African-American Stereotypes in Advertising and its Effects on Society Grace E. Curry Savannah State University Abstract: Advertising aims to adapt to the ever-changing consumer perceptions of all races, sexes, ethnicities and attitudes of consumers worldwide. Yet to date there are still advertisements that portray persons of color in either a stereotypical manner or as background to European Americans. For instance, there are two different commercials for the same product that depicts an African American and European American family having dinner. In the commercial with the African American family, only the mother is having dinner with her kids. However, in the commercial with the European American family, the mother and father are having dinner with their kids. This research study explores and investigates the role of advertising in targeting African American consumers through stereotypes and further examines the following questions. What affects do today’s commercial advertisements have on race relations? • • • Do the stereotypes project the true feelings of African American population? Are these types of stereotypical advertisements harmful to the self-esteem of people of color, and how do these perceptions affect different ethnicities including gender? Should advertisers be held responsible for the continued perpetuation of the stereotypes? Keywords: Stereotypes, minorities, race, gender, advertisement INTRODUCTION The racial and ethnic composition of America has changed considerable from a decade ago. And this composition is going to continue to change decades into the future. With the influx of immigrants from all over the globe, America is becoming browner. Yet, it does not seem as though many advertisers are paying close attention to this undeniable fact. Although the representation of ethnic groups in advertisements have increased in recent years (Zinkhan et al, 1988), it is still not enough. There are still less representation of people of color in television and magazine advertisements. And when people of color are represented there is a hint of covert stereotyping. Stereotyping is a psychological categorization of specific social groups held by the general public which influences decision making and information processing tasks (Gaertner and McLaughlin, 1983; Bodenhausen, 1988; Greenwald and Banaji, 1995). Stereotyping has a negative association with a particular culture or race. However, there is such a term as positive stereotyping. Positive stereotypes point out the “positive” attributes of a culture or ethic group e.g., Asian are great at math, AfricanAmericans are better at sports, or the “Latin Lover.” However, positive stereotypes are just as damaging as “negative stereotypes.” For example, if Asians are smarter at math, does that man that blacks and whites are not? Additionally, when saying that African Americans are better at sports, does that mean they don’t have the mental capacities to be intelligent? And “positive stereotypes” can cause unnecessary stress to the persons who are trying to live up to them. Stereotypes as a whole are dangerous because they lump one group of people unfairly into a specific category. African Americans currently face the same stereotypes from times passed. Many African Americans in television advertisements are seen, but not heard. However, when they are heard they share the advertisement with a white counterpart. This is noteworthy; television advertisements merit particular attention because they are key components of television, and because as Entman and Rojecki (2000) contended, they are “indicators of the culture’s racial heartbeat” (p. 162), representing both cultural norms but also having the potential to improve racial relations (Henderson and Baldasty, 2003). Advertisers may not realize how important of a role they play when it comes to stereotyping African Americans, and are not intentionally producing stereotypical advertisements. Their goal is to get information out about a product or service to their market segment. But advertisers need to pay attention to who they are marketing to, and let their market segment assist with determining if a particular print or commercial may be perceived as stereotypical, especially if the market segmentation is African American. Advertisers have a strong influence on shaping the perceptions of people, based on the how characters in the advertisements are portrayed. Furthermore, advertisements have the ability to either help eradicate the negative perceptions of African-Americans, or they can facilitate pervasive stereotypes, which may increase racism. There is also more at risk. Negative stereotypes of African American in advertising, may affect the self-esteem of African Americans. The continuation of minimal representation or negative depictions of oneself reflected in media may cause many African Americans to question their self-worth and importance to society. More importantly, these destructive images or lack thereof affect African American children exponentially, since African American children watch considerably more television than Caucasian children (Slaughter, 2003). It is also important to note that children cannot fully differentiate what is fact and what is fiction when watching television. Advertisers must realize that more is at stake than selling a product or service. There are also social responsibilities to contend with. Currently, African Americans have the purchasing power of approximately $910 billion, and this figure is expected to increase to $1.2 trillion by the year 2012. The buying power of African Americans is further reason why advertisers must revisit how they are targeting their markets. Not only is nonstereotypical advertising a matter of social responsibility, but it can quite possibly affect the bottom line. It is also apparent that advertisers are not acknowledging the changing demographics when it comes to targeting African Americans. In this research study the stereotyping of African American in advertisements will be explored. This stereotyping not only affects African Americans, but all Americans as a whole. The goal of this research is to examine how advertising stereotypes propagate racism, its effects on society and determine if advertisers share a responsibility in the perpetuation of stereotypes or if they are simply providing Americans with what they want. LITERATURE REVIEW Stereotypes Gaertner and McLaughlin (1983), Bodenhausen (1988), Greenwald and Banaji (1995), as stated earlier, have defined stereotyping is as a psychological categorization of specific social groups held by general public which influences decision making and information processing tasks. Stereotyping has been further dichotomized into polarized appraisal theory (Linville, 1982; Linville and Jones, 1980) or in- group bias theory (Brewer, 1979; Wilder, 1981). These theories assume that people categorized individuals on whether they belong to the in-group or out-group (Qualls and Moore, 1990). In-groups are those that look like the persons doing the categorization and out-group are those that look dissimilar to the person doing the categorization. Figure 1: African Americans in marginal roles (1924 & 2011) African American stereotypes have been studied for many decades. Shuey et al. reported that .6% of African Americans were in advertisements, 95.3 % of the time they were represented as unskilled laborers, entertainers or athletes (1953). Kern-Foxworth expressed that people of color are frequently not present; when they are, it is often in marginal or stereotypical roles (1994). One may wonder how the absence of African Americans my lead to stereotypes. One explanation is dominant group ideology. Hirschman (1993, 537), defines ideology as a widely shared “system of beliefs that emanate from the promulgate” a certain world view and that ideology is one means by which dominant groups sustain and dominate their power over other groups. It is possible to communicate dominant group ideology through media. Duckitt states that media can communicate racial prejudice in a number of ways, including omission (i.e., ignoring the existence of African Americans), stereotyping and showing African Americans in disproportionate number of “bad” or low status roles (1992). As Bristor et al. stated, it is important to bring attention to these types of covert and possible unintentional negative portrayals of African Americans in order to readdress race-based inequalities (1995). Additionally, racism in advertising is reflected by low minority visibility and the use of certain stereotypes. When referring to advertisements for television, Bristol et al. reported that only 17% of African Americans were featured in minor roles (1995). However, Taylor and Lee found consolatory gains in the frequency of African Americans (11% of ads used African American models), as well as improvements in role portrayals and products advertised. However, these gains may not be enough. Although the frequency of African Americans appearing in print ads has increased, there still is an imbalance of positive portrayals. As compared to Caucasian Americans, African Americans are portrayed more as minimum wage workers or single mothers in advertisements. There should be a rise in the number of African-Americans being portrayed as professionals, at least to the same extent as Caucasian Americans. Bandura opines that media is an important environmental influence on human functioning and the way character roles are portrayed on television influences how viewers interpret and respond to characters (1977, 1986, and 2002). All images of African Americans can shape and strengthen postulations of those who have limited interactions with them. And if more negative portrayals are presented via advertisements, stereotypes of African Americans will persist. In order to prevent these negative assumptions, Bristor et al. suggests African Americans should be portrayed in roles other than successful athletes, but also in high status roles, such as managers and other professional (1995). Most importantly, the consideration of African American seeing these advertisements must be taken into account. Advertisement that portray African Americans negatively or maintain stereotypes may also affect the self-esteem of African Americans. Another apprehension that should be expressed is the possible loss of revenue advertisers may undergo from the African American community. African Americans, like any other population, are more likely to purchase products or services from companies that have persons in advertisements that look like them. Whittler and Dimeo (1991) suggest that advertisers cannot afford to offend African Americans by not including them in their commercial messages. African American buying power has increased 166% from 1990-2007 compared to a 124% in White buying power (Selig Center, 2007). Therefore, it also seems plausible that the commercial messages should not contain any negative stereotypes. African Americans are brand loyal, demonstrate a high propensity for big ticket purchases, and prefer to support companies that embrace diversity (Miller & Kemp, 2005). The purpose of this paper is to examine exactly what type damage may come about due to advertising stereotypes of African Americans. Additionally, this research will answer the following questions: • Do the stereotypes project the true feelings of the African American population? • Are these types of stereotypical advertisements harmful to the self-esteem of people of color and how do these perceptions affect different ethnicities including gender? • Should advertisers be held responsible for the continued perpetuation of the stereotypes? CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK Ad Evoked Feelings Stereotypes Attitude towards Ad Attitude towards Brands Figure: 2: Stereotypes Conceptual Framework Purchase Intentions Nearly 70% of African Americans are concerned with how they are portrayed in advertising (Beasty, 2005) and almost 60% believe that print and television advertisements are intended for solely for white people (Mueller, 2008). The Stereotypes Hypothesis Model expounds upon the relationship between stereotypes, ad evoked feelings, attitudes towards ads, attitudes towards brands and purchase intentions. Figure 2 demonstrates the conceptual framework. The conceptualization is that there is a direct relationship of stereotypes with ad evoked feelings, attitude towards the ad, and attitude towards the brand, which all will affect current or future intentions to purchase a product. Figure 3: African Americans portrayed in over-exaggerated roles in 1940 and 2011 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY A sample of 72 students from a Historically Black College University (HBCU) was surveyed for the purpose of this research. Out of the 72 students surveyed 60% were women and 81.9% were African American, and 9.7% were Caucasian American. Ten health and fashion magazines (Seventeen, Shape, Men’s Health, Women’s Health, Oprah, Allure, Elle, Details, Lucky, and Cosmopolitan) were examined, which resulted in 60 advertisements. Table 1 below provides details of the sample. Table 1: Gender, Age and Ethnicity of the Sample Gender Frequency Percent Cumulative Valid Percent Percent 29 40.3 40.3 40.3 Female 43 59.7 59.7 100.0 Total 100.0 100.0 Valid Male 72 Age Frequency Percent Cumulative Valid Percent Percent Valid 21 - 25 50 69.4 69.4 69.4 26 - 30 14 19.4 19.4 88.9 31 - 35 6 8.3 8.3 97.2 36 - 40 2 2.8 2.8 100.0 Total 100.0 100.0 72 Ethnicity Valid Frequency Percent Cumulative Valid Percent Percent African American 59 81.9 84.3 84.3 White (Caucasian) 7 9.7 10.0 94.3 Asian 1 1.4 1.4 95.7 Native Hawaiian or 1 other Pacific Islander 1.4 1.4 97.1 Other 2 2.8 2.9 100.0 70 2 72 97.2 2.8 100.0 100.0 Total Missing System Total Qualitative Stage – 60 ads/stimuli were presented to a panel of 21 students and five faculty members. The purpose of this qualitative stage was to determine which ads contained negative stereotypical portrayals of African Americans. The occurrences of negative stereotypical portrayals were counted and gave rise to five ads/stimuli that were nominated – one of an African American male portrayed in a marginal role, two of African-American women in erotic roles, and two of African-American women in over-exaggerated portrayals. Quantitative Stage – These five ads were formulated into an online survey to measure attitudes and behaviors amongst males and females. Seventy-two students participated in the survey, in random order. The population at a HBCU is mostly homogenous with parallel socio-demographic backgrounds. Gender and was included as a cataloging question. The findings from this research were indiscriminate for males versus females. DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS A one-way analysis of variance was conducted to explore the impact of positive and negative cultural meanings on gender, as shown in Table 2. Figure 4 shows the cultural ad-interpretation differences between males and females. Stereotypes generate different responses amongst males and females, as shown in Table 2 and Figure 4. Figure 2: Gender Based cultural ad-interpretation differences Table 2: ANOVA Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Does the ad provide a Between Groups different but POSITIVE Within Groups meaning when applied to different (your) culture/sub- Total culture? 3.691 1 3.691 3.226 .077 78.956 69 1.144 82.648 70 Does the ad provide a Between Groups different but NEGATIVE Within Groups meaning when applied to different (your) culture/sub- Total culture? 11.726 1 11.726 13.436 .000 60.218 69 .873 71.944 70 View the ad again, at least view it 3 more times – on repeated viewings, do you encounter different POSITIVE messages? Between Groups 7.290 1 7.290 8.029 .006 Within Groups 62.654 69 .908 Total 69.944 70 View the ad again, at least view it 3 more times – on repeated viewings, do you encounter different but NEGATIVE messages? Between Groups 11.060 1 11.060 13.201 .001 Within Groups 57.813 69 .838 Total 68.873 70 Table 3 provides the male and female ad-interpretation differences in detail as below. Table 3: Cultural Ad-Interpretation Differences by Gender Gender Does the ad provide a different but POSITIVE meaning when applied to different (your) culture/sub-culture? Does the ad provide a different but NEGATIVE meaning when applied to different (your) culture/sub-culture? Male Female Male Female View the ad again, at least Male view it 3 more times – on repeated viewings, do you encounter different Female POSITIVE messages? Statistic Std. Error Mean 2.3448 .18752 Skewness .122 .434 Kurtosis -1.022 .845 Mean 1.8810 .17109 Skewness .922 .365 Kurtosis -.578 .717 Mean 2.4828 .17631 Skewness .187 .434 Kurtosis -.788 .845 Mean 3.3095 .14252 Skewness -1.064 .365 Kurtosis -.011 .717 Mean 2.4138 .16843 Skewness .275 .434 Kurtosis -.562 .845 Mean 1.7619 .15168 Skewness 1.156 .365 View the ad again, at least Male view it 3 more times – on repeated viewings, do you encounter different but Female NEGATIVE messages? Kurtosis .312 .717 Mean 2.4828 .18316 Skewness .291 .434 Kurtosis -.906 .845 Mean 3.2857 .13324 Skewness -.843 .365 Kurtosis -.442 .717 A two-tailed correlation was performed for the ad-evoked feelings, as shown in Table 4, along with the exploratory factor analysis. Factor Analysis is presented as Table 5. Table 4: Correlation Analysis for Ad-Evoked Feelings •Worried Pearson ____carefree Correlation Worried Insulted______honor Indifferent___ Irritated______pl Depressed__ Regretful__ _carefree ed 1 .806 Sig. (2-tailed) N 71 •Nervous Pearson .898 _____calm Correlation ** .742 ** eased .812 ** ___cheerful __rejoicing .766 ** .849 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 71 71 71 71 71 .768 ** .737 ** .748 ** .738 ** .783 Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 71 71 71 Contemplative Pearson ___impulsive ** _interested .762 ** .765 ** .747 ** .753 ** .754 ** .655 ** ** Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 71 71 71 Critical Pearson .768 ___accepting Correlation ** .742 ** .689 ** .739 ** .687 ** .660 Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 71 71 71 Cautious____ Pearson adventurous ** .836 ** .780 ** .727 ** .745 ** .740 ** .803 Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 71 71 71 ** ** Dubious_____ Pearson confident ** .780 ** .800 ** .791 ** .797 ** .759 ** Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 N 71 •Pessimistic__ Pearson _hopeful .831 .806 .000 .000 71 ** .869 .000 71 ** .848 .000 71 ** .885 .000 71 ** .906 71 ** .768 ** Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 71 71 71 Callous____ Pearson .728 affectionate Correlation ** .784 ** .708 ** .794 ** .833 ** .670 Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 71 71 71 Bad_______ Pearson .796 good Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 N 71 Sad___happy Pearson .862 ** .914 ** .000 .844 ** .000 71 ** .685 .673 ** .000 71 ** .883 .807 ** .000 71 ** .876 .821 ** .000 71 ** .730 ** 71 ** .822 ** Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 71 71 71 1 .772 Insulted_____ Pearson honored Sig. (2-tailed) .000 N 71 ** .923 ** .876 ** .774 ** .742 71 ** .772 ** .000 .000 .000 .000 71 71 71 71 1 .815 ** .843 ** .777 ** Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 N 71 Irritated_____ Pearson pleased ** Correlation Indifferent___ Pearson _interested .806 .812 .000 71 ** .923 71 ** .815 ** .000 .000 71 71 1 .904 .000 71 ** .803 ** Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 Unemotional_ Pearson __sentimental Correlation .749 ** .743 ** .833 71 ** .772 ** .000 .000 71 71 .826 ** .837 ** Sig. (2-tailed) .000 N 71 Depressed__c Pearson heerful .000 71 ** .876 .000 71 ** .843 71 ** .904 ** .000 .000 71 71 1 .833 Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 71 Regretful____r Pearson ejoicing .766 .000 .849 ** .774 ** .777 ** .803 .000 71 ** .833 71 ** 1 Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 N 71 71 71 71 71 Table 5: Rotated Component Matrix Component 1 2 3 4 5 •Worried _______carefree .361 .666 .300 .327 .408 •Nervous ________calm .273 .632 .297 .322 .499 •Contemplative____impulsive .387 .237 .296 .743 .313 •Critical ________accepting .411 .293 .214 .219 .772 •Cautious______adventurous .311 .570 .332 .633 .133 •Dubious________ confident .357 .410 .446 .576 .260 •Pessimistic_______hopeful .625 .229 .471 .370 .378 •Callous______affectionate .658 .128 .298 .431 .415 •Bad___________good .270 .377 .186 .297 .797 •Sad____________happy .610 .612 .209 .336 .180 •Insulted_________honored .693 .393 .357 .265 .266 •Indifferent______interested .315 .231 .753 .299 .365 `•Irritated_________pleased .664 .342 .478 .216 .285 •Unemotional___sentimental .306 .413 .694 .429 •Depressed______cheerful .663 .297 .565 .243 .205 •Regretful______rejoicing .371 .650 .556 .152 .188 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 12 iterations. 71 ** KMO and Bartlett's Test Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy. .905 Bartlett's Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 1825.812 df 120 Sig. .000 RESEARCH FINDINGS After conducting the research, it was found that women viewed the advertisements depicting AfricanAmericans as having a more negative cultural meaning and less positive cultural meaning than men, as shown in Table 1. After viewing the ads multiple times, women were least likely to see positive cultural meaning in the advertisement than men. However, upon further viewing women perceived more negative meaning from the advertisements. This means that stereotypes disturb women more than men. Positive meanings from an advertisement and brand may lead to brand interest and an optimistic intent to purchase. However, advertisements that represent brands that are perceived as having negative meaning will not lead to positive intentions to purchase. Also, with regard to ad-evoked feelings of regret, depression, irritation, indifference, insult and worried feelings, women were found to be more affected than men with the presence of negative stereotypes, as illustrated in Table 6 below. Table 6: Effect of Ad-Evoked Feelings on Gender Gender Regretful______rejoicing Female Male Statistic Mean 4.1724 .29806 Skewness -.078 .434 Kurtosis -.670 .845 3.4762 .29165 .289 .365 -.761 .717 4.1724 .30621 Mean Skewness Kurtosis •Depressed____cheerful Female Std. Error Mean Skewness Kurtosis Male Mean Skewness Kurtosis •Irritated_______pleased Female Mean Skewness Kurtosis Male Mean Skewness Kurtosis •Indifferent_____interested Female Mean Skewness Kurtosis Male Mean Skewness Kurtosis •Insulted______honored Female Mean Skewness Kurtosis Male Mean Skewness Kurtosis •Worried _____carefree Female Mean Skewness Kurtosis Male Mean Skewness Kurtosis .064 .434 -.631 .845 3.4048 .29328 .322 .365 -.765 .717 4.0690 .33635 .200 .434 -.847 .845 3.1905 .32374 .567 .365 -.909 .717 4.0000 .34024 .262 .434 -.861 .845 3.6190 .28299 .149 .365 -.882 .717 3.6897 .34421 .201 .434 -.964 .845 3.1190 .31642 .705 .365 -.727 .717 4.3103 .31429 .043 .434 -.731 .845 3.3810 .32502 .471 .365 -1.037 .717 CONCLUSIONS The purpose of this study is to examine the types of damage that may come about due to advertising stereotypes of African Americans. The proposed ‘stereotypes’ conceptual model may be accepted to explain that perceived stereotypes affect African Americans feelings about an advertisement, which leads to attitudes about the brand and intentions to purchase a product. The research findings from this study are significant with the respect to the use of positive/negative synchronic polysemy and positive/negative diachronic polysemy to determine positive intent of purchase. There are a few limitations with this research that should be discussed. First, a convenient sample of 72 students from a Historically Black College University was surveyed and the majority of the students were African American. Therefore, the sample size was not representative. Second, there were limitations with the advertisements chosen. Out of the five selected advertisements, four were portrayals of women. And full depictions of all possible African American stereotypes were not examined. These finding reflect similar results from previous research. African Americans are aware and affected by negative portrayal in advertisement and yet advertisers still use them. One reason may be that those making decisions about the advertisements are not African Americans and may not see the advertisements as stereotypical portrayals. Another indication is that African-Americans are seen as a homogenous group that only responds to broad advertisements. Implications from this study demonstrate that more realistic advertisements of African Americans in roles other stereotypical depictions are needed. For instance, more advertisements of African Americans as managers, bank tellers, teachers, and doctors, etc., are necessary to reflect more accurate portrayals of the group. Future research may focus on the reasons why women are affected by stereotypical portrayals in advertisements more than men. Additionally, more research should be conducted determine why advertisers continue to use stereotypical advertisements of African Americans. REFERENCES Bailey, A. A., (2006). A year in the life of the African-American male in advertising. Journal of Advertising. 35(1), 83-104. Bandura, A. Social Learning Theory. Oxford, England: Prentice-Hall, 1977, Print. Brewer, M. B. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitive-motivational analysis. Psychology Bulletin. 86(2), 307-324. Bristor, J. M., Lee, R. G., Hunt, M. R., (1995). Race and ideology: African American images in television advertising. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing. 14(1), 48-59. Duckitt, J. The Social Psychology of Prejudice. New York: Praeger Press. 1992. Entman, R., Rojecki, A. (2000). The Black image in the white mind: Media and race in America. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Gaertner, S. L., McLaughlin, J. P., (1983). Racial stereotypes: associations and ascriptions of positive and negative characteristics. Social Psychology. 56, 5-18 Greenwald, A. G. and Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitues, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychology Review. 102, 4-27. Henderson, J. J., Baldasty, G. J. (2003). Race, advertising, and prime-time television. The Howard Journal of Communications. 14(2), 97-112. Hirschman, E. C. (1993). Ideology in consumer research, 1980 and 1990: A marxist and feminist critique. Journal of Consumer Research. 19, 537-555. Hollerbach, K. L., (2009). The impact of market segmentation on African-American frequency, centrality and status in television advertising. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 53(4), 599614. Linville, P. W. (1982). The complexity-extremity effect and age-based stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 42(2), 193-211. Linville, P. W., Jones E. (1980). Polarized appraisal of out-group members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 38(5), 689-703. Maher, J. K., Herbst, K. C., Childs, N. M., Finn, S. (2008). Racial stereotypes in children’s television commercials. Journal of Advertising Research. 48(1), 80-93. Mastro, D. E., Stern, S. R., (2003). Representations of race in television commercials: A content analysis of prime-time advertising. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 47(4), 638-637. Messineo, M. J. (2008). Does advertising on black entertainment television portray more positive gender representations compared to broadcast networks? Sex Roles. 59(9/10), 752-764. Millard, J. E., Grant, P. R. (2006). The stereotypes of Black and White women in fashion magazine photographs: The pose of model and the impression she creates. Sex Roles. 54(9/10), 659-673. Miller, P., & Kemp, H. (2005). What’s black about it? Ithaca, NY: Paramount Market Publishing, Inc. Qualls, W. J., & Moore, D. J., (1990). Stereotyping effects on consumers’ evaluation of advertising: Impact of racial differences between actors and viewers. Psychology & Marketing. 7(2), 135-151. Selig Center (2007). The multicultural economy 2007. Atlanta, GA: University of Georgia Press. Shuey, A. M., King, A., & Griffith, B. (1953). Stereotyping Negroes and Whites: An analysis of magazine pictures. Public Opinion Quarterly. 17(Summer), 281-287. Slaughter, E. “Survey of Children’s Health and Obesity in America.” Prevention Magazine, 2003. Stevenson, T. H., & Swayne, L. E., (1999). The Portrayal of African-Americans in business-to-business direct mail: A benchmark study. Journal of Advertising. 28(3), 25-35. Taylor, C. R., & Lee, J. Y. (1995). Portrayals of African-American, Hispanic, and Asian Americans in magazine advertising. American Behavioral Scientist. 38(4), 608-621. Watson, S., Dejong, P. E., & Slack, J. L., (2009). Impact of racial attitudes on consumers’ evaluation of Black character advertisements: Does spokesperson skin color make a difference? Communication Research Reports. 26(2), 91-104. Whittler, T. E. (1991). The effects of actors’ race in commercial advertisement: Review and extension. Journal of Advertising. 20(1), 54-60. Wilder, D. A. (1981). Perceiving Persons As Group: Categorization and Intergroup Relations. In D.L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive Process in Stereotyping in Intergroup Behavior. Hillsdale, N. J. Zinkhan, G., Qualls, W. J., & Biswas, A. (1988). The racial composition of domestic advertising: A longitudinal analysis of blacks in magazines and television. Working Paper, University of Houston. The purpose of this study was to examine the types of damage that may come about due to advertising stereotypes of African-Americans.