The dynamics of mass interaction

advertisement
-
--
.
...-
.—
-
....,
..—
The dynamics of mass interaction
Steve l~ttaker,
Loren Terveen, W~ Hti, Lynn Cherny
An Labs-Research
180 ParkAve
FlorhamPark NJ 07932 USA
+1 9733608339
{stevew,terveeW_,cherny}
@esearch.att.com
over 17,000 newsgroups, with approximately 3 tion
users worldwide [7]. ~s
groti
has been achieved
without any cenmfied organisation or governing body [9].
me ubiquity of Usenet and the fact that it suppoti
conversations between hundreds or even thousands of
pticipants, provides the opportunity to study what we
term mass interaction. However, we currentiy lack basic
data about Usenet interactions. me current paper analyses
over 2.15 Won
messages produced by 659,450 people in
500 representative newsgroups co fleeted over 6 months.
We
provide
descriptive
dab
about newsgroup
demographics, communication strategies and interactivity.
We hen derive predictions from tie common pound
model of communication to test predictions about how
these parametersinteract
ABSTRACT
Usenet may be regarded as the worl$s
largest
conversatioti appficatioq with over 17,000 newsgronps
and 3 fion
users. Despite its ubiquity and poptiari~,
however, we know Etie about the natore of the interactions
it suppo~.
~s
empirical paper investigates mass
interaction in UseneL We anrdyse over 2.15 Mon
messages from 659,450 posters, co~ected from 500
newsgoups over 6 months. We fit characterise mass
interactio~ presenting basic data about demographics,
conversational strategies and interactivity- Using
predictions tiom the common ground [3] model of
interactio~ we next conduct causal modetig to determine
relations bemeen demographics, conversational strategim
and interactiv~.
We find evidence for moderate
conversatiomd
threading, but
hge
participation
ine@ties
in Usene4 with a d
minority of participants
posting a large proportion of messages. Contrary to the
common gromd model and ‘Etiquette” guidefies [8,10]
we ho find that “cross-posting” to extemd newsgroups is
hig~y fiequenk Our predictions about the eEects of
demographics on conversatioti
strategy were largely
cotie~
but we found disconfirming evidence about the
relations befi’een conversational strategy and interactivity.
Contrary to our expectations, boti cross-posting and short
messages promote interactivity. We conclude that in order
to e~lain mass interaction, the common ground model
must be mowed to ticorporate notions of weak ties [5,q
and communication overload [11,18].
Previous research on Usenet has tended to carry out sma~
sde
qtitative
studies of specific newsgroups, their
ctitie and tieir conversation [1,17]. me
these studies
have drawn attention to impotit
phenomena, their
specfic focus means tiey cannot address general questions
tit are central to mass interaction such as the levels of
communication betieen ~erent newsgroups. ~ey also
cannot easly examine the effects on interaction of different
demographic variables or communication strategies, in
order to test specXc communication models.
me current study attemptsto redress the balance. We f~st
present basic information from the 500 newsgroups about
mass interaction addressing the fo~owing questions. What
are tie dernogaphics of a typical newsgroup: how many,.
people contribute and how often do they do so? Is
participation roufly equal or are groups dominated by a
few verbose individurds?A second set of questions concern
conversational strate~-es. Do participants restrict their
interactions to the current newsgroup or do they broadcast
them widely to mtitiple groups (a phenomenon known as
cross-posting)? How long is a typical message? Do most
newsgroups have FAQs ~Cfists of tiequenfly asked
questions”) and how often do they post them? Finafly we
can ask questions about interactivi~ how deep is a typical
conversational threa~ and how often are attempts to
initiateconversation successti?
Ke~ords
hlass interactio~ Usene< conversatio~ newsgroups,
common groun~ moderatio~ FAQS, netiquette, empirid.
lNTRODUC~ON
Usenet maybe regarded as the worl&s largest and fmtest
growing conversationrd appficatiom k 1988 there were
fewer than 500 news~oups. bent
estimatesvary, but at
the time of our dab co~ection in Dec. 1996, there were
Ptission 10de distal or hard copies ofall or pti ofthis ~vorkfor
personal or classroom use is granted without f= protided that copies
Zre notmade or dis~%uted for profit or commercial advantage and that
copies bear this notice and the fill citation on the fimt paga To copy
othm~ise to republish, to post on sen’em or to redistribute to Iis&
Tquires prior sptific permission and~ora fee
CSCW 98 Seatie Wastinson USA
‘Cop3ti@tACLI 199s 1-581i34094/98/11-..Soooo
I
--—
37
-.
*.
.
,.
,,
——
—.—
.
.
. _..
We *O
tested how weu the common pound
communication theory [3] expw
mass interaction The
sc~e of mass interactio~ and the huge numbers of
messages and participants, gives rise to WO novel
communication problems. The &t of these concern how
participants estabkh common ground in mass interaction
Co=on
ground is a key principle of fice-to-face
conversation and refers to tie fact that participants must
estabhh a degree of muti
knowledge for their
conversational contributions to be understood [3]. Face-b
face conversations genetiy take place between dyads or
smti groups, however. So how can conunon ground be
estabtihed when there is a huge set of conversationrd
participants with potenMy
diverse perspectives? There
are rdso issues concerning the stabtity of the newsgroup
pop&tioL h some newsgroups, there is a core set of
participantswho repeatedy converse, flowing participants
to become ftiar
with one another. This ftiarity
supports conunon groun~ but how do conversation fae in
newsgroups where the participants are constantly
changing? Some newsgroups employ a strategy of
moderation to addressthe problem of common ground h a
moderated group, W interactions are fltered by a d
set
of moderators who are knowledgeable about the gords and
history of the newsgroup. My messages that are relevant
to the newsgroup’s goti are dewed to appear. Our fit
set of hypotheses concern how the three demographic
tictors of newsgroup ske, the familian-~ of participants,
and the effects of moderation affect common ground We
*O investigate how these same factors &ect interactivip.
One measure of conversationrd interactivity is the extent to
which a given conversationrd contribution depends on prior
context [3,14]. By this defitio~
deeply threaded
conversations are indicators of interactivity. We therefore
looked at how demographic fictors tiect threading.
The second major connnunication problem arises directiy
horn the ~cd~
of estabtihing common ground bck of
common ground may mean that participants are inundated
with postings that are redundant or irrelevant For example,
new participants who are ignorant of a newsgroup’s go~
or conversation history, may post questions that have
been discussed before, or are largely fielevant to the
group’s interests.We therefore investigated a second set of
factors concerning people’s conversational strategim in
addressing redundancy and irrelevance. We tested the
effects of three conversatioti smtegies on interactivity:
(a) FAQ production; @) long messages; (c) low levek of
cross-posting. ~ese strategies are ~ derived from the
notion of common ground FAQs exernp~
common
ground by ~ing
prior discussions and providing
tiormation about newsgroup ctitiq cross-posting can be
seen as a f~ure
of common ground representing
participants’ need to go outside the newsgroup for
conversation long messages can be taken as evidence for
substantive discussions and hence estab~ed
common
.
a.-
..-
‘~>1-‘-.
ground. The common ground model would therefore
predict that FAQ productio~ decreased cross-posting and
greater message length shodd au increase titeractivity. A
second set of partia~y overlapping predictions about the
effects of conversational strategy on interactivity, can be
derived
from
the
Netiquette
guidekes
(news.announce.newusers and news.answers). These area
set
of
prescriptive
guideties
about
effective
communication strategies [S,10]. They tell users to be
succinct ~avoid long postings”), avoid redundantor repeat
postings ~read the FAQ’), and to avoid widespread
posting of messages of ody marginal relevance ~don’t
Cross-posfl). With the exception of succinctness, tie
Netiquette guidekes and common ground model make
identicrd predictions about the effects of conversational
strategieson interactivity.
To summarise, the paper first presents basic descriptive
statistics for newsgroups: (a) demographics - stie,
ffiarity
and moderatio~ @) conversation strategies FAQ productio~ message len~
and cross-posting; (c)
interactivity - the extent of conversational threading. We
then use predictions derived from the common ground
model to test three further questions: (a) how the three
demographic variables affect conversational strategy; @)
how demographic variables affects interactivity; (c) how
conversational strategy affects interactivity. We conclude
with a general characterisation of mass interaction and a
discussion of how we~ the common ground model apphes
to mass interaction.
USENET
ORGANISATION,
INTERACTION
DISTRIBUTION
AND
There are several Ievek of s~cture in Usenet. CoUections
of messages are clustered into newsgroups, and
newsgroups themelves are organised into hierarchies.
Each hierarchy is intended to address different
conversational topics. There are over a hundred different
hierarchiesbut the majority of newsgroups belong to one of
eight main hierarchies ~’the big eight”). These eight are: alt
for alternative topics; comp for computer issues;
humanitia; mkc for misce~aneous discussions; nws for
discussions about Usene~ rec for recreational topics; soc
for social issues; and talk for general conversations. Each
hierarchy is then subdivided into more specfic
subhierarchies. The name of each newsgroup begins with
the relevant main hierarchy, and tem
of increasing
specificity are added to this. Typical newsgroup names are
rec.music.dylan and rec.music.beatles, where both
newsgroups are part of the music subhierarchy within rec.
Twelve percent of newsgroups are moderated, and
moderation is reflected in the newsgroup name:
soc.russian.ctiture.moderated. Newsgroup distribution also
varies from site to site. Newsgroups are distributedthrough
an infod
network of servers, with the specific set of
.—
—.
—___
..——-
.
.
groups. These messages contain computer programs or
images and we were ody interested in textual messages,
since we are studying conversation. For the same reaso~
we excluded n~vs, *bk*, and ~obs * groups, because these
groups are places for generrd announcements and
advertisements, not conversation. We excluded regional
groups, as our focus was on groups with global
distriiutiom
Fiagure1: Interaction md threading in r~.mwic.d>~Ian
A third level of structurewithin newsgroups is achieved by
a mechanism that trach conversationrdthreads (see Figure
1). A conversation begins with an initiating post about a
nem7 issue or question ~Re Robert Johnson and Don
Harnmon~, posted by HW61) and any messages
responding to the new posting ti be Med to the message
to which they respon& This is a conversationrdthrea~ i.e.
a conversation consists of an tiitiating post along with
various responses (if the posting eficits responses). There
are &o mtitiple Ievek of threading, so responding to an
initialpost is different from responding to a response to the
initial posL For e-le,
Figure 1 shows that the fired
message in the ‘Robert Johnson” thread (from ~
routier) was acwy
a response to tie second posting in
fiat thread(Horn Sadiejane), as revealed by the be joining
them hfost newsreading software organises messages
according to threads, so users can view conversations in
terms of the initiating posL They can rdso view the tiead
depth of a given message, ie. whether it is an initiating
post (depth of zero) a response an initiating post (depth of
1), and so OE FMy,
cross-posting a message to mtitiple
newsgroups is done by entering the names of additionrd
newsgroups in the nen~sgroups field wMe composing a
message.
DATA COLLEC~ON AND SMPLING
We sampled from 26 Merent to~level categories,
inclutig seven of the ‘%ig eight” categories. We excluded
17ariousnewsgroups for Merent reasons. We first excluded
altgroups. hlmy server administratorshave a poficy of not
distributing alt groups because of the tone and subject
matter of many alt discussions. This *es
alt groups less
ubiquitous in terms of their distriiutiom Alt groups &o
~er horn N other soups in their approval and creation
process. T$7e*O excluded hmzanitim groups because there
are ordy a few groups fiess b
10 worldwide thatwe have
been able to find). We did not coflect data for any binaries
—
.,.—
—
This left us considering groups from the categories comp,
mist, ret, sci, SOC, and talk. From our newsfee~ we
selected a stratied random sample of 500 newsgroups
from these groups in proportion to their global occurrence,
where the proportions were derived from a master list of
17,112 currendy active newsgroups dodoaded
from
uunet (ftp.uu.net). We selected ody “active” groups, which
we defined as those groups for which tiere were at least
1S0 messages over 6 months, equivalent to one message
per day. A til
selection criterion was moderation the
groups selected were representative of the overa~ level of
moderation in UseneL
We coflected header information about each message in
each of those newsgroups over a six month perio~ from
Jtiy to December 1996. For each message we extracted
various types of tiormation including: the ed
addressof
the message posteq the date that the message was poste~
the subject he of the message; message length (number of
hes in the body of the message); and thread depth. h
cases where messages were cross-posted we also gathered
information about the newsgroups that the message was
posted to.
We first present descriptive data about newsgroup
demographics, conversation strate~”m and interactive@.
We then outie spec%c hypotheses about the relations
beween these factors, and present a causal model testing
these hypotheses.
DESCRIPTIVE
RESULTS
Demogmpti=
Table 1 shows demographic statistics for the 500
newsgroups. For each newsgroup, we calcdated the total
number of messages that people posted over the 6 month
interv~ and the number of different people who posted to
that newsgroup. Overti levek of message traffic were
hi~ with each group on average receiving 4299 messages
(an average of 24 messages per newsgroup per day). h
additio~ each newsgroup attracted contributions from an
average of 1319 Merent posters. Together these statistics
provide evidence for mass interaction.
To calcdate thefamiliarip of posters in a newsgroup, we
used &e criterion of repeat posting, i.e. whether a person
posts more than once to a newsgroup. Our datashow thatin
gened a si@cant
proportion of users are titic
27% of messages are from “singleton posters” who ody
contribute to a newsgroup once. This raised the issue of
——-
●
.—..
—
-.—. —
whe&er newsgroups have participation ineqtity.
Some
participants must be contributing large numbers of
messages, given (a) the high proportion of singIeton
poster$ and @) that the mean level of contiiution is 3.1
messages/poster. Figure 2 shows hi~y uneqti Ievek of
pardcipation in each newsgroup. The right hand side of the
~=ph shows that the majority of people post ody a few
times, wMe the left hand side revefi that there are a few
people who post a large number of times. A fid statistic
bearing this out is tiat a tiny percentage (2.9%) of posters
ti each newsgroup account for an average 25~0 of the toti
posts.
DATA COLLE=D
Number pardcipants
I Demographics
I
I
0/0messages horn repeatposters
Number messages
I
hlessage length
I Conversatioti
‘/0
messages tit were cross-
hlssage byPosters (rink@
●
a
1319
73
Figure 2: Levels
newsgroup
4299
44
Thread depti
individual participation
within
Interactivity
34
Fina~y we looked at interactivi~. The average thread depth
is 1.S messages, suggesting atypical exchange in which the
average message refers to approximately two other
messages. A substantialproportion of messages (33Yo) had
two or more threads, indicating frequent extended
conversations. We ako calcdated a complementary
measure of interactivity, the number of initiating posts
(those with zero references and thread depth of one). A
high proportion of initiating messages would indicate
repeated fadures to start conversations, or a prevalence of
conversational dead-ends. We found that initiating
messages are hi~y prevalen$ accounting for over more
than 40% of messages. This suggests a view of Usenet in
which it is hard to starta conversation. Once a conversation
starts, however, then it seems to atiact mtitiple
contributions.
+ 0.4
bteractivity
of
1
1.s
Table 1: D~criptive statisticsfor the 500 newsgoups over
the 6 nzo?zthsof the stud~~
Conversational Shtegies
lt7e found cross-posting to be prevalent On average 34%
messages b each newsgroup were addressed to at least one
other group, and the average cross-posted message
targetied 3.1 ofier newsgroups. Cross-posting was not
specific eitiex newsgroups were hig~y inconsistent in tie
set of extemrd groups they cross-posted to. Each
newsgroup oved
cross-posted to a mean of 272 distinct
groups. To tier
tivestigate the precision of cross-posts,
we cdctiated the mean number of cross-posts per distinct
cross-posted group. Strong ~
betieen groups wodd
lead to a large number of cross-postings to a sd
number
of groups and hence a high meu However the mean was
5.4. me absence of specfic cross-posting argues against
the view tiere are strong communication ties between
specific newsgroups.
TESTING THE COMMON GROUND MODEL
We next use the common ground model to derive a spec~c
set of predictions. Each of the conversational strategy
variables of (a) increased FAQ production, @) decreased
cross-posting; (c) longer messages can be seen as an index
of common ground. From our eartier arguments we would
therefore expect demographics to affect common ground
and hence conversational strategim in the following ways:
FAQs were detected autornaticdy by an algorithm which
searched subject ~es for tie words “FAQ” or ‘Trequendy
Asked Questions”. It excluded certain other special cases
such as subject ties including the word “re”. FAQs are a
common featnre of newsgroups which is shown by the fact
that 54% of newsgroups had FAQs, although less than
0.5?4 of messages were FAQs. We rdso cdctiated average
nzessagelength, wtich was 44 hes.
Hl~b,c Newsgroups that are larger in size %ti have more
Bcdty
establishing common ground, we would therefore
exTect them to have (a) fewer FAQs; @) more crosspostin~ and (c) shortermessages;
H2~b,~ Newsgroups containing many repeat posters (i.e.
people who arefamiIiar with the newsgroup) will estabfish
common ground more easily, they wi~ therefore have (a)
more FAQs; @) less cross-posting; and (c) longer
messages;
~60
———
-——. ——
——
Afoderation
-——
—
-—
.->::
_-
FAQs
0.16
F
1
i?
O*M
Figure 3: Causal model sho~vingeffuts of vanabIa in structural quation analysis,for demographic,
conversational strate~ and interactivip van-abla
H9: Moderated newsgroups *
have greater interactivity
as dested
by increased thread depti,
H3~b,c
J[oderated newsgroups shodd more easfiy
estabfish common grom~ and hence have (a) more FAQs;
@) less cross-posting and (c) longer messages;
We tested these predictions using tie fo~owing causal
mode~ which was tested using a series of regression
tiyses
[4]:
Tt7e fio exTected conversational strateg variables to
newsgroups can estabkh
tiuence
interactivity. E
common
ground tiough
effective communication
strategies, we shotid expect this to be Mested
in terms
of more interactive conversation as indicated by greater
tiead depti
Demographics + Conversatioti strategies+ kteractivity
I
1
me oved
anrdysis involved two steps. k the fist we
regressed each of the demographic variables onto the
conversational strate~ variables to test H1-H3, and second
we regressed W variables onto interactivi~ to test H4-H9.
Where relevant variables were normafised to allow for the
fact that there were different numbers of messages and
participants contributing to each news~oup. For several
variables (e.g. cross-posting) we experimented with
Merent operationatiations (e.g. mean number of crosspostimessage, mean overa~ number of groups crossposted to), choosing the operationrdisation that best
accounted for the variance in the regression equations.
H* NTewsgroups with more FAQs ~
have greater
interactivity as Mested
by increased threaddep~
=
Newsgroups with less cross-posting ti have greater
interactivity as -ested
by increased thread depti,
H& Newsgroups with longer messages ti have greater
titeractivi~ as manifested by increased thread dep~
We shotid dso exTect demographic factors to have direct
&ects on interactivity:
NTewsgroupsthat are larger in stie ti
have less
~
interactivity as manifested by reduced thread dep@
:
Effects of demographics on conversational strategy
Figure 3 shows the results of three regressions of
HS: NTewsgroupscontaining repeat posters (ie. people who
with the newsgroup) ~
have greater
are fmiliar
interactivity as manifested by increased tiead depti,
demographics on conversational strategy. me numbers on
the arcs represent the standardised beta weights. For
b
261
. . . ..-
-.
.
..
., ..,.
I
simpEcity of presentation we show ody
significant
relationships. Ovefi
the modek for each of tie three
factors was statisticdy significmt For message length
(~3,49q = 413, p <0.001, R2 = 020), for cross-posting
(f13,49q = 11.3, p <0.001,
R2 = O.OQ, and for FAQs
(~3,49q = 5.89, p <0.01, R2 = 0.03). We now discuss
tie spec%c predictions about the effects of each
demographic factor. We
stie had no effect on FAQs
@la), predictions concerning hypothesis Hlb and Hlc
were cofied
newsgroups that are hger in she have
initiating attempts. This suggests a problem of
conversational ineti.a: it seems to be problematic to starta
mass interaction but it is rehtively easy to continue it once
started
We &o found massive pati-c~ation inequaliti~ between
ditTerentpeople in a given newsgroup. This is an important
clarification of the view that the ktemet is an egatitian
fomm [9]. We
it is true that anyone can post to a
newsgroup, the descriptive data clearly show that
conversations in newsgroups are dominated by a minority
of hifly verbose participants.Again this argues for a view
that Usenet conversations do not stricdy involve repeated
levek of mass participatio~ as evidenced by the fact that
27% of messages are generated by people who ody
contribute once to the newsgroup. OveraU these retits
suggest that as far as active posting is conceme~ mass
interaction may be a tinomer.
Typical conversations
involve ody sma~ numbers of posters, and newsgroups are
often dominated by cfiques of verbose contiiutors. This
pardcipation inequality contrasts with research on face-toface interaction and video-mediated communication which
has shown much more equal levels of participation [14].
Participation inequality may not have whofly negative
outcomes, however. The majority of Usenet participants
may wefl be satisfied with making tiequent contributions,
posting ody about issues that are important to them. Such
peripheti participation may enable people to remain in the
backgroun~ monitoring general conversations unti they
spot a topic of direct relevance, or they need to pose a
question of tieir own. This type of background
involvement has been observed to be beneficial in the case
of corporate ed
[11] and interpersonal communication
[12]. This form of participation has also been proposed as
an important way for novices to learn about a novel topic
[12].
more cross-posting (t= 2.2, p <0.05, B= 0.10) and shorter
messages (t = 3.2, p c 0.01, P= 0.14). The f~ty
predictions H2b and H2c were rdso confirmed newsgroups
containing more famZiar contributors had less crossposting (t= 4.5, p <0.001, B= 0.02) and longer messages
(t= 2.4, p <0.05, P= 0.01), although again there was no
fiect of ftiarity
on FAQs, so H2a was not cofie~
Fin~y predictions H3<b, and c were W confirme&
Jfoderation led to more FAQs (t= 3.5, p <0.001,
O.1~, reduced cross-posting (t = 2.8, p c
P=
0.01, ~=
0.13), and longer messages (t = 9-5, p < 0.001, ~=
0.39).
Effec& of demograpties
and conversational s~tegy
on interactivity
model was ve~ successti
at predicting
The ~
titimCtiVi@ (f16,493] = 133.9, P <0.001, R2 = 0.62).
However, two of the predictions about the effects of
conversation strategy on interactivity were &confirmed
H5 predicted that newsgroups with less cross-posting@
have greater interactivity, but in fact tie less cross-posting
there w~ in the group, the Ims the threading (t = 23.3, p <
0.001, )= 0.73). H6 predicted thatnewsgroups witi longer
messages d
have greater intemctivity, but in fact
message len@ reducd interactivity (t= 52, p <0.001,
&;~S).
There were no effects of FAQs on interactivi~
J~e
the conversational and participation data suggest a
view in which conversation is carried out Iocafly by a few
participants, the cross-posting data suggest a s~ghtiy
~erent
perspective. It turns out that cross-posting is
fiequen~ although it propagates potentia~y irrelevant
messages and is thus contrary to both the common ground
model and Netiquette guidehes. Cross-posting can also
inform us about the relations between newsgroups. When
people do cross-pos< they are unspecific about the groups
they cross-post to, as indicated by the fact that the average
newsgroup cross-posted to over 200 others. This in turn
argues against the view that there are tight conversational
M
between ~erent newsgroups [15].
Ody one of the predictions about the effects of
was confirme~ namely H8,
demographi~ on interacti~
newsgoups containing f~ar
contributors ~
have
greaterbead deph (t= 16.2,p <0.001, B= 0.05). Neither
FR nor H9 concerning the effect of FAQs and stie were
vfidated
CONCLUS1ONS
We cm draw a number of
conclusions about mass
interaction from this researck Mthough there are large
numbers of people contributing to each newsgroup and
large numbers of messages ove~
the descriptive data
show that the defadt mass interaction consists of a 43 he
message referring to two previous messages. This indicates
moderate but not large amounts of interactivity. This
geneti view about interactivity shotid be tempere~
however, by the fact that many attempts to initiate
interaction were fdures; over 40°A of messages were
We *O tested a number of predictions derived from the
common ground model. These were matiy verified for the
effects of demographics on common ground. The
demographic variables of moderation fdarity
and
der
newsgroups au increased common ground as
measured by increased message length and decreased
262
:
,-
●
:
~
~
~
~
;“
i.
I
1
!
:
,
~
1
~
‘
-
>.
-,
!.-.
-—.—————-
.--.,
~
.
—
.
anrdysisof FAQs and moderators’ strategiesis necess~
determinewhether these suggestions are the case.
cross-posting. me effects on FAQ production were less
clear however.
My
moderation increased FAQ
prOductiO~ with neither ~ty
nor she having an
effect ~
view argues against the view that FAQs are a
“defence mecti~”
tiing from participants’ desire to
Our &dings shodd ako be qutied
by a number of
provisos. Our stricfly quantitative anrdysis needs to be
complemented by content tiysis,
as wefl as surveys of
Usenet participants.The cwent dysis
is silent about the
effects of conversatioti contenti which specific topics or
conversationrd styles encourage large responses and which
fd to eticit a response? ~t
factors encourage or
discourage flaming? mat are people’s reactions to flagrant
examples of cross-posting? Surveys and interviews could
*O addresspeople’s attitudesto, and satisfaction with their
Usenet interactions. My do people contribute to certain
discussions but not to others? How long do people lurk
before they tit post? And how is dominance viewed?
my do certain people post mdtiple messages and how are
they perceived by others for doing so? Again some of these
issues have been addressed in sma~ scale studies, but more
of ti me of work is needed [1,17]. There are also issues
about moderation and FAQ maintenance which are higMy
relevant for issues of group memory [2]: why do certain
people take on the responsib%ty of moderating or
maintaining FAQs, and what is their motive for doing so?
Other issues that need to be addressed by content analysis
include deh%erateattemptsto subvert Usenet conversation
or the generation of messages by
rw amrning, “trohg”)
Mcial
agents ~bots”).
insfl consensus in diverse or rapidy changing user
poptiations. Mther FAQs are most ~ely to arise under the
orderly conditions induced by moderation
Jf%at about tie effects of derno~phics on interactivity?
Here the common ground model f~ed much less wek ody
f~arity
directiy increased heading,
with neither
moderation nor ske having an effect Furthermore, and
contrary to our expectation,
interactivity.
~s
is counter
cross-posting increased
to both the common ground
view and the NTetiquetteguidehex both of these contend
that cross-posting shotid Mute conversatiomd focus and
hence reduce interactivity. This retit is consisten~
however, with a ~erent
perspective on large scale
interactio~ namely }veak ti~ [5,@. The positive effects of
aoss-posting on interactivity suggests that people exploit
the mass distribution properties ~a diverse poptition of
Usenet to go beyond a particdar newsgroup to carry out
their conversations. Combining the retits about the effects
of f~arity
and cross-posting on interactivity indicates
thatthere may be ~o complementary sets of circumstances
hat facfitate mass titeractiom me first occurs when
ftiar
participants share common ground and the second
is where people seek out diverse perspectives by posting
outside their newsgroup. Our M
prediction about the
effects of message length on interactivity was *O
&co*e&
shorter messages acdy
promoted
interactivity. How can we explain this? One possl%fity is
consistentwith communication overload [11, 18]. Given tie
huge amounts of conversation tic
in Usene~ people
are less ~ely to rea~ and hence reply to, long messages.
Such an interpretation wodd be consistent witi the
Netiquette strictore ~avoid long postings”).
FMy there are issues concerning the use of thread depth
to measure interactivity. One potentird objection to using
threading is it f~ to include ‘%ackchannel” responses in
emaik Usenet users report that they sometimes reply to a
pubfic Usenet message privately in emafl. However, our
interesthere was in pubhcly observed mass interaction, and
ed
conversations are not part of the pubfic record. This
argument *O appties to the issue of “lurking”, i.e. reading
newsgroup messages without responding to them. me
lurking may be a prevalent behaviour, again it leaves no
pubfic conversationrdtrace. FinaUy there is the question of
“flaming”. Deeply threaded Usenet interactions sometimes
retit
from emotiona~y charged and occasional
persotiy
abusive exchanges. However this addresses the
question of conversational conten~ an issue which we leave
to fntore work
Mthough moderation and FAQ production increased
common groun~ they ha no effecs on interactivity. my
was this tie case? One reason why moderation does not
increase threading may be that moderatom incrae
conversatioti relevance by deciding that a new posting is
tan~enti to tie topic, and stopping the current thread h
doing so, however, they automaticdy reduce the amount
of threading. A reason for the absence of expected
conversatiomd benefit of FAQs may be that FAQs have
two contradictory effects. FAQs may promote conversation
by providing access to information about group titure,
conversatioti
expectations ~a
a precis of group
conversatioti
history. At the same time they may
discourage newcomers from productively revisiting a
previous conversation out of the mistaken be~ef that the
topic has been efiausted New research involving content
mat are the theoreticrd implications of these restits? Our
data show thatw~e the common ground model provides a
good account of the effects of demographics on
conversational strategy, it is much weaker at predicting
interactivity. Two major mo~cations need to be made in
applying the common ground model to mass interaction.
The fact that shorter messages promoted interactivity
suggests that a model of mms interaction also needs to
incorporate the notion of conversational overload
participants have to flter large numbers of messages to
fia relevant inforrnatio~ with the consequence that long
263
—-.
to
-.
●
—-—
. .. . .
.,-.,-
..__
messages may be ignored Our finding that cross-posting
benefits interaction indicates that he model needs to take
account of tie benefits of both -arity
and diversity. On
fie one hand common ground can promote consensus but
conversations can grow stie through ovefitiarity
of
topics and people. On tie other han~ a diverse popdation
may stirndate interaction but their widely disparate
perspectives may mean tit no conversatioti progress can
be made.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank hfarc Smi@ Lyn W~er
and Jtia Hirschberg
for comments on early versions of this work
REFERENCES
1. Ban
N. titerpreting soap operas and creating
community: bide
a computer-mediated fa ctitie,
Journal ofFolHore Research, 30,143-176,1993.
2. Berw L., Jefies, R, O~ay, V., Paepcke, A., and
J$%arto$ C., were did you put it? ksues in the design
and use of a group memory. h Proceedings of CH’93,
33-30,1993.
3. Clark K Arenas of language use. University of
ticago Press, Chicago, 1992.
4. Cohe% P. Empirical methods for artiicial intelligence,
Bosto~ h~ Press, 1995.
5. Constan$ D., Spro~ L., and Kiesler, S. The kindness
of stigers, Organtiational Science, 7, 119-135, 1996.
6. Grano\’etter,hL me strength of weak ties. American
Journal ofSociolo~, 7S, 1360-13S0, 1973.
7. Harrisoq hI. me Usenet handbook O’Re~y,
1994.
8. Hortoq hL (1993): Rties
news.rnisc,news.answers.,~
Bosto~
for posting to Usene~
at timi~edw
10.Kofloc~ P and Smiti, M. Managing the Virtual
Cornmom h S. Herring (Ed.) Computer-Mediated
Communication, John Benjamins, Ptiadelphia, 1996.
‘“
1l. Krau~ R and Attewe~, P. Media use in a global
corporation. k S. Kiesler (Ed.), Culture of the Internet,
Erlba~ NJ., 1997.
12.Krauq R, Fish R, ROOLB. and ChaKonte, B. Mormal
communication in orgtiations.
k R Baecker @d.),
Groupware and Computer Supported Co-operative
Work, 2S7-314, Morgan KaCA, 1992.
13.Marti,
M. Towards a critical mass theory of
interactive media. h J. Fuk and C. Stetileld (Eds.),
Organtiations and Communication Technolog, Sage,
CA, 1990.
14. O,Con@
B., Whittaker, S., and WSbur, S.
Conversations over video
conferences,
Human
&mputerInteraction, S, 3S942S, 1994.
15.Smi@ M. Measuring the social s~cture of the Usene~
Unpubbhed paper, University of CaMornia, Los
Angeles, 1997.
16.Sprod, L., and Kiesler, S. Connections, Boston, MT
Press, 1991.
,‘
17.Suttoq L. Gender, power, and sflencing in electronic
discourse on Useneg Proceedings of the 20th Berkel~
Linguistics Socie@, University of California, Berkeley,
1994.
lS.~ttaker,
S. and Sidner, C. Ed
overload exploring
personal information management of emafl. h
Proceedings of CH96, ACM Press, ~,
1996.
19.Whittaker, S. T*g
to strangers: an evaluation of the
electronic
collaboration,
h
factors
tiecting
Proceedings of CSCW96, ACM Press, ~,
1996.
I
I
9. King, J. Grinter, R and Pickefig, J. The rise and fM of
NehNe. h S. Kiesler @&), Culture of the intemet.
Erlba~ NJ, 1997.
I
I
1
I
.—.
_
—.
—
Download