physical or psychological presence was experimentally manipulated, has
provided compelling evidence of the theoretically predicted associations among maternal
availability, infant exploration, and infant fear/wariness (Ainsworth & Wittig, 1969; Carr, Dabbs,
& Carr, 1975; Sorce & Emde, 1981). In sum, for Bowlby (1988), the secure base/safe haven
concept was at the heart of attachment theory: ‘‘No concept within the attachment framework is
more central to developmental psychiatry than that of the secure base’’ (p. 163–164).
Internal Representational Models and their Role as a Mediating Mechanism of the Link
between Attachment and Response to Threat
In addition to the normative links between attachment and response to threat, individual
differences in the quality of attachment relate in important ways to variation in children’s
response to threat. Although virtually all children become attached to their primary caregivers
(yet see Dozier & Rutter, 2008, and Zeanah, Smyke, Koga, & Carlson, 2005, for descriptions of
extreme circumstances), the quality of the attachment varies as a function of the nature of the
specific infant-caregiver relationship. Some infants – labeled securely attached – are able to
effectively use their caregivers as a secure base from which to explore and a safe haven to return
to in times of need, as described above, whereas other infants – labeled insecurely attached –are
unable to do so effectively (Ainsworth et al., 1978; for reviews, see Cassidy & Shaver, 2008).
Several studies of infants and young children have reported links between attachment quality and
behavioral response to threat, with children securely attached to their mothers showing less
fearfulness than insecurely attached children (e.g., Kochanska, 2001; Shamir-Essakow, Ungerer,
& Rapee, 2005; Stevenson-Hinde & Shouldice, 1990).
Attempts to explain how attachment and early attachment-related caregiving experiences
are linked to behavioral response to threat have centered in large part on infant representations as
7
the causal mechanism. In order to understand how representational models mediate this link, we
begin this section by describing these models. Next, we explain the formation of representational
models, focusing on the centrality of early experiences. Then, we describe how these models are
thought to influence response to threat.
According to attachment theory, infants develop experience-based mental models of their
caregivers’ likely behavior (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973, 1979, 1980, 1988). The models, which
develop early in life, begin as simple sensorimotor expectations and become more complex,
flexible, and extendable over time (Bretherton & Munholland, 2008; Thompson, 2008). These
models contain information about “how [an infant’s] mother and other significant persons may
be expected to behave, how he himself may be expected to behave, and how each interacts with
the other” (Bowlby, 1969/1982, p. 354). These models then operate like cognitive schemas and
scripts, allowing individuals to efficiently predict, interpret, and guide their interactions with
others (Bretherton & Munholland, 2008; Main et al., 1985; Waters & Waters, 2006). For
instance, securely attached infants have representations of their caregivers as effective secure
bases – as such, they may have predictive scripts such as, “If I’m upset, then my father will
comfort me,” whereas insecure infants may have a predictive scripts such as, “If I’m upset, then
my father will push me away.” These models also allow infants to interpret their caregivers’
behavior in new situations. Finally, these models guide how infants behave with caregivers.
Securely attached infants, as compared to insecurely attached infants, will be more likely to
approach their caregiver when distressed because they have an expectation that the caregiver will
respond and be an effective aid. Individual differences in infants’ representational models were
recently demonstrated in a quasi-experimental study: Securely attached infants’ cognitive
expectations for the behavior of caregivers and infants followed a secure base script (wherein a
distressed infant seeks and receives care), whereas insecurely attached infants’ expectations of
8
infant-caregiver interactions deviated from a secure base script (Johnson, Dweck, & Chen, 2007;
Johnson et al., 2010).
What are the precursors of representational models? According to Bowlby, these
representational models are experience-based – he viewed them as “tolerably accurate reflections
of the experiences those individuals have actually had” with their caregivers, rather than
projections of the child’s internal psyche as Freud had proposed (Bowlby, 1973, p. 202). Securely
attached infants represent their caregiver as a secure base and safe haven because past
experiences during attachment-related interactions provided information that their caregiver was
available, responsive, and sensitive. This information is stored in the mental model and is used as
the basis from which to create expectations for the caregiver’s future behavior. As such,
individual differences in the content of these representations are based on the caregiver’s
parenting behavior during day-to-day attachment-related interactions. To the extent that
caregiving experiences remain stable, children’s attachment representations remain stable as
well, and they become increasingly resistant to change (Bowlby, 1988; Bretherton &
Munholland, 2008). If, however, caregiving experiences change, either for the better or for the
worse, then children’s attachment representations will change accordingly (Vaughn, Egeland,
Sroufe, & Waters, 1979; Weinfield, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2000).
That Bowlby viewed representations as playing an important role in predicting children’s
response to threat is clear. In his discussion of representational models, Bowlby (1973) noted that
“intimately linked to the type of [representational] forecast a person makes of the probable
availability of his attachment figures, moreover, is his susceptibility to respond with fear
whenever he meets any potentially alarming situation during the ordinary course of his life” (p.
203). It is the nature of secure and insecure children’s representations of the availability and
responsiveness of their caregivers in times of threat that is thought to account for the variation in
9
the ways in which these children respond to threat. Specifically, it is because securely attached
infants are more likely than insecurely attached infants to predict caregiver availability and
responsiveness that they are able to interpret the threat as manageable and respond with less fear
and anxiety.
A Move from the Level of Representations: Back to Biology
Two important and related advances since the time of Bowlby’s (1969, 1973) initial
writings about infant attachment and response to separation have greatly extended scientists’
understanding of the link between attachment and response to threat. One important advance
originated in the research of Myron Hofer that began to emerge during the 1970s. Hofer, a
developmental psychobiologist, noticed defensive vocal protest responses to maternal separation
in infant rat pups, and asked what non-representational process could account for these
responses. This work has facilitated researchers’ consideration of processes beyond the level of
representation (see Main, 1999, for a call to examine these nonrepresentational processes in
humans). A second and related advance has been an extensive examination of response to threat
beyond the behavioral and emotional levels to an examination of response to threat at the
physiological level. The explosion of research examining neuroendocrine systems and epigenetic
processes, much of this work done by researchers examining what is called stress reactivity, has
brought a greatly enriched understanding of the links between early attachment experiences and
response to threat at multiple levels of analysis.
We begin this section with a review of Hofer’s seminal work on the physiological
subsystems that become disrupted in response to maternal separation, as well as the work of
Seymour Levine who examined the effects of maternal separation on rat pups’ physiological
threat response system. Next, we discuss the important work of Michael Meaney and colleagues,
who identified – at the level of the gene – how maternal caregiving experiences influence gene
10
expression and response to threat in rats. We then describe evidence from studies of primates,
again showing how attachment experiences shape responses to threat in mammals without fully
developed representational systems. We conclude this section with a brief review of caregiving
influences on the threat response system in humans, highlighting recent evidence that suggests
the presence of underlying physiological subsystems that serve regulatory functions early in
development in response to threat.
Hidden Regulators in Mother-Infant Interactions: The Case of the Rat
Hofer’s (1970) observations that rat pups responded with significant distress when
separated from their mothers – similar to distress signals in humans – led him to wonder about
the mechanisms of separation distress in nonhuman mammals. After all, rat pups lack the
capacity for mental representations and these signals of distress could not, therefore, be rooted in
representational processes. To explore this question, Hofer and colleagues conducted a series of
tightly controlled experiments to identify what physiological subsystems might be disrupted
when mothers were removed from their pups (for reviews, see Hofer, 1994, 2002, 2006). These
studies revealed a number of “hidden regulators” that no longer function properly if rat pups are
separated from their mothers. These regulators are “hidden” because they are not immediately
visible, but operate “under the skin.” Hofer and colleagues found that when rat pups were
removed from their mothers, they showed a dramatic decline in multiple physiological and
behavioral systems, such as those controlling heart rate, body temperature, food intake,
movement, and exploratory behavior. Hofer argued that maternal separation resulted in the
removal of the important regulatory functions that the mother serves for her offspring, and the
absence of these regulatory components resulted in rat pups’ visible behavioral distress.
Interestingly, these studies demonstrated that the underlying physiological regulatory systems are
11
interconnected in complex ways. For example, replacing heat that is lost in the mother’s absence
led to a slower rate of decline in rat pups’ activity levels. Similarly, rat pup heart rate could be
maintained by artificially regulating food intake (Hofer, 1970). These studies led Hofer to
conclude that mother-infant interactions have embedded within them a number of vital
physiological regulatory functions that are negatively affected by separation from maternal care
and that do not require cognitive representations in order to influence infants’ responses to
maternal separation.
But how do mother-infant interactions have such a profound impact on rat pup stress
physiology? What is it about maternal behavior that “gets under the skin?” The work of Meaney
and colleagues sheds light on this question. Meaney found that certain caregiving experiences
early in infancy were associated with long-term changes in rats’ stress response physiology (Liu
et al., 1997). Specifically, rat pups that received high levels of maternal licking and grooming
and arched-back nursing positions showed attenuated responses to threat and increased
exploratory behavior – effects that lasted into adulthood (Caldji et al., 1998; Liu et al., 1997; see
Meaney, 2001, for a review). Meaney and his colleagues further identified that these individual
differences in maternal behavior led to differences in gene expression for the glucocorticoid
receptor gene promoter (Weaver et al., 2004). Although the genes themselves did not differ
between rats, the ways in which the genes operated differed as a function of maternal behavior
(see Weaver et al., 2004, for a more detailed account of the DNA methylation process).
Levine’s (1957) groundbreaking recognition that the environment can exert an influence
over the infant’s developing stress response system represented a paradigm shift for the field.
This new understanding led to investigations into the influence of maternal care on rat pups’
developing physiology that identified ways in which mothers serve as regulators of infants’
12
pituitary-adrenal stress response system (see Levine, 2001, 2005, for reviews; Stanton, Gutierrez,
& Levine, 1988; Stanton, Wallstrom, & Levine, 1987). In one study, for example, rat pups were
separated from their mothers and later exposed to a novel experience. Rat pups’ physiological
stress response, characterized by the increased release of corticosterone, was related to the length
of time that the pups had been separated from their mothers (Stanton et al., 1988). In other words,
early caregiving experience (in this case, maternal separation) contributed to dysregulation in
endocrine system functioning. Interestingly, findings from another study showed that the
corticosterone response to maternal separation could be buffered by feeding rats during mothers'
absence. These findings suggest that one hidden regulator of the rats' stress response system is
mothers' regulation of infant feeding (Suchecki, Rosenfeld, & Levine, 1993). Levine’s large body
of research taught researchers about the important role that mothers play as regulators of their
infants' fragile and developing stress response systems.
Attachment and Response to Threat in Nonhuman Primates
Just as rodent mothers serve as regulators of their pups’ physiology, so too do nonhuman
primate mothers serve as regulators of their infants’ biological functioning (Boyce, Champoux,
Suomi, & Gunnar, 1995; Levine & Wiener, 1988; see Suomi, 2008, for a review). For example,
following an initial state of hyperarousal, infant pig-tail macaque monkeys who were separated
from their mothers showed declines in heart rate and body temperature (Reite, Kaufman, Pauley,
& Stynes, 1974). Similarly, infant behaviors changed as a result of maternal separation: The
youngest infants exhibited characteristic depressed behaviors (e.g., slouched posture and motor
coordination problems) and the older infants showed decreased play behavior. These findings
suggest that mothers’ absence is a threatening experience, and the lack of maternal availability
leads to the dysregulation of behavioral and physiological systems.
13
Additional evidence for maternal regulation of infant physiological systems comes from
studies of nonhuman primates’ endocrine dysregulation in response to separation (e.g., Sanchez
et al., 2005). Rhesus macaque infants who were separated physically from their mothers, but who
could still see, hear, and smell their mothers, showed a lower cortisol stress response, in
comparison to infants who were completely isolated from their mothers (Levine, Johnson, &
Gonzalez, 1985). Similarly, when infant rhesus macaques were separated from their mothers for
four days, they showed elevations in plasma cortisol (Bayart, Hayashi, Faull, Barchas, & Levine,
1990). This effect was attenuated, however, when infants were able to see their mothers. In
another study, infant rhesus monkeys who had been repeatedly separated from their mothers
between three and six months of age exhibited long-term changes in cortisol stress reactivity
during an acoustic startle paradigm (Sanchez et al., 2005). These effects of regulation of infant
biology appear to be specific to contact with the mother, as evidenced by findings that infant
monkeys who accidentally sought contact from another monkey experienced increases, rather
than decreases, in physiological arousal (Suomi, 1979).
Moreover, a number of studies have documented connections between the quality of
maternal care and infants’ biological and behavioral responses to threat (Coplan et al., 1996;
McCormack, Sanchez, Bardi, & Maestripieri, 2006). Coplan and colleagues (1996) created a
stressful caregiving environment for a sample of bonnet macaque infants and their mothers by
varying the stability of available food. Monkeys who had been raised by mothers in the variable
food condition had elevated corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) in cerebrospinal fluid in
adulthood. In comparison, monkeys who were raised in consistent, predictable environments –
characterized by either low or high levels of effort required to obtain food – had lower CRF
levels. Thus, these long-term changes in the biological stress system were not due simply to the
14
availability of food, but rather to the stress of uncertainty in the caregiving environment.
Similarly, monkeys who had been abused by their mothers showed dysregulation in
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis functioning (see Suomi, 2008, for a review). In
summary, it is clear that variations in early caregiving experiences play an important role in
regulating nonhuman primates’ physiological responses to threat.
Early Attachment-Related Experiences and Human Infant Biological Response to Stress
As discussed earlier, the infant’s experiences with an attachment figure contribute to the
development of representations about the availability of support when needed. These same
experiences also contribute to the development of a number of biologically based regulatory
processes. Our focus is on evidence of connections between infants’ interactions with their
mothers and HPA axis physiology, but we note that caregiving interactions also influence other
physiological systems, such as cardiac activity (e.g., Moore et al., 2009) and electrical activity in
the brain (e.g., Hane, Henderson, Reeb-Sutherland, & Fox, 2010).
A fully developed human stress response system is present at birth (see Adam, KlimesDougan, & Gunnar, 2007). This system becomes activated in response to stressful events by
releasing a dynamic cascade of chemicals, ultimately leading to the production of cortisol. In
response to stress, the hypothalamus releases corticotropin-releasing hormone and vasopressin.
These hormones, in turn, stimulate the anterior pituitary, which releases adrenocorticotropic
hormone, stimulating the adrenal glands to produce and release cortisol. Under normal
conditions, this system exhibits a diurnal rhythm, with higher levels of cortisol in the morning
and near zero levels by evening (Kirschbaum & Hellhammer, 1989; Watamura, Donzella, Kertes,
& Gunnar, 2004).
15
The HPA axis is sensitive to environmental and contextual perturbations in humans as it
is in rats and some nonhuman primates, and a growing body of research suggests that differences
in the quality of early caregiving experiences contribute to important variations in the initial
calibration and continued regulation of this system. Disruptions to normative caregiving
experiences can lead to a variety of dysregulated stress response patterns, including overactive
responses to stress and disruptions to the diurnal rhythm of cortisol production (see Adam et al.,
2007, and Gunnar & Donzella, 2002, for reviews). The regulation of the HPA axis in turn plays
an important role in shaping behavioral responses to threat (Jessop & Turner-Cobb, 2008;
Shirtcliff & Essex, 2008).
Researchers have examined the connections between caregiving experiences and infant
stress physiology by comparing infants’ cortisol levels before and after a stressful task (e.g., the
laboratory Strange Situation paradigm that contains two brief episodes of infant-mother
separation; Ainsworth et al., 1978). For example, Luijk and colleagues (2010) examined the
contributions of maternal depression and infant attachment security to infants’ cortisol stress
responses and found that insecure infants who also had depressed mothers showed the largest
stress response during the Strange Situation. Similarly, Nachmias, Gunnar, Mangelsdorf, Parritz,
and Buss (1996) found that inhibited toddlers who were insecurely attached to their caregivers
exhibited elevated cortisol following exposure to novel stimuli. In another study, Feldman and
colleagues (2009) found that low maternal sensitivity was associated with elevated infant cortisol
reactivity in response to fearful stimuli, and this effect was independent of maternal depressive
and anxiety symptoms. Additional evidence for the role of maternal influence in shaping the
infant’s stress response comes from experimental evidence showing that mothers’ touch buffered
infants’ cortisol stress response typically associated with a stressful experience (in this case,
16
during the still-face laboratory procedure in which mothers are asked to cease interaction with
their infants; Feldman, Singer, & Zagoory, 2010). Similar evidence that sensitive maternal care
can buffer stress reactivity comes from a study in which secure infants, relative to insecure
infants, had a lower cortisol response during the transition to a new daycare environment
(Ahnert, Gunnnar, Lamb, & Barthel, 2004).
Children living in violent families endure particularly stressful caregiving environments,
and such exposure is extremely dysregulating for children. The constant uncertainty and turmoil
of these environments has consequences for children’s stress reactivity (Taylor, Repetti, &
Seeman, 1997). A number of studies have documented the disrupted stress response of
maltreated children (e.g., Davies, Sturge-Apple, Cicchetti, & Cummings, 2007, 2008). Even
living in a family in which the violence does not involve them directly has negative
consequences for children, and some evidence suggests that the quality of caregiving in these
hostile environments plays an important role in modifying the stress response. For example, in a
longitudinal study of intimate partner violence (IPV) and children’s adrenocortical responses to
stress, Hibel and colleagues found that children who were exposed to IPV but whose mothers
demonstrated high levels of sensitivity were buffered from heightened cortisol reactivity,
compared to children who were exposed to IPV and low maternal sensitivity (Hibel, Granger,
Blair, Cox, & The FLP Investigators, 2011).
Attachment as a Regulator/Calibrator of Infant Stress Reactivity: Evolutionary
Underpinnings
In the species discussed in this chapter, the stress reactivity system serves to help the
organism mobilize protective resources in times of threat, and, in the absence of threat, to devote
metabolic and psychological resources elsewhere (e.g., food gathering, exploration). It seems
17
reasonable, therefore, that the most evolutionarily adaptive stress reactivity system would include
a component that can extract information about the organism’s environment, gauge the level of
environmental threat, and prompt an appropriate response. With high environmental threat,
greatest adaptation means a stress reactivity system that is calibrated to recognize, attend to, and
respond to threat quickly. Importantly, one central factor that demarks the extent of
environmental threat is the presence or absence of a reliably responsive caregiver; an
environment lacking a protective attachment figure is an environment of increased risk. Thus, for
an infant without a responsive attachment figure, adaptation means being vigilant to threat and
ready for a quick response.
Just as infants are thought to have evolved with the capacity to use experience-based
information about the availability of a protective caregiver to calibrate their attachment
behavioral system (Main, 1990), and given the close intertwining of the attachment and fear
systems as described above, it is likely that infants evolved with the capacity to use information
about the availability of an attachment figure to calibrate their threat response system at both the
behavioral and physiological levels (Cassidy, 2009). This thinking about the role of attachment in
the calibration of the infant’s stress reactivity emerges directly from Bowlby’s (1969/1982, 1973)
theories of attachment and fear (see also Kobak & Madsen, 2008; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007),
Hinde’s (1986) evolutionary thinking about parental signaling about the state of the environment,
and Main’s (1990) ideas of attachment strategies, as well as from the thinking of contemporary
evolutionary biologists (e.g., West-Eberhard, 2003). This thinking also is compatible with the
thinking of Flinn, Nepomnaschy, Muehlenbein, and Ponzi (2011) and Meaney (2010), and with
much of the empirical work described above (e.g., the link between maternal behavior and infant
18
DNA methylation in rats [Weaver et al., 2004] may have evolved because maternal licking and
grooming of rat pups signals the availability of an invested mother).
The Interaction of Representational and Non-representational Processes in Predicting
Children’s Response to Threat
Just as the study of the correlates of attachment security at multiple levels of analysis is
important, so too will examination of how cognitive processes interact with physiological
processes to influence child outcomes constitute a significant extension of attachment theory and
research. An important set of questions emerges: How are representational and physiological
processes linked and how do they influence each other? How do these two sets of processes work
together to predict child functioning? Does the nature of the interaction vary across particular
aspects of child functioning, and across developmental periods? How do these processes interact
during the initial attachment formation versus during maintenance of an attachment? How can
we understand these interactions in relation to both normative development and individual
differences? The growth of scientific knowledge since the time of Bowlby’s initial writings,
including findings from the studies described here, has brought many scientists to a working
assumption: Anything that influences representations influences many parts of the body and
brain.
The most fundamental question relates to how representational and non-representational
processes are linked. In humans, each process is thought to initiate the other in ways unlikely to
be the case in other species. Sapolsky (2004) noted that in humans, representational processes –
the anticipation of threat when none currently exists – can launch a stress response: “A stressor
can also be the anticipation of that happening. Sometimes we are smart enough to see things
[threats] coming and, based only on anticipation, can turn on a stress-response as robust as if the
19
event had actually occurred" (p. 6). Relatedly, Bowlby (1973), focusing on the link between
attachment and fear, specified representational “forecasts of availability or unavailability” of the
attachment figure as “a major variable that determines whether a person is or is not alarmed by
any potentially alarming situation” (p. 204). Thus, the representations that others will be
unavailable or rejecting when needed that characterize insecure attachment could contribute to
chronic activation of physiological stress response systems, as could the associated
representations of others as having hostile intent (see Dykas & Cassidy, 2011, for a review of the
social information processing patterns associated with secure and insecure attachment). Just as
representational processes are useful in the anticipation of threat, so are they useful in the
anticipation of protective resources that will dampen the stress reactivity system. Thus, in times
of both anticipated and actual threat, the capacity to represent a responsive attachment figure can
reduce physiological responses associated with threatening or painful experiences (for evidence
of reduced neural responses to painful stimuli when participants viewed pictures of their
attachment figure, see Eisenberger et al., 2011). Consideration of the linkage between
representational and non-representational processes must also contain a recognition that not only
can representations lead to both increases and decreases in physiological stress reactivity, but that
pathways of influence also can run in the opposite direction: Physiological stress responses can
lead an individual to engage in higher level cognitive process to understand, justify, or change the
stress (Festinger, 1957; Schacter & Singer, 1962).
When and how do young children come to use attachment-related representations as
regulators of stress? Both normative trajectories and individual differences in the developmental
course of the use of representations to influence stress reactivity have been rarely examined.
Evidence that stress dysregulation can lead to the conscious engagement of representational
20
processes comes from children as young as age four who are able to describe cognitive
mechanisms for alleviating distress (e.g., deliberately changing thoughts or goals, reappraising
the situation, mental distraction; Davis, Levine, Lench, & Quas, 2010; Sayfan & Lagattua, 2009).
Younger children may possess these capacities without being able to verbalize them. Moreover,
there is behavioral evidence of representations dampening distress in young children:
Representations of their mothers (in the form of photographs) were calming to 24-month-olds
experiencing a brief laboratory separation (inferred from greater engagement in play; Passman &
Longeway, 1982), and to young children experiencing extended separations of several days
(Robertson & Robertson, 1971, 1989).
Hofer (2008) and Suomi (2008) converge in their thinking about the biological
advantages of the ways in which representational and non-representational processes work
together in relation to human attachment. Hofer proposed that mechanisms wherein parents are
early physiological regulators of infant stress reactivity (which prepare infants for the
environment they are likely to face; he refers to this as the “predictive role of parenting;” see the
initial thinking of Hinde, 1986) will be most effective when they allow a means for “corrective
effects” (that is, input from later environments, some of which will involve representations) if the
later environment differs substantially from that predicted by earlier parent-child interaction.
Furthermore, Hofer (2006) called for future research to understand how specific components of
mother-infant interaction, such as touch, voice, and imitation, link both to representational and to
neural and hormonal processes, and how these processes interact to predict later functioning.
Hofer’s proposition that hidden regulators constitute some of the “building blocks” of
representations (1994, p. 205) has a compelling basis in the animal literature, and merits
examination in humans.
21
Relatedly, Suomi’s (2008) speculations about how physiological mechanisms and
cognitive representations interact contained the notion that advanced levels of cognition are not
needed for the complex workings of the attachment system within most primates, and that
humans have higher level cognitions superimposed on these other biological processes: “One
might argue that working models are exclusively human constructions that are built upon a basic
foundation that is essentially biological in nature and universal among the more advanced
primate species. Cognitive constructions per se may not be necessary for long-term
developmental or cross-generational continuities in attachment . . . That is, such continuities are
essentially “programmed” to occur in the absence of major environmental disruption and are in
fact the product of strictly biological processes that reflect the natural evolutionary history of
advanced primate species, human and nonhuman alike” (p. 186).
Suomi further described representational processes and underlying biology in relation to
both continuity and change: “Working models . . . may represent a luxury for humans that
enables individuals to cognitively reinforce the postulated underlying biological foundation, in
which case the predicted developmental continuity may actually be strengthened. On the other
hand, the existence of a working model that has the potential to be altered by specific experiences
(and/or insights) in late childhood, adolescence, or adulthood may provide a basis for breaking an
otherwise likely continuity between one’s early attachment experiences and subsequent
performance as a parent. These important issues deserve not only further theoretical
consideration, but empirical investigation as well” (p. 186-187).
From a historical perspective, it is interesting to note that Bowlby’s early work (e.g.,
1958), focusing on the evolutionary basis of the initial formation of the attachment bond,
contained no mention of representational processes – an understandable state of affairs given
22
Bowlby’s initial reliance on Harlow’s studies of infant rhesus monkeys separated from their
mothers (Harlow, 1958, 1960; see van der Horst, 2011). In fact, Bowlby clarified his “wish to
distinguish [his theory about separation anxiety] from states of anxiety dependent on foresight
[i.e., representations] (Bowlby, 1961, p. 267, cited in van der Horst, 2011, p. 120). It was only
later, when Bowlby turned his attention to individual differences in the quality of attachment
throughout the early years of human life, that the role of children’s representations as a
mechanism linking attachment and functioning came to be highlighted.
Summary and Additional Considerations
In this chapter, we described the ways in which the same experiences with caregivers
that contribute to infant attachment representations contribute to non-representational regulatory
processes, some of which reflect response to threat at the physiological level; in this way, early
caregiving experiences contribute to child response to threat directly, without involving
representational processes. Since the time of Bowlby’s original writing, there has been an
explosion of research examining these non-representational processes, and the idea that early
caregiving experiences influence both immediate and long-term behavioral and physiological
response to threat is well replicated and widely accepted with respect to some rodents, nonhuman
primates, and humans (for reviews, see Adam et al., 2007; Champagne & Curley, 2009; Loman
& Gunnar, 2010; Meaney, 2001; Polan & Hofer, 2008; Suomi, 2008).
Several important topics are beyond the scope of this chapter; we mention a few of these
here briefly. First, the focus of this chapter is on the early years of life. Bowlby, however,
emphasized the fact that attachment characterizes humans “from the cradle to the grave”
(1969/1982, p. 208). Relatedly, Hofer (1984) proposed that hidden regulators continue to
function throughout life, and suggested that the withdrawal of hidden regulators may account for
23
bereavement processes in adulthood. We speculate that hidden regulators play a role not only in
adult bereavement but also in more daily adult attachment processes (e.g., accounting for
alterations in sleep/wake patterns when a long-term romantic partner is away on a business trip).
Moreover, extensive evidence has emerged describing, for instance, the ways in which cortisol
dysregulation is associated with both problematic precursors and negative sequelae in older
children, adolescents, and adults (e.g., Ashman, Dawson, Panagiotides, Yamada, & Wilkinson,
2002; Barker, Greenberg, Seltzer, & Almeida, 2012; Marsman et al., 2008). Furthermore, if
evolution has provided organisms with the capacity to extract information from their
environments about the level of likely threat, it seems reasonable that this capacity would be
present as early in development as possible, including during the prenatal period. A recent surge
of theory and research has examined this phenomenon (called the fetal-programming model, fetal
origins hypothesis, or developmental origins of health and disease, see Ellison, 2010; see also,
Gluckman, Cutfield, Hofman, & Hanson, 2005, for the predictive-adaptive-response
model). Numerous studies indicate that exposure to prenatal adversity can “program” a variety of
biological systems in the offspring of both animals and humans (e.g., Clarke, Wittwer, Abbott, &
Schneider, 1994; Ravelli et al., 1998; van den Hove et al., 2006; for reviews, see Ellison, 2010;
Welberg & Seckl, 2001). One study of human newborns reported a link between prenatal
maternal depression and infants’ elevated cortisol levels (Field et al., 2004). Another intriguing
study in humans found that exposure to prenatal maternal depression led to epigenetic changes in
infants’ gene expression that were associated with elevated stress reactivity in the HPA axis
(Oberlander et al., 2008; see also Ponder et al., 2011). Relatedly, understanding of physiological
processes related to the intergenerational transmission of stress reactivity in humans, which has
been examined in rhesus monkeys (Suomi & Levine, 1998) and in rats (Francis, Diorio, Liu, &
24
Meaney, 1999), will be an important advance.
Second, in this chapter, we focused on the ways in which maternal caregiving
experiences contribute to the calibration and regulation of infant neuroendocrine responses to
threat, but it is likely that these same caregiving experiences also manifest themselves in a
number of other physiological changes. For example, variations in caregiving quality have been
shown to be associated with telomere length attrition (Drury et al., 2011), inflammatory profiles
(Chen, Miller, Kobor, & Cole, 2011) and sympathetic nervous system activity (Davis & Granger,
2009; Moore et al., 2009).
Third, Main noted that "[A]spects of the brain and/or brain chemistry particularly relevant
to fear experiences may provide us with one starting point for increasing our understanding not
only of the attachment system itself, but also of developmental differences originating in early
experiences closely combining attachment and fright" (1999, p. 855). The research that Main
calls for is particularly important given that the children most at risk for problematic
development and psychopathology are children who are in the untenable situation wherein the
person to whom they are biologically predisposed to turn to in times of threat (i.e., the attachment
figure) is also the source of the threat (from whom the child is biologically predisposed to flee).
Such a child is considered to face fright without solution (Hesse & Main, 2006, p. 310; see also
Cassidy & Mohr, 2001, and Lyons-Ruth & Jacobvitz, 2008).
Fourth, this chapter discusses only the behavioral and physiological levels of response
to threat; other levels should be considered. For instance, response to threat at the informationprocessing level (e.g., involving attention, memory) is likely to emerge from early attachmentrelated experiences (see Dykas & Cassidy, 2011, for a review). In addition, interconnections
among the levels of response to threat (e.g., links between the behavioral and physiological levels
25
of response to threat) are important to understand.
Fifth, a rich set of questions about clinical implications emerges from consideration of
the issues discussed in this chapter. Given the extent to which many forms of psychopathology
reflect problems of self-regulation in the face of stress (e.g., internalizing problems, externalizing
problems, substance abuse; Kring & Sloan, 2010), what can “hidden regulators” embedded
within human infant-mother interactions tell us about the precursors of psychopathology? What
about hidden regulators embedded within the relationship with the therapist (who, according to
Bowlby [1988], comes to serve as an attachment figure within the context of long-term
psychotherapy)? When change occurs following long-term therapy, does this change emerge
through representational mechanisms, through physiological regulation, or both (see also Stern,
2004)? What are the neural and physiological mechanisms associated with change? Do
therapeutic changes in representations contribute to these other neural and physiological
changes? Or could it be that it is the other neural and physiological changes that drive the change
in representations? (See Beebe, Knoblauch, Rustin, & Sorter, 2005, for a perspective wherein
implicit, nonverbal, and nonconscious components of therapy are essential contributors to
change; see also Pally, 2000). In relation to early parenting interventions designed to reduce the
risk of insecure infant attachment, which have typically aimed to change maternal representations
and/or behavior with little attention to hidden regulators (see Anisfeld, Casper, Nozyce, &
Cunningham, 1990, for an exception): How should consideration of maternal regulators of infant
physiology be included in the intervention? What child physiological changes are evident
following parenting interventions (see Dozier, Peloso, Lewis, Laurenceau, & Levine, 2008, for
evidence of children’s increased cortisol regulation following an intervention with foster
parents)?
26
We end by noting that the theory and data reviewed here are fully compatible with the
earliest thinking of Bowlby, whose scientific roots were in biology. Yet in Bowlby’s lifetime,
tools for examining neural and physiological processes that scientists now possess were
unavailable – as was even the concept of experiential effects on gene expression. We imagine
that Bowlby would have found these newest developments enormously exciting.
Notes
1. Preparation of this manuscript was supported by a grant from the National Institute on Drug
Abuse to Jude Cassidy (DA25550), by a Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service
Award to Katherine Ehrlich (DA027365), and by an Ann G. Wylie Dissertation Fellowship to
Laura Sherman.
2. An important part of the secure base/safe haven construct is that during times of threat, not
only is the fear system activated and attachment behavior increased, but exploratory behavior
also decreases (Ainsworth et al., 1978).
References
Adam, E. K., Klimes-Dougan, B., & Gunnar, M. R. (2007). Social regulation of the
adrenocortical response to stress in infants, children, and adolescents. In D. Coch, G.
Dawson, & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain: Atypical
development (2nd ed., pp. 264-304). New York, NY: Guilford.
27
Ahnert, L., Gunnar, M. R., Lamb, M. E., & Barthel, M. (2004). Transition to child care:
Associations with infant-mother attachment, infant negative emotion, and cortisol
elevations. Child Development, 75(3), 639-650.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1963). The development of infant-mother interaction among the Ganda. In
B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 67-104). New York: Wiley.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of love. Baltimore,
MD: Johns Hopkins Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Attachment and dep: A comparison. In J. L. Gewirtz (Ed.),
Attachment and dependency (pp. 97-137). Washington, DC: V. H. Winston & Sons.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A
psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and the exploratory behaviour of oneyear-olds in a strange situation. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour
(Vol. 4, pp. 113-136). London: Methuen.
Anisfeld, E., Casper, V., Nozyce, M., & Cunningham, N. (1990). Does infant carrying promote
attachment? An experimental study of the effects of increased physical contact on the
development of attachment. Child Development, 61(5), 1617-1627.
Ashman, S. B., Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., Yamada, E., & Wilkinson, C. W. (2002). Stress
hormone levels of children of depressed mothers. Development & Psychopathology,
14(2), 333-349.
Barker, E. T., Greenberg, J. S., Seltzer, M., & Almeida, D. M. (2012). Daily stress and cortisol
patterns in parents of adult children with a serious mental illness. Health Psychology,
31(1), 130-134.
28
Bayart, F., Hayashi, K. T., Faull, K. F., Barchas, J. D., & Levine, S. (1990). Influence of maternal
proximity on behavioral and physiological responses to separation in infant rhesus
monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behavioral Neuroscience, 104(1), 98-107.
Beebe, B., Knoblauch, S., Rustin, J., & Sorter, D. (2005). Forms of intersubjectivity in infant
research and adult treatment. New York, NY: Other Press.
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child's tie to his mother. The International Journal Of
Psychoanalysis, 39, 350-373.
Bowlby, J. (1961). Separation anxiety: A critical review of the literature. Journal Of Child
Psychology & Psychiatry, 1(4), 251-269.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London, UK: Tavistock
Publications Limited.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development.
New York, NY: Basic Books.
Boyce, W., Champoux, M., Suomi, S. J., & Gunnar, M. R. (1995). Salivary cortisol in nurseryreared rhesus monkeys: Reactivity to peer interactions and altered circadian activity.
Developmental Psychobiology, 28(5), 257-267.
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. (2008). Internal working models in attachment relationships:
Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.),
29
Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 102127). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Caldji, C., Tannenbaum, B., Sharma, S., Francis, D., Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J. (1998).
Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the
expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
95(9), 5335-5340.
Carr, S. J., Dabbs, J. M., & Carr, T. S. (1975). Mother-infant attachment: The importance of the
mother's visual field. Child Development, 46(2), 331-338.
Cassidy, J. (2009, March). New directions in attachment theory and research. Paper presented at
the Attachment Pre-conference of the meetings of the Society for Research in Child
Development, Denver.
Cassidy, J., & Mohr, J. J. (2001). Unsolvable fear, trauma, and psychopathology: Theory,
research, and clinical considerations related to disorganized attachment across the life
span. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8(3), 275-298.
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical
applications (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Champagne, F. A., & Curley, J. P. (2009). Epigenetic mechanisms mediating the long-term
effects of maternal care on development. Neuroscience And Biobehavioral Reviews,
33(4), 593-600.
Chen, E., Miller, G. E., Kobor, M. S., & Cole, S. W. (2011). Maternal warmth buffers the effects
of low early-life socioeconomic status on pro-inflammatory signaling in adulthood.
Molecular Psychiatry, 16(7), 729-737.
30
Clarke, A., Wittwer, D. J., Abbott, D. H., & Schneider, M. L. (1994). Long-term effects of
prenatal stress on HPA axis activity in juvenile rhesus monkeys. Developmental
Psychobiology, 27(5), 257-269.
Coplan, J. D., Andrews, M. W., Rosenblum, L. A., Owens, M. J., Friedman, S., Gorman, J. M.,
& Nemeroff, C. B. (1996). Persistent elevations in cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of
corticotrophin-releasing factor in adult nonhuman primates exposed to early-life stressors:
Implications for the pathophysiology of mood and anxiety disorders. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 93(4), 1619-1623.
Davies, P. T., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (2007). The role of child
adrenocortical functioning in pathways between interparental conflict and child
maladjustment. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 918-930.
Davies, P. T., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (2008). Adrenocortical
underpinnings of children’s psychological reactivity to interparental conflict. Child
Development, 79(6), 1693-1706.
Davis, E. P., & Granger, D. A. (2009). Developmental differences in infant salivary alphaamylase and cortisol responses to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(6), 795-804.
Davis, E. L., Levine, L. J., Lench, H. C., & Quas, J. A. (2010). Metacognitive emotion
regulation: Children's awareness that changing thoughts and goals can alleviate negative
emotions. Emotion, 10(4), 498-510.
Dozier, M., Peloso, E., Lewis, E., Laurenceau, J., & Levine, S. (2008). Effects of an attachmentbased intervention of the cortisol production of infants and toddlers in foster care.
Development And Psychopathology, 20(3), 845-859.
31
Dozier, M., & Rutter, M. (2008). Challenges to the development of attachment relationships
faced by young children in foster and adoptive care. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.),
Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 698717). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Drury, S. S., Theall, K., Gleason, M. M., Smyke, A. T., DeVivo, I., Wong, J. Y. Y. . . . & Nelson,
C. A. (2011). Telomere length and early severe social deprivation: Linking early adversity
and cellular aging. Molecular Psychiatry. Advance online publication.
doi:10.1038/mp.2011.53
Dykas, M. J., & Cassidy, J. (2011). Attachment and the processing of social information across
the life span: Theory and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 137(1), 19-46.
Eisenberger, N. I., Master, S. L., Inagaki, T. K., Taylor, S. E., Shirinyan, D., Lieberman, M. D.,
& Naliboff, B. D. (2011). Attachment figures activate a safety signal-related neural region
and reduce pain experience. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(28),
11721-11726.
Ellison, P. (2010). Fetal programming and fetal psychology. Infant & Child Development, 19(1),
6-20.
Feldman, R., Granat, A., Pariente, C., Kanety, H., Kuint, J., & Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2009).
Maternal depression and anxiety across the postpartum year and infant social
engagement, fear regulation, and stress reactivity. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(9), 919-927.
Feldman, R., Singer, M., & Zagoory, O. (2010). Touch attenuates infants’ physiological
reactivity to stress. Developmental Science, 13(2), 271-278.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
32
Field, T., Diego, M., Hernandez-Reif, M., Vera, Y., Gil, K., Schanberg, S., & ... GonzalezGarcia, A. (2004). Prenatal maternal biochemistry predicts neonatal biochemistry.
International Journal of Neuroscience, 114(8), 933-945.
Flinn, M. V., Nepomnaschy, P. A., Muehlenbein, M. P., & Ponzi, D. (2011). Evolutionary
functions of early social modulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis development
in humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(7), 1611-1629.
Francis, D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across
generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science, 286, 1155-1158.
Freud, S. (1953). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. In J Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The
standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 7).
London, England: Hogarth. (Original work published 1905)
Freud, S. (1953). Introductory lectures on psycho-analysis. Part III. In J Strachey (Ed. & Trans.),
The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 16).
London, England: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1917)
Gluckman, P. D., Cutfield, W., Hofman, P., & Hanson, M. A. (2005). The fetal, neonatal, and
infant environments--the long-term consequences for disease risk. Early Human
Development, 81(1), 51-59.
Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B. (2002). Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human
development. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27(1-2), 199-220.
Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Ordinary
variations in human maternal caregiving in infancy and biobehavioral development in
early childhood: A follow-up study. Developmental Psychobiology, 52(6), 558-567.
Harlow, H. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13(12), 673-685.
33
Harlow, H. (1960). Primary affectional patterns in primates. American Journal of
Orthopsychitary, 30, 676-684.
Hesse, E., & Main, M. (2006). Frightened, threatening, and dissociative parental behavior in lowrisk samples: Description, discussion, and interpretations. Development and
Psychopathology, 18(2), 309-343.
Hibel, L. C., Granger, D. A., Blair, C., Cox, M. J., & the FLP Investigators (2011). Maternal
sensitivity buffers the adrenocortical implications of intimate partner violence exposure
during early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 23(2), 689-701.
Hinde, R. A. (1986). Some implications of evolutionary theory and comparative data for the
study of human prosocial and aggressive behaviour. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. RadkeYarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial and prosocial behaviour (pp. 13–32). Orlando,
FL: Academic Press.
Hofer, M. A. (1970). Physiological responses of infant rats to separation from their mothers.
Science, 168, 870-873.
Hofer, M. A. (1984). Relationships as regulators: A psychobiologic perspective on bereavement.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 46(3), 183-197.
Hofer, M. A. (1994). Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. In N. Fox (Ed.), The
development of emotion regulation, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 59, (2-3, Serial No. 240), 192-207.
Hofer, M. A. (2002). The riddle of development. In D. J. Lewkowicz & R. Lickliter (Eds.),
Conceptions of Development (pp. 5-30). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
Hofer, M. A. (2006). Psychobiological roots of early attachment. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 15(2), 84-88.
34
Jessop, D. S., & Turner-Cobb, J. M. (2008). Measurement and meaning of salivary cortisol: A
focus on health and disease in children. Stress, 11(1), 1-14.
Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S., & Chen, F. S. (2007). Evidence for infants' internal working
models of attachment. Psychological Science, 18(6), 501-502.
Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S., Chen, F. S., Stern, H. L., Ok, S., & Barth, M. (2010). At the
intersection of social and cognitive development: Internal working models of attachment
in infancy. Cognitive Science, 34(5), 807-825.
Kirschbaum, C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1989). Salivary cortisol in psychobiological research: An
overview. Neuropsychobiology, 22(3), 150-169.
Kobak, R., & Madsen, S. (2008). Disruptions in attachment bonds: Implications for theory,
research, and clinical intervention. In J. Cassidy, P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 23-47). New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Kochanska, G. (2001). Emotional development in children with different attachment histories:
The first three years. Child Development, 72(2), 474-490.
Kring, A., & Sloan, D. (Eds.). (2010). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A
transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Levine, S. (1957). Infantile experience and resistance to physiological stress. Science, 126, 405.
Levine, S. (2001). Primary social relationships influence the development of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal axis in the rat. Physiology & Behavior, 73(3), 255-260.
Levine, S. (2005). Developmental determinants of sensitivity and resistance to stress.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(10), 939-946.
35
Levine, S., Johnson, D. F., & Gonzalez, C. A. (1985). Behavioral and hormonal responses to
separation in infant rhesus monkeys and mothers. Behavioral Neuroscience, 99(3), 399410.
Levine, S., & Wiener, S. G. (1988). Psychoendocrine aspects of mother-infant relationships in
nonhuman primates. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 13(1-2), 143-154.
Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freedman, A, Sharma, S., . . .
Meaney, M. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression
and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science, 277(5332), 1659-1662.
Loman, M. M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2010). Early experience and the development of stress
reactivity and regulation in children. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 867876.
Luijk, M. P. C. M., Saridjan, N., Tharner, A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg,
M. J., Jaddoe, V. W. V., … Tiemeier, H. (2010). Attachment, depression, and cortisol:
Deviant patterns in insecure-resistant and disorganized infants. Developmental
Psychobiology, 52(5), 441-452.
Lyons-Ruth, K., & Jacobvitz, D. (2008). Attachment disorganization: Genetic factors, parenting
contexts, and developmental transformation from infancy to adulthood. In J. Cassidy & P.
R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications
(2nd ed., pp. 666-697). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Main, M. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of attachment organization: Recent studies, changing
methodologies, and the concept of conditional strategies. Human Development, 33(1), 4861.
36
Main, M. (1999). Attachment theory: Eighteen points with suggestions for future studies. In J.
Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment. (pp. 845-887). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A
move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points
of attachment theory and research, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 50(Serial No. 209), 66-104.
Marsman, R., Swinkels, S. N., Rosmalen, J. M., Oldehinkel, A. J., Ormel, J., & Buitelaar, J. K.
(2008). HPA-axis activity and externalizing behavior problems in early adolescents from
the general population: The role of comorbidity and gender: The TRAILS study.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(6), 789-798.
McCormack, K., Sanchez, M. M., Bardi, M., & Maestripieri, D. (2006). Maternal care patterns
and behavioral development of rhesus macaque abused infants in the first 6 months of
life. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(7), 537-550.
Meaney, M. J. (2001). Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual
differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annual Reviews Neuroscience, 24,
1161-1192.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and
change. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Moore, G. A., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., Propper, C. B., Calkins, S. D., Mills-Koonce, W. R., &
Cox, M. J. (2009). Mother-infant vagal regulation in the face-to-face still-face paradigm
is moderated by maternal sensitivity. Child Development, 80(1), 209-223.
37
Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M., Mangelsdorf, S., Parritz, R. H., & Buss, K. (1996). Behavioral
inhibition and stress reactivity: The moderating role of attachment security. Child
Development, 67(2), 508-522.
Oberlander, T., Weinberg, J., Papsdorf, M., Grunau, R., Misri, S., & Devlin, A. (2008). Prenatal
exposure to maternal depression, neonatal methylation of human glucocorticoid receptor
gene (NR3C1) and infant cortisol stress responses. Epigenetics: Official Journal Of The
DNA Methylation Society, 3(2), 97-106.
Passman, R. H., & Longeway, K. P. (1982). The role of vision in maternal attachment: Giving 2year-olds a photograph of their mother during separation. Developmental Psychology,
18(4), 530-533.
Pally, R. (2000). The mind-brain relationship. New York, NY: Other Press.
Polan, H., & Hofer, M. A. (2008). Psychobiological origins of infant attachment and its role in
development. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 158-172). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Ponder, K. L., Salisbury, A., McGonnigal, B., Laliberte, A., Lester, B., & Padbury, J. F. (2011).
Maternal depression and anxiety are associated with altered gene expression in the human
placenta without modification by antidepressant use: Implications for fetal programming.
Developmental Psychobiology, 53(7), 711-723.
Ravelli, A. J., Van der Meulen, J. P., Michels, R. J., Osmond, C. C., Barker, D. P., Hales, C. N.,
& Bleker, O. P. (1998). Glucose tolerance in adults after prenatal exposure to famine.
Lancet, 351(9097), 173-177.
38
Reite, M., Kaufman, C., Pauley, D., & Stynes, A. J. (1974). Depression in infant monkeys:
Physiological correlates. Psychosomatic Medicine, 36(4), 363-367.
Robertson, J., & Robertson, J. (1971). Thomas, aged two years four months, in foster care for ten
days. In Young Children in Brief Separation Film Series. Concord Video and Film
Council.
Robertson, J., & Robertson, J. (1989). Separation and the very young. Oxford England: Free
Association Books.
Sanchez, M. M., Noble, P. M., Lyon, C. K., Plotsky, P. M., Davis, M., Nemeroff, C. B., &
Winslow, J. T. (2005). Alterations in diurnal cortisol rhythm and acoustic startle response
in nonhuman primates with adverse rearing. Biological Psychiatry, 57(4), 373-381.
Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Why zebras don’t get ulcers (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Henry Holt and
Company, LLC.
Saridjan, N. S., Huizink, A. C., Koetsier, J. A., Jaddoe, V. W., Mackenbach, J. P., Hofman, A.,
… & Tiemeier, H. (2010). Do social disadvantage and early family adversity affect the
diurnal cortisol rhythm in infants? The Generation R Study. Hormones and Behavior,
57(2), 247-254.
Sayfan, L., & Lagattuta, K. (2009). Scaring the monster away: What children know about
managing fears of real and imaginary creatures. Child Development, 80(6), 1756-1774.
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional
state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379–399.
Shamir-Essakow, G., Ungerer, J. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2005). Attachment, behavioral inhibition,
and anxiety in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An Official
39
Publication of the International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent
Psychopathology, 33(2), 131-143.
Shirtcliff, E. A., & Essex, M. J. (2008). Concurrent and longitudinal associations of basal and
diurnal cortisol with mental health symptoms in early adolescence. Developmental
Psychobiology, 50(7), 690-703.
Sorce, J. F., & Emde, R. N. (1981). Mother's presence is not enough: Effect of emotional
availability on infant exploration. Developmental Psychology, 17(6), 737-745.
Stanton, M. E., Gutierrez, Y. R., & Levine, S. (1988). Maternal deprivation potentiates pituitaryadrenal stress responses in infant rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102(5), 692-700.
Stanton, M. E., Wallstrom, J., & Levine, S. (1987). Maternal contact inhibits pituitary-adrenal
stress responses in preweanling rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 20(2), 131-145.
Stern, D. (2004). The present moment in psychotherapy and everyday life. New York, NY: W.
W. Norton.
Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Shouldice, A. (1990). Fear and attachment in 2.5-year-olds. British
Journal Of Developmental Psychology, 8(4), 319-333.
Suchecki, D., Rosenfeld, P., & Levine, S. (1993). Maternal regulation of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal axis in the infant rat: The roles of feeding and stroking. Developmental
Brain Research, 75(2), 185-192.
Suomi, S. J. (1979). Differential development of various social relationships by rhesus monkey
infants. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), Genesis of behavior: The child and its
family (Vol. 2, pp. 219-244). New York: Plenum Press.
40
Suomi, S. J. (2008). Attachment in rhesus monkeys. In J. Cassidy, P. R. Shaver, J. Cassidy, P. R.
Shaver (Eds.) , Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd
ed., pp. 173-191). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Suomi, S. J., & Levine, S. (1998). Psychobiology of intergenerational effects of trauma:
Evidence from animal studies. In Y. Danieli (Ed.), International handbook of
multigenerational legacies of trauma (pp. 623-637). New York, NY: Plenum.
Taylor, S. E., Repetti, R. L., & Seeman, T. E. (1997). Health psychology: What is an unhealthy
environment and how does it get under the skin? Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 411447.
Thompson, R. A. (2008). Attachment-related mental representations: Introduction to the special
issue. Attachment & Human Development, 10(4), 347-538.
van den Hove, D. A., Steinbusch, H. M., Scheepens, A. A., Van de Berg, W. J., Kooiman, L. M.,
Boosten, B. G., & ... Blanco, C. E. (2006). Prenatal stress and neonatal rat brain
development. Neuroscience, 137(1), 145-155.
van der Horst, F. C. P. (2011). John Bowlby: From psychoanalysis to ethology. Chichester, West
Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
Vaughn, B. E., Egeland, B. R., Sroufe, L., & Waters, E. (1979). Individual differences in infant–
mother attachment at twelve and eighteen months: Stability and change in families under
stress. Child Development, 50(4), 971-975.
Watamura, S. E., Donzella, B., Kertes, D. A., & Gunnar, M. R. (2004). Developmental changes
in baseline cortisol activity in early childhood: Relations with napping and effortful
control. Developmental Psychobiology, 45(3), 125-133.
41
Waters, H. S., & Waters, E. (2006). The attachment working models concept: Among other
things, we build script-like representations of secure base experiences. Attachment &
Human Development, 8(3), 185-197.
Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D’Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R. . . .
& Meaney, M. J. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature
Neuroscience, 7(8), 847-854.
Weinfield, N. S., Sroufe, L., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from infancy to early adulthood
in a high-risk sample: Continuity, discontinuity, and their correlates. Child Development,
71(3), 695-702.
Welberg, L. M., & Seckl, J. R. (2001). Prenatal Stress, Glucocorticoids and the Programming of
the Brain. Journal Of Neuroendocrinology, 13(2), 113-128.
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Zeanah, C. H., Smyke, A. T., Koga, S. F., & Carlson, E. (2005). Attachment in Institutionalized
and Community Children in Romania. Child Development, 76(5), 1015-1028.
Download