What makes a group worth dying for? William B. Swann University of Texas at Austin 1944 2 What the story of the bomber crew tells us Illustrates power of relational ties between individuals in groups These relational ties involve family like allegiances: “brothers in arms” 3 Experts on military link relational ties to self-sacrifice “American soldiers in battle don’t fight for what some president says on TV, they don’t fight for mom, apple pie, the American flag—they fight for one another.” Lt. Colonel H. Moore “…people almost never kill and die for the Cause, but for each other: for their group…—their brotherhood, fatherland, motherland, homeland, totem, or tribe.” S. Atran, 2010. p. 33 Therefore, when it comes to dying for one’s group, relational ties matter But what about collective ties? Social Identity Theory • For Nazi’s, social category was everything; his personal qualities and relationships with guards were nothing • Exported this idea from other-perception to self-perception • To disambiguate impact of categorization, minimized role of: • relational ties • personal self • emotion 5 Fusion approach builds on classic SIT Fusion theory enriches analysis of collective ties with the three influences Tajfel & Turner de-emphasized, namely: • Relational ties • Personal self • Emotion 6 What is identity fusion? Identity fusion is a visceral sense of oneness with a group • Personal self remains salient; fused persons feel that they both strengthen and draw strength from the group, resulting in elevated personal agency • Fused persons view the group as “family” and believe that family membership requires sacrifices • When a family member is imperiled, they experience emotions akin to when self is imperiled (cf. IRT) • Result: Strongly fused people will make extreme sacrifices for the group 7 Fusion vs. Identification • Both rest on the distinction between personal identity and group identity • Both involve an alignment of personal and group identity • BUT alignment invokes different processes 8 Unique paths to self-group overlap Personal Identity Collective Identity Identification Depersonalization— Salient group eclipses person & relationships Fusion Personal selves remain agentic and stable. Relational ties between group members, as well as collective ties, motivate progroup action 9 Motivational assumptions Rationale underlying pro-group behavior • Identification: Utilitarian--to achieve positive and distinct social identity (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) • Fusion: Deontological: e.g., “Anything for family”; not instrumental Structural sources of motivation • Identification: Collective ties • Fusion: Collective ties + personal identity + relational ties At first blush, fusion seems like identification on steroids, but … Galloping is not merely fast walking because: fast vs. slow twitch fibers, gait differs, galloping horses become airborne… 10 Measurement of fusion Pictorial (Swann et al. 2009) “Choose the option that best represents your relationship with the group” Fused Self Group Self Group Self Group Self Group Self Verbal (Gómez et al., 2011) I am one with my country. I am strong because of my country. I make my country strong. I have a deep emotional bond to my country Group Forms of Identity Fusion Local fusion Extended fusion One goal today: Integrating relational & collective ties accounts of extreme behavior • According to identity fusion theory, key is projecting family ties onto a large collective. • This projection process causes people who are strongly fused to the group to react to threats to members of the collective as if they were family. 13 Study 1: compared potency of extended vs. local fusion Measured two things: • Extended: To what extent are you willing to fight/die for your country? • Local: Which of 10 groups are you most willing to die for (family, friends, political party, country)? 14 Participants in 11 countries across 6 continents (Swann, Buhrmester et al., JPSP, 2014) Continent Country N Europe Germany Spain 112 250 Poland Africa SouthAfrica N.Amer. USA S. Amer. Chile Australia Australia Asia China Indonesia Japan India TOTAL 147 317 250 181 100 239 642 107 100 2445 Extended fusion predicts pervasive willingness to fight and die for the country 6 Fusion fight&die 5 r’s =.33 4 .32 .36 .59 .43 .38 3 2 1 0 .61 .48 .54 .61 .45 Yet local fusion is more powerful than extended • Question: Which of 10 groups were participants most willing to die for? • Vast majority (86%) nominated ‘family’(local fusion). Range was 79% [China & Japan] to 96% [Poland] (Swann, Buhrmester et al., JPSP, 2014) 17 Conclusions and a hypothesis (a) Worldwide, motivation to sacrifice the self for large groups (extended fusion) is higher among strongly fused persons relative to weakly fused persons (b) BUT, local fusion (with small groups) is better predictor of self-sacrifice than extended fusion (with large groups) (c) Given “a” and “b”, inducing people to think of large groups as if they were small groups should augment extreme group behavior 18 Perception of shared essence transforms a large group into a small group 19 Creating a sense of shared essence by priming shared genes • Chinese or Indian participants read one of two articles*: • Shared Genes condition: “Scientists Pinpoint Genetic Underpinnings of Race” • Unshared Genes condition: “Scientists Reveal that Race has No Genetic Basis”. * Adapted from Williams & Eberhard, 2008 20 Willingness to fight/die for country as a function of shared biological essences (Swann, Buhrmester et al., 2014) Shared genes condition Non-shared genes condition Weakly fused (-1 SD) Strongly fused (+1 SD) 21 Creating a sense of shared essence by priming shared values • American participants read one of two articles: • Shared core values condition: “Americans agree on core American values” (e.g., freedom, liberty, and democracy). • Unshared core values condition: “Americans disagree on core American values”. 22 Willingness to fight/die for country as a function of priming of shared social essences (Swann, Buhrmester et al., 2014) Weakly Fused (-1 SD) Strongly Fused (+1 SD) 23 Priming studies showed that • Experimentally increasing perception of shared essence can bolster pro-group behavior among fused persons • No direct evidence that familial ties played a role in this process • Do familial ties mediate effect of fusion on pro-group behavior 24 The Boston Marathon bombing Prospective design • Time 1: Measured fusion with America • Time 2a: Bombing occured • Time 2b: Measured familial ties toward USA • “Members of my country are like family to me” • “I see other members of my country as brothers and sisters” • “If someone in my country is hurt or in danger, it is like a family member is hurt or in danger” • Time 2c: Support behaviors • Reports of blood donation, actual letters of support to victims. One dependent variable: letter to Jeff Study 3: 54% wrote to Jeff • Examples: • “Jeff, thank you so much for helping provide clues for the authorities. Even though you were in pain, waking up and wanting to be sure you shared that information is a true show of spirit. America is thinking of you.” • “Jeff, the news images of you and your story has really moved me. I feel so much for you and wish you a happy life and wish I could help in any way. We will all be there for you.” Perceptions of familial ties mediate impact of fusion on support for victims (Buhrmester et al., 2014) Familial Ties β= .63** β= .67** Fusion (Time 1) β= .48** β= .06, n.s. Sobel z = 3.42, p<.001 Actions to support bombing victims The mechanism underlying impact of familial ties? • When a family member is imperiled, it is akin to oneself being imperiled. • One implication: For fused persons, threat to group members invokes emotional reactions that foster pro-group behavior 30 Summoning-the-death-train paradigm Conducted in Madrid 5 Spaniards are doing maintenance works in a railway tunnel. Suddenly, you see that a train is headed their way, and the 5 Spaniards are going to die. You are in another railway tunnel, and if you flip a switch, the train will divert to the railway track where you are. You have two options: … you can let the train go ahead, killing the 5 Spaniards, or … you can flip the switch which diverts the train to the railway track where you are, killing you but leaving the 5 Spaniards unharmed. In this paradigm, 90% of participants viewed self-sacrifice as the “moral” response What do people think as they contemplate self-sacrifice? (Swann, Gomez, et al., JPSP, 2014) • Think aloud study: • Method: • Participants pondered the dilemma out loud while we recorded them. • Later, independent judges rated transcripts. • Replication of earlier work: The more fused people were, the more likely they were to endorse selfsacrifice for their countrymen. 32 What were they thinking? Analysis of audiotapes •Weakly fused persons •Distanced themselves from the group members. •Dispassionately declined to endorse self-sacrifice. •Strongly fused persons •Said that the imperiled group members were “like family.” •Expressed emotional distress but then quickly endorsed self-sacrifice. Emotional engagement meditated the impact of fusion on the decision to self-sacrifice (Swann, Gomez, et al., JPSP, 2014) Emotional Engagement (rated by judges) β= .93*** β= .31*** Fusion β= .79*** (β= .52, p=.07) Decision to self-sacrifice 95% CI: 0.0607, 0.6162, boots = 5000 Conclusions from the think aloud study 1. Emotional reactions mediated effects of fusion on endorsement of sacrificing self for group members. 2. Immediate gut reactions of strongly fused persons told them what to do. First hypothesis from think aloud study If strongly fused persons know immediately that they should self-sacrifice and weakly fused persons slowly add up the pluses and minuses, then minimizing time to reflect should exaggerate fusion effects Again using the summoning the death train dilemma: (a) Recruited strongly and weakly fused participants (b) Gave them time to reflect on the trolley dilemma (no time pressure) or hurried them (time pressure—resource deprivation) (c) Predictions 36 % of respondents who chose self-sacrifice For fused persons, resource deprivation increased self-sacrifice (Swann, Gomez, et al., JPSP, 2014) Control Time pressure N = 607, Fusion X Condition interaction B = .78, OR = 2.18, Wald χ2 = 27.65, p < .001; In Swann et al., under review 37 Second hypothesis from think aloud study If fused persons go with their “gut” response, then utilitarian considerations that require reflection should have little impact on their decision Utilitarian consideration that we focused on: number of people saved by sacrificing the self 38 Self-sacrifice as a function of identity fusion and number of ingroup members (Swann, Gomez, et al., JPSP, 2014) 100 90 79.7 80 76.1 70 57 60 50 Self-sacrifice for 1 ingroup member 40 Self-sacrifice for 5 ingroup members 32.1 30 20 10 0 Weakly fused Strongly fused 39 Third hypothesis from think aloud study If, for fused persons, emotional engagement motivates the decision to self-sacrifice, then activating the emotional system by increasing focus on personal self should amplify the effects of fusion Tested in 2 ways: • Increased arousal • Activated personal self 40 For fused persons, increasing arousal spills over into endorsement of fight/die for Spain (Swann et al., 2010) Control Arousal Extreme actions for Spain Extreme actions for Europe For fused persons, activating self encourages “selfless” actions Self-sacrifice Allow 5 ingroup members to die Preliminary conclusions 1. Think alouds indicate that perception of familial ties & emotional reactions were key to self-sacrifice among strongly fused participants 2. Minimizing time to reflect exaggerated fusion effects; for strongly fused, sacrifice is an emotional, “gut” response 3. Because strongly fused persons “go with the gut”, utilitarian considerations have less impact 4. Because self-sacrifice is an emotional response for strongly fused persons, activating emotions through arousal amplified the effects of fusion 5. In short, deep seated emotions drive effects of fusion 6. If so, fusion may be stable and resistant to change 43 Is fusion irrevocable? Vazquez, Gomez, Swann, 2015 Pilot studies in the lab Reactions of Spanish participants to three historic, group-relevant events • a positive event (Spain’s victory in the 2010 World Cup) • two negative events (a corruption scandal involving the Royal Family and separatist efforts by Catelonia--a prosperous region of Spain) • Before and after event, measured fusion, fight/die, relational & collective ties to Spain 44 Evidence for stability 1. The rank orderings of the fusion scores of strongly fused individuals were more stable (r’s = .60) than those of either moderately or weakly fused individuals (r’s = .30). 2. Strongly fused individuals remained far more fused to Spain than weakly fused individuals 3. Negative events did not diminish a) the extent to which strongly fused intended to fight and die for their country b) felt relational ties to their group 45 Evidence for contextual sensitivity • Average change scores: Strongly fused were most likely to increase fusion in response to positive events and decrease fusion in response to negative events. • The effect of a negative historic event on posttest identity fusion was partially mediated by weakening of the collective (but not relational) ties that strongly fused participants had toward Spain. 46 Does identity fusion matter in naturally occurring contexts? Identity fusion in revolutionary Libya (Whitehouse et al., PNAS, 2014) Data collected by anthropologists Harvey Whitehouse & Brian McQuinn 47 Obtaining participants • McQuinn flew to Misrata in early 2011 during fighting • Developed a relationship with a local • Used that relationship to gain access to members of fighting groups • Whitehouse arrived several months later and began trying to entice me to join them… 48 “Hey Bill, things have almost returned to normal” 49 Methods • 179 Libyans (all male, Mage = 28 yrs) part of 4 pro-revolution combat units. • Participants indicated whether their primary unit role was fighter or logistical supporter (24% were primarily fighters) • Participants indicated how fused they were to: 1. Kin (family) 2. Fighting unit (Katiba) 3. Kin VERSUS Katiba 50 Pilot testing: Fusion with (a) combat unit and (b) kin 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% Unit Kin Fusion with combat unit VERSUS kin as a function of combat Role moderated by 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% Logis cal Supporters 50% Fighters 40% 30% 20% 10% X2 = 4.1, p < .05 0% Unit Kin Summary People are far more likely to endorse dying for a relational group (e.g., family) than a collective group (e.g., country). Yet, everyday, people die for collective groups. How does this happen? 1. When people fuse with a group, they come to see group members as family with whom they share essence, and these perceptions compel self-sacrifice for the group 2. When group members are imperiled, fused person experience strong emotional reactions, which mediate their tendency to sacrifice themselves for the group 3. By encouraging fusion/perception of familial ties, one can harness an emotional apparatus that originally evolved to bind people to members of familial groups (with whom they shared genes) to members of collective groups (with whom they often do not share genes) 53 Some next steps • Power of fusion is derived from combination of relational and collective ties acting together, but there are many other synergies (values, threats, utilitarian concerns, etc) • Are relational ties as essential to these synergies as military historians and anthropologists suggest? For example, will people fight for values that aren’t supported by relational ties? • Identification is associated with outcomes such as selfenhancement and health (Jetten, Haslam et al); fusion is hedonically neutral. What are implications of this distinction? • Fusion and inter-group relations: As Leah Fredman will discuss later, we’ve begun to examine how relational and collective combine to trigger outgroup hate 54 Key collaborators Brock Bastian (UNSW) Jolanda Jetten (Queensland) Michael Buhrmester (Oxford) Sanaz Talifar (Texas) Leah Fredman (Texas) Alexandra Vázquez (UNED) Ángel Gómez (UNED) Harvey Whitehouse (Oxford) 55