INTRODUCTION Kinsey (1948, p. 639) said: “The world is not to be divided into sheep and goats. Not all things are black nor all things white. It is a fundamental of taxonomy that nature rarely deals with discrete categories. Only the human mind invents categories and tries to force fact into separated pigeon holes. The living world is a continuum in each and every one of its aspects. The sooner we learn this concerning human sexual behaviour the sooner we shall reach a sound understanding of the realities of sex”. While the notion of bisexuality has been used in the pre-Kinsey era (Stekel, 1922), it was Kinsey’s research which first created an interest in bisexuality (Kinsey, Pomeroy & Martin, 1948; Kinsey, Pomeroy, Martin & Gebhard, 1953). Despite Kinsey’s support of the idea of a valid bisexual identity, bisexuality has been denied to a large extent by society. Regardless of work by Kinsey and others to conceptualize sexuality as a continuum, many people view sexual identity as a dichotomous variable, with heterosexuality or homosexuality as the only choices (Herek, 2002). This dichotomization effectively expunges all points on the continuum between these two extremes, and has a profound effect on research on sexual orientation (Eliason, 1997). Research over the past years has provided us with much information on the correlates of individuals’ attitudes toward lesbians and gay men (Arndt, 2004; Evans & D’Augelli, 1996; Herek, 1995; Lance, 2002). However, there are a limited amount of studies outside the United States of America on bisexuality, and few of these that do not rely heavily on relations established for attitudes towards lesbians and gay men (Herek, 2002). However, according to Bhugra and De Silva (1998, p. 153) research into the phenomenon of bisexuality needs to be pursued, as “greater knowledge and understanding of bisexuality will also enhance the overall understanding of human sexuality, its manifestations and problems”. Furthermore, in a recent study, Mohr, Israel, and Sedlacek (2001) found the existence of attitudes toward bisexual men and women that are unique to bisexuality and not explainable or reducible to attitudes toward lesbians and gay men. According to Mead (1975, p. 6), “Changing traditional attitudes toward homosexuality is in itself a mind-expanding experience for most people. But we shall not really succeed in discarding the straightjacket of our own cultural beliefs about sexual choice if we fail to come to terms with the well-documented, normal human capacity to love members of both sexes”. Stereotypes abound in popular culture, with bisexuals widely perceived as promiscuous, who cannot have committed relationships or likely to contract sexually transmitted diseases (Eliason, 2001; Rust, 1995; Spalding & Peplau, 1997). Declaring an open, unequivocal bisexual identity in either heterosexual or gay and lesbian communities often results in experiences of discrimination, hostility and invalidation. This “double discrimination” by heterosexuals and the gay and lesbian communities is seldom recognized or acknowledged as forces of external oppression, yet this oppression is real and has many damaging effects on bisexuals (Firestein, 1996). However, the literature review in this proposal focused for the most part on research conducted in the USA, thus, caution must be used in generalising the results to the South African population. The dominance of the homosexual/heterosexual paradigm has led to a paucity of research on bisexuality. Although the articles on bisexuality in International Journals have increased, few of the articles discoverable in a search for the terms “bisexual” report findings specifically pertaining to bisexuals. In most articles based on empirical research, the authors simply acknowledged that bisexuals were included in their samples of lesbians and gay men by adding “and bisexual” to their sample descriptions and keyword lists (e.g. Holtzen, 1994; Ludwig & Brownell, 1999). The lack of published bisexual related articles in the South African Psychology Journals is an illustration of this scarcity. A review of the articles published in South African Psychological Journals revealed no articles pertaining to attitudes toward bisexual men and women. In view of the abovementioned it appears noteworthy to investigate the attitudes towards bisexuality in a South African sample. 2 PROBLEM STATEMENT General survey research on sexuality generally focuses specifically on heterosexuality or homosexuality. Much of the research data, from respondents with both homosexual and heterosexual attractions and behaviour, has typically been grouped with data from individuals with exclusively homosexual attractions and behaviour. Furthermore, most studies determine sexual orientation by asking the individual whether they are heterosexual or homosexual, and do not utilize measurements to determine sexual orientation on a continuum (Fox, 2004). Weinberg and Williams (1974) questioned the polarization of sexual orientation into heterosexuality and homosexuality, noting that by defining heterosexuality as the norm, there also has been the tendency to view persons as either heterosexual or homosexual. This poses the danger of ignoring the great range and heterogeneity of homosexuals. Although substantial and valuable research has been conducted on the attitudes that individuals have toward gay men and lesbians and on homophobia, there is very little research on the attitudes that individuals have toward bisexuals (Herek, 2002). According to Herek (2002), bisexual men and women are becoming increasingly visible, and therefore attitudes toward them will continue to evolve. Esses, Haddock and Zanna (1993, p. 138) maintain that an attitude toward a social group can be viewed as a “favourable or unfavourable overall evaluation of a social group, with an unfavourable evaluation being labelled prejudice”. As such attitudes develop, the expressions of prejudice, intolerance, and antagonism against bisexuals may increase. Knowledge of the socio-political context that bisexual people face becomes an important prerequisite to effective counselling with this population (Queen, 1996). According to Firestein (1996) many individuals who seek counselling are struggling with issues of bisexuality in their emotional and sexual lives. It becomes important to health workers to effectively and ethically serve client’s who are exploring issues of sexual orientation with a working knowledge of bisexuality to enhance their understandings of the gay, lesbian, and heterosexual experience. Cochran (2001, p. 941) purports that it would be “unconscionable for psychology as a profession to ignore the possibility of differences associated with sexual orientation in both mental health morbidity and treatment experience”. 3 AIM AND JUSTIFICATION OF STUDY WITH REFERENCE TO RELEVANT AND RECENT LITERATURE It is proposed that a study be done on attitudes toward bisexual men and women, as well as the relationship between attitudes and respondents sexual orientation. In many contemporary Western societies, particularly those influenced during the 19th and 20th centuries by European concepts of gender and sexuality, bisexuality is defined as a combination of sexual attractions to or sexual contacts with both men and women. According to Firestein (1996, p. xx) “bisexuality is the capacity to love and sexually desire both same and other-gendered individuals”. Bisexuality has existed as a concept and descriptive term in the psychological literature since the process of psychosexual development was first conceptualized. For example, bisexuality has been used as a theoretical construct to explain aspects of evolutionary theory, psychopathology, masculinity and femininity, psychosexual development, and homosexuality (Rust, 2002). The concept of bisexuality first appeared in psychoanalytic theory as a way of explaining the development of homosexuality in terms of evolutionary theory (Ellis, 1942; Freud, 1962). Psychoanalytic theorists believed that the human species evolved from a primitive hermaphroditic state to the gender-differentiated physical form of today. Freud (1962) found the theory of bisexuality helpful in accounting for homosexuality, which he saw as an indication of arrested psychosexual development. Furthermore, evidence from historical and cross-cultural research clearly indicates that bisexuality has existed throughout history in diverse cultures around the world (Fox, 1996). The 1973 American Psychiatric Association’s decision to remove homosexuality as a diagnostic category signalled a move away from the illness model and toward a more affirmative approach to homosexuality. In line with this, Klein (1978, 1993) and Wolff (1979) challenged the traditional psychiatric position that sexual relationships with both men and women are an indication of immaturity and psychopathology. According to Wolff (1979, p. 45): “We certainly are bisexual creatures, and this innate disposition is reinforced by the indelible memory of childhood attachments, which know no limitation of sex”. Klein (1978) maintained that bisexuality is as normal an outcome of the developmental process as is heterosexuality or homosexuality and that awareness and 4 expression of both heterosexual and homosexual attractions can enhance the individual’s experience of intimacy and personal fulfilment. However, the dichotomous model of sexual orientation remained and supported the belief that bisexuals were psychologically maladjusted. In fact, researchers (Bode, 1976; Galland, 1975) found that self-identified bisexuals were characterized by high self-esteem, self-confidence, autonomy, positive self-concepts independent of social norms, assertiveness and cognitive flexibility. Just as research has confirmed that homosexuality is not associated with psychopathology, research has revealed no indication of psychopathology or psychological maladjustment in bisexual men and women (Masters & Johnson, 1979; Nurius, 1983; Weinberg & Williams, 1974). Yoshino (2000) argues that the invisibility of bisexuality relative to homosexuality does not reflect the incidences of those orientations in the population. According to Yoshino (2000) each of the major sexuality studies demonstrates that the number of bisexuals is greater than or comparable to the number of homosexuals. In the Kinsey et al. studies (1948, 1953), we see a remarkable consistency of results, in which the percentage of bisexuals is one and a half times the percentage of homosexuals for both men and women. Masters and Johnson (1979) also found that the incidence of bisexuality was greater than or comparable to the incidence of homosexuality. Janus and Janus (1993) reported a one-to-one ratio of bisexuals to homosexuals for both men and women. Wellings, Wadsworth and Johnson (1994), and Laumann, Gagnon, Michael and Michaels (1994) also found bisexuals to exist in numbers greater than or comparable to the number of homosexuals. Although such demographic information cannot be directly translated to the South African context they do give some indication of the prevalence of bisexuality. The abovementioned suggests that bisexual invisibility is not a reflection of the fact that there are fewer bisexuals than homosexuals in the population, but is rather a product of social erasure. Yoshino (2000) explains the erasure of bisexuality by positing that both self-identified heterosexuals and self-identified homosexuals have overlapping interests in the erasure of bisexuality that includes - the stabilization of exclusive sexual orientation categories, the retention of sex as an important diacritical axis, and the protection of norms of monogamy. 5 According to Ochs (1996) bisexuality makes homosexual and heterosexual individuals uncomfortable. The discomfort and the behavioural measures used to protect the security of their position within the existing hierarchy in response to these threats may be called biphobia. Many people wish that bisexuals would just go away, or at least not talk about it, because the very existence of bisexuality is seen as a threat to the social order (Ochs, 1996). The conceivability of bisexuality as a combination of hetero- and homosexuality, and the simultaneous inconceivability of such a combination, underlie contemporary attitudes toward bisexuality, these attitudes reflect the paradox of their roots. Foremost among these negative attitudes is the belief that bisexuality does not exist (Ochs, 1996). Bisexual men and women are often assumed to be lesbians or gay men who are denying their true sexuality either because they are going through a transitional phase of coming out, because they are afraid to face their own and others’ homophobia, or because they are unwilling to shoulder the burden of being a member of an oppressed minority (Esterberg, 1997). Furthermore, individuals who engage in same-sex activity are often believed to be heterosexuals who are merely experimenting. Regardless of whether there are individuals for whom bisexual behaviour would appropriately be called experimental or transitional, these invalidating beliefs (Rust, 1995) are a cultural phenomenon which function to preserve the belief that bisexuality cannot exist by reconciling the fact that some people are attracted to both men and women with the construction of heterosexuality and homosexuality as the only two true forms of sexuality (Armstrong, 1995). Bisexuals have either been rendered none existing or paradoxically made visible through misconceptions and stereotypes and therefore, are all that most people know as working definitions of bisexuality. Such misconceptions are prejudices based on the most marginal and confused understanding of bisexuality. Unfortunately, systematic research to document individual’s views about bisexuals is lacking. The best available information comes from bisexual men and women (Weinberg, Williams & Pryor, 1994). Several of the contributors to collected works of writings by bisexuals discuss the isolation, confusion, fear, shame and denial that they experienced on discovering their attraction to people of the “wrong” sex (Aranow, 1991; Bryant, 1991; Ochs, 1992; Young, 1992). 6 As a result of negative attitudes, Shuster (1991, p. 267) states that bisexuals frequently experience themselves as existing in two different worlds, and not fully fitting in either – “a feeling of political and personal homelessness”. The negative attitudes towards bisexuals are internalized by bisexuals, compromising their health and psychological well-being (Ochs, 1996). Prejudices and misconceptions toward bisexuals include: firstly, bisexuals are seen as rejecting sexual monogamy. Bisexuals are viewed as promiscuous individuals who desire concurrent relationships with both men and women and who pursue several sexual relationships at the same time. Secondly, bisexuals are seen as particularly likely to contract sexually transmitted diseases through unsafe sexual practices and to spread these diseases to their heterosexual partners. Many researchers adopted the public perception of bisexuals as a bridge for the transmission of HIV to the heterosexual population. Thirdly, bisexuals are seen as likely to deceive a partner about their other romantic affairs, and unlikely to make a lasting commitment. Fourthly, bisexuals are seen as sexually talented lovers who enjoy an active sex life and who are highly knowledgeable and open-minded about sexuality and having expert knowledge about sex or as using special sexual techniques because of their experience with both men and women. Finally, because bisexuals are believed to reject the cultural ideal of monogamous and committed relationships, bisexual’s romantic relationships may be seen as generally poor in quality, lower in love, satisfaction, and intimacy, and higher in conflict than the relationships of heterosexuals (Eliason, 1997; Eliason, 2001; Eliason & Rahein, 1996; Rust, 1993; Rust, 1995; Spalding & Peplau, 1997; Weinberg et al., 1994). Research findings suggest that on average bisexual men have their first same-gender sexual experience in their middle teens, while bisexual women have their first samegender sexual experience in their early 20’s (Firestein, 1996). Furthermore, findings propose that on average bisexual women have their first sexual relationships with men in their middle teens, somewhat earlier than bisexual men who have their first sexual experiences and relationships with women. The findings from several studies indicate that on average bisexual men and women first self-identify as bisexual in their early and middle 20’s. Bisexual women and men typically first disclose their sexual orientation to another person in their early 20’s, at about the same time that they first self-identify as 7 bisexual (Fox, 1996, Weinberg, et al. 1994). Research findings also suggest that bisexual women experience their first homosexual attractions and behaviour after their first heterosexual attractions and behaviour, and it is also more common for women to adopt a bisexual identity sooner after their first homosexual attractions than bisexual men. In contrast, a greater proportion of bisexual men experience their first homosexual attraction and behaviour before or at about the same age as their heterosexual behaviour (Weinberg, et al., 1994). In view of the above, Klein (1990) states that university years are a common time when individuals begin to come out, whether as bisexual or as gay or lesbian. Theorists have attempted to model coming out as a bisexual man or woman, by adding bisexual identity to existing models of lesbian and gay identity development. There are a number of reasons bisexuals might not fit these models. Many bisexuals come out twice, once as lesbian and gay in a heterosexual community and then again as bisexual in a lesbian and gay community when they revalidate their attractions to the other sex (Schwartz & Blumstein, 1998). Bisexual men and women that come out first as lesbian or gay, and learned to rely on the lesbian/gay community for support in the face of homophobia, also face rejection from lesbians and gay men when coming out as bisexual in this community (McLean, 2001). According to Rust (1999) bisexuals are psychological and socially threatening to lesbians and gay men because they blurred the distinction between straight and lesbian/gay, thus undermining the basis for lesbian and gay identities and quasi-ethnic lesbian and gay political movements. Newly self-identified bisexuals have a more difficult time finding a supportive community of people like themselves compared to the highly organized and visible lesbian and gay community. The coming out experience is often very stressful for people, it can be doubly so for bisexuals, who often meet with resistance from both the homosexual and heterosexual worlds, as well as with their own internalized cultural values, which rarely see bisexuality as a valid option (Rust, 2001; Williams, 1999). Chickering and Reisser’s (1993) theory of seven developmental vectors represents the most current, inclusive and widely used theory of student psychosocial development. The vectors include the developmental tasks of developing competence, managing emotions, moving through autonomy toward interdependence, developing mature 8 interpersonal relations, establishing identity, developing purpose and developing integrity. A close examination of these vectors reveals theoretical assumptions that put bisexual students at considerable disadvantage in terms of achieving successful resolution of developmental tasks. According to Fassinger (1995) one might ask how an individual can possibly establish a secure identity in the face of marginalization, invisibility and societal censure. Moreover, one might expect that developing purpose – clarifying vocational, avocational and lifestyle plans and goals – is very likely to be ignored or stalled in the face of more pressing identity concerns related to safety, acceptance and belonging. Finally, one might ask how the process of developing a personally relevant set of beliefs and values by which to live – such as integrity – is influenced by invisibility and stigmatization of lifestyle. As a result of the abovementioned, many individuals who come out as bisexual continue to switch back and forth between gay/lesbian, heterosexual and bisexual identities (Rust, 1992, 1996). Therefore, bisexual identity once achieved is often not consistently maintained and cannot be appropriately understood as the static end stage of a developmental process of self-discovery. Weinberg et al., (1994) suggested a stage sequential model of bisexual identity development based on research with bisexuals. Their model begins with initial confusion, followed by finding and adopting the bisexual identity, followed by continued uncertainty due to the lack of social validation for bisexual identity. Paradigmatic shifts in scientific theory and research occur when information becomes available which challenges a discipline’s fundamental assumptions and for which the current disciplinary matrix cannot account (Kuhn, 1970). Firestein (1996, p. 266) asserts that a “paradigm shift is underway in the social sciences’ view of sexual orientation due to an increasing recognition of the bisexual experience”. That is, the traditional model of sexual orientation was often a dichotomous one that denied or obscured bisexuality, in the new paradigm, sexuality and sexual orientation will be conceptualized as multidimensional fluid and continuous in nature”. 9 In conclusion, writers have noted invisibility in public and academic spheres and the concomitant negative impact this marginalization has on the lives of bisexual women and men (Firestein, 1996; Ochs, 1996). According to D’Emilio (1992) the expansion of scholarly production is a vital piece of any strategy for equality. Information, education, and ideas are powerful levers for social change, and as we have more information, we will be increasingly empowered to act in the world. In view of the fragmentary state of knowledge in the abovementioned area, the aim of the research is to investigate attitudes toward bisexual men and women. Based on the literature review, the researcher has formulated the following hypotheses: 1. There are negative attitudes toward bisexual men and women. 2. Men and women have different attitudes toward bisexual men and women. 3. Men and women in different race groups have different attitudes toward bisexual men and women. 4. Individuals whom reported knowing a bisexual man or women have more positive attitudes toward bisexual men and women. 5. There is a relationship between participant’s sexual orientation and their attitudes toward bisexual men and women. 6. Individuals on the more extreme poles of the sexual orientation continuum will be more negative towards bisexuals. 10 METHODOLOGICAL ORIENTATION AND RESEARCH METHOD The Social Identity Theory underlies the proposed study. Tajfel and Turner (1979, p. 255) defines social identity as that part of an individual's self-concept which derives from his knowledge of his membership in a social group (or groups) together with the value and emotional significance attached to that membership. The assumption is made that, however rich and complex may be the individuals' view of themselves in relation to their surrounding world, some aspect of that view are contributed by the membership of certain social groups or categories. Some of these memberships are more salient than others; and some may vary in salience in time as a function of a variety of social situations". Social Identity Theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979; Turner, 1975) explains the incidence of bias as a two-step process that includes both cognitive and motivational factors. First, the perceiver must cognitively divide the social world into at least two distinguishable social categories that separate the self from others ("us" versus "them"). Second, the desire for positive self-esteem, derived in part from the social categories to which individuals belong, motivates people to view the in-group as superior to the out-group. Social Identity Theory therefore, utilizes the human propensity to categorize individuals into groups and then offers a logical rationale for the development and maintenance of inter-group bias. Self-administered questionnaires are a commonly used method of eliciting information on sensitive topics in surveys. Self-administered questionnaires have been widely used in studies of sexual behaviour and improve reporting of sensitive information by increasing privacy and reducing social desirability effects associated with interviewer administration (Smith, 1992; Tourangeau & Smith, 1996). In order to ascertain the attitudes towards bisexual men and women respondents will complete a questionnaire. The questionnaire will contain questions about demographic variables (age, gender, race, and contact with bisexuals); the Attitude Regarding Bisexuality Scale (Mohr & Rochlen, 1999) and The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid (Klein,1993). 11 SCALE 1 ATTITUDE REGARDING BISEXUALITY SCALE (ARBS) One hindrance to conducting empirical studies of attitudes regarding bisexuality is that until 1999, there was no valid and reliable measure of attitudes. Mohr & Rochlen (1999) designed the Attitude Regarding Bisexuality Scale (ARBS). The Attitude Regarding Bisexuality is designed to assess two dimensions of attitudes toward bisexual men and women. Tolerance, the first dimension, relates to the degree to which bisexuality is viewed as an acceptable, morally tolerable sexual orientation. Stability, the second dimension, relates to the degree to which bisexuality is viewed as a legitimate, stable sexual orientation. The Social Identity Theory that underlies the research offers a plausible explanation for the readiness with which humans are inclined to embrace negative attitudes and stereotypes of the out-group (bisexuals). The results of the current studies offers support for the reliability and validity for the three versions of the ARBS for use with both lesbian and gay student populations and heterosexual student populations. High internal consistency estimates were obtained for subscales of the three versions of the ARBS (ranging from .83 to .91) (Mohr & Rochlen, 1999). SCALE 2 THE KLEIN SEXUAL ORIENTATION GRID (KSOG) The dichotomous model of sexual orientation is an over-simplification of sexual orientation. According to Rust (2000) criticism of research practices paved the way for other authors to produce alternatives to the dichotomous model of sexuality in the hope that researcher would adopt these models and produce more theoretically informed research. The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid (Klein, 1993) is a system for describing a person’s Sexual proclivities in a way more detailed and informative than previous models. The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid was developed to measure a person’s sexual orientation as a dynamic multi-variable process. Individuals rate themselves on a seven-point scale. The seven-point continuum takes into account seven distinct variables – sexual 12 attraction, sexual behaviour, sexual fantasies, emotional preference, social preference, lifestyle and self-identification. Klein (1993) contended that sexual orientation is not fixed or permanent. Therefore, The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid recognizes not only the distinction between different dimensions of sexuality, including the fact that one’s sexual self-identity bears no necessary relationship to one’s actual sexual experience, but also the fact that one’s sexuality can change over time. Using The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid, an individual need not discredit or ignore her or his past sexual experiences if they differ from her on his current experience, but can describe the variety in his or her sexual history (Rust, 2004). According to Klein (1993) the concept of sexual orientation as an ongoing dynamic process is necessary if we are to understand a person’s orientation in its entirety. The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid is a reliable and valid instrument that can differentiate sexual orientation by taking into account the meaningful dimension of time as well as other varied dimensions (Chung & Katayama, 1996). STATISTICAL ANALYSIS The following statistical procedures will be conducted: An exploratory factor analysis for the Attitude Regarding Bisexuality Scale and The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid (construct validity); reliability of the abovementioned scales (internal validity); the correlation between the Attitude Regarding Bisexuality Scale and The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid; scatter plots to investigate the relationship between the two scales will be conducted as hypothesis 6 predicts a non-linear relationship; an inter-battery factor analysis or canonical correlation for the Attitude Regarding Bisexuality Scale and The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid, as well as the relationship between the subscales of the Klein Sexual Orientation Grid (Bless & Kathuria, 2000). SAMPLE The sample (N = 1 000) will consist of postgraduate students in different faculties at a university in South Africa. 13 PROPOSED STRUCTURE OF STUDY Chapter 1 Introduction Chapter 2 A history of bisexuality and cross-cultural studies Chapter 3 Bisexuality in men and women Chapter 4 Coming out, sexual identity and bisexuality. Chapter 5 Stigma and sexual orientation Chapter 6 Research method Chapter 7 Results Chapter 8 Discussion Chapter 9 Recommendations, limitations and conclusion 14 BIBLIOGRAPHY Armstrong, E. (1995). Traitors to the cause? Understanding the lesbian/gay/bisexual debate. In T. Tucker (Ed.) Bisexual politics: Theories, queries, and visions (pp. 199217). New York: Harrington Park Press. Aranow, R. (1991). Potential lovers. In L. Hutchins and L. Ka’ahumanu (Eds.) Bi any other name: Bisexual people speak out (pp. 233-239). Boston: Alyson. Arndt, M. (2004). Attitudes toward Lesbians and Gay men: A university survey. Unpublished dissertation. University of Johannesburg. Bhugra, D. & De Silva, P. (1998). Dimensions of bisexualilty: An exploratory study using focus groups of male and female bisexuals. Sexual and Marital Therapy, 13, 145157. Bless C. & Kathuria R. (2000). Fundamentals of Social Statistics: An African perspective. Johannesburg: Juta and Co, Ltd. Bode, J. (1976). View from another closet: Exploring bisexuality in women. New York: Hawthorne. Bryant, W. (1991). Love, friendship, and sex. Potential lovers. In L. Hutchins and L. Ka’ahumanu (Eds.) Bi any other name: Bisexual people speak out (pp. 110-143). Boston: Alyson. Chickering, A.N. & Reissner, L. (1993). Education and identity. San Francisco: JosseyBass. Cochran, S.D. (2001). Emerging issues in research on lesbians and gay men’s mental health: Does sexual orientation really matters? American Psychologist, 56, 931-947. Chung, Y. & Katayama, M. (1996). Assessment of Sexual Orientation in Lesbian/Gay/Bisexual Studies. Journal of Homosexuality, 30, 49-62. 15 D’Emilio, J. (1992). Making trouble: Essays on gay history, politics and the university. New York: Routledge. Eliason, M.J. (1997). The prevalence and nature of biphobia in heterosexual undergraduate students. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 26, 317-326. Eliason, M.J. (2001). Bi-negativity: The stigma facing bisexual men. Journal of Bisexuality, 1, 137-154. Eliason, M.J. & Rahein, S. (1996). Categorical measurement of attitudes about lesbian, gay and bisexual people. Journal of Gay and Lesbian Social Services, 4, 51-56. Ellis, H. (1942). Studies in the psychology of sex (Vol. 1). New York: Random House. (Original work published 1905). Esterberg, K.G. (1997). Lesbian and bisexual identities: Constructing communities, constructing selves. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Evans, N.J. & D’Augelli, A.R. (1996). Lesbians, gay men, and bisexual people in college. In R.C. Savin-Williams & K.M. Cohen (Eds.) The lives of lesbians, gays and bisexuals: Children to adults (pp. 201-226). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. Fassinger, R.E. (1995). From invisibility to integration: Lesbian identity in the workplace. Career Development Quarterly, 44, 148-167. Firestein, B. (Ed.) (1996). Bisexuality: The psychology and politics of an invisible minority. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Fox, R.C. (1996). Bisexuality in perspective: A review of theory and research. In B.A. Firestein (Ed.) Bisexuality: The psychological and politics of an invisible minority (pp. 350). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Fox, R.C. (2004). Current Research on Bisexuality. New York: Harrington Park Press. 16 Freud, S. (1962). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. New York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1905). Galland, V.R. (1975). Bisexual women (Dotoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, San Francisco. Dissertation Abstracts International, 36 (6), 3037B. Herek, G.M. (1995). Stigma and sexual orientation: Understanding prejudice against lesbians, gay men, and bisexual. Psychological Perspectives on Lesbian and Gay issues. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Herek, G.M. (2002). Heterosexuals’ attitudes toward bisexual men and women in the United States. The Journal of Sex Research, 39, 264-275. Holtzen, D.W. (1994). Handedness and sexual orientation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 702-712. Janus, S.S. & Janus, C.L. (1993). The Janus report on sexual behaviour. New York: John Wiley. Kinsey, A.C., Pomeroy, W.B. & Martin, C.E. (1948). Sexual behaviour in the human male. Philadelphia: WB Saunders. Kinsey, A.C., Pomeroy, W.B., Martin, C.E. & Gebhard, P.H. (1953). Sexual behaviour in the human female. Philadelphia: WB Saunders. Klein, F. (1978). The bisexual option: A concept of one-hundred percent intimacy. New York: Arbor House. Klein, F. (1990). The need to view sexual orientation as a multi-variable dynamic process: A theoretical perspective. In D.P. McWhirter, S.A. Sanders and J.M. Reinisch (Eds.) Homosexuality/Heterosexuality: Concepts of sexual orientation (pp. 277-282). New York: Oxford University Press. 17 Klein, F. (1993). The bisexual option. New York: Harrington Park Press. Kuhn, T.S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lance, L.M. (2002). Heterosexism and homophobia among college students. College Student Journal, 36, 410-415. Laumann, E.O., Gagnon, J.H., Michael, R.T. & Michaels, S. (1994). The social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United State: Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Ludwig, M.R. & Brownell, K.D. (1999). Lesbians, bisexual women, and body image: An investigation of gender roles and social group affiliation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 89-97. Masters, W.H . & Johnson, V.E. (1979). Homosexuality in perspective. Boston: Little Brown. Mead, M. (1975, January). Bisexuality: What’s it all about? Redbook, pp. 6-7. McLean, K. (2001). Living life in the double closet: Bisexual youth speak out. Hecate, 27, 109-118. Mohr, J,J. & Rochlen, A.B. (1999). Measuring attitudes regarding bisexuality in lesbian, gay male and heterosexual populations. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 46, 353369. Mohr, J.J., Israel, T. & Sedlacek, W.E. (2001). Counselors’ attitudes regarding bisexuality as predictors of counselors’ clinical responses: An analogue study of a female bisexual client. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 48, 212-222. Nurius, P.S. (1983). Mental health implications of sexual orientation. Journal of Sex Research, 19, 119-136. 18 Ochs, R. (1992). Bisexuality, feminism, men and me. In E.R. Weise (Ed.) Closer to home: Bisexuality and feminism (pp. 127-132). Seattle, WA: Seal. Ochs, R. (1996). Biphobia: It goes more than two ways. In B.A. Firestein (Ed.) Bisexuality: The psychology and politics of an invisible minority (pp. 217-239). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Queen, C. (1996). Bisexuality, sexual identity, and the sex-positive perspective. In B.A. Firestein (Ed.) Bisexuality: The psychological and politics of an invisible minority (pp. 103-126). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rust, P.C. (1992). The politics of sexual identity: Sexual attraction and behaviour among lesbian and bisexual women. Social Problems, 39, 366-386. Rust, P.C. (1993). “Coming out” in the age of social constructionism: Sexual identity formation among lesbian and bisexual women. Gender and Society, 7, 50-77. Rust, P.C. (1995). Bisexuality and the challenge to lesbian politics: Sex, loyalty and revolution. New York: New York University Press. Rust, P.C. (1996). Managing multiple identities: Diversity among bisexual women and men. In B.A. Firestein (Ed.) Bisexuality: The psychological and politics of an invisible minority (pp. 53-83). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rust, P.C. (1999, June). Lesbian and bisexuality: Cultural categories and the distortion of human sexual experience. Paper presented at the 25th International Academy of Sex Research Annual Meeting, Stony Brook, New York. Rust, P.C. (2001). Two many and not enough: The meaning of bisexual identities. Journal of Bisexuality, 1, 31-68. Rust, P.C. (2000). Bisexuality: The state of the union. Research, 13, 180-241. 19 Annual Review of Sex Rust, P.C. (2004). Bisexuality in the United States: A social science reader. New York: Columbia University Press. Schwartz, P. & Blumstein, P. (1998). The acquisition of sexual identity: Bisexuality. In E.J. Haeberle and R. Gindorf (Eds.) Bisexualities: The ideology and practice of sexual contact with both men and women (pp. 182-212). New York. Continuum. Shuster, R. (1991). Beyond defense: Considering next steps for bisexual liberation. In L. Hutchins and L. Ka’ahumanu (Eds.) Bi any other name: Bisexual people speak out. Boston: Alyson. Smith, T.W. (1992). A methodological analysis of the sexual behaviour questions on the General Social Survey. Journal of Official Statistics, 8, 309-325. Spalding, L.R. & Peplau, L.A. (1997). The unfaithful lover: Heterosexuals’ perceptions of bisexuals and their relationships. Psychology of Woman Quarterly, 21, 611-625. Stekel, W. (1922). Bi-sexual Love. New York: Emerson Books. Tajfel, H. & Turner J. C. (1979). An integrative identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S.Worchel and W.G. Austin (Eds.) The Pscyology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Tourangeau, R. & Smith, T.W. (1996). Asking sensitive questions: The impact of data collection mode, question format and question context. Public Opinion Quarterly, 60, 275-304. Turner, J.C. (1975). Social Comparison and social identity: Some prospects for intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5, 5-34. Wellings, K., Wadsworth, J. & Johson, A. (1994). Sexual diversity and homosexual behaviour. In A.M. Johnson, J. Wadsworth, K. Wellings and J. Field (Eds.) Sexual attitudes and lifestyles (pp. 183-224). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific. 20 Weinberg, M.S. & Williams, C.J. (1974). Male homosexuals: The problems and adaptations. New York: Oxford University Press. Weinberg, M.S., Williams, C.J. & Pryor, D.W. (1994). Dual attraction: Understanding bisexuality. New York: Oxford University Press. Williams, M.J.K. (1999). Sexual pathways: Adapting to dual sexual attraction. Westport, CT: Praeger. Wolff, D.G. (1979). The lesbian community. Berkeley: University of California Press. Yoshino, K. (2000). The epistemic contract of bisexual erasure. Stanford Law Review, 52, 353-459. Young, S. (1992). Breaking the silence about the “B-word”: lesbian-feminist discourse. Bisexual identity and In E.R. Weise (Ed.) Closer to home: Bisexuality and feminism (pp. 75-87). Seattle, WA: Seal. ……………………… …………………………….. Supervisor Departmental Chairperson 21 ……………….. Date