12/15/12 Ev ernote Web Sperry, Roger Saturday, December 15 2012, 11:23 AM Roger Sperry (1913-1994) Citation: Todman, D. (2008) History of Neuroscience: Roger Sperry (1913-1994), IBRO History of Neuroscience [http://www.ibro.info/Pub/Pub_Main_Display.asp?LC_Docs_ID=3473] Accessed: date Don Todman Roger Sperry was an iconic figure in neuroscience who shared the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine with David Hubel (b.1926) and Torsten Wiesel (b.1924) in 1981 (Figure 1). His long period of research questioned established dogmas and led to important discoveries in the fields of neural development and hemispheric specialization with his work on the human split-brain. His achievements established him as one of the foremost neuroscientists of the twentieth century. Roger Wolcott Sperry was born in Hartford, Connecticut in 1913. His father died when Roger was 11 and his mother raised two sons by securing a position as assistant to the principal at a local high school. At the Hall High School in West Hartford, Sperry was an accomplished athlete who captained the basketball team and achieved a state record in javelin throw. He studied at Oberlin College in Ohio on an academic scholarship, completing his bachelor's degree in English in 1935 (Voneida, 1994). He continued at Oberlin and received a masters degree in experimental psychology in 1937 under Dr RH Stetson (1872-1950), an authority on the physiology of speech. Subsequently he was accepted as a doctoral student at the University of Chicago under Dr Paul Weiss (1898-1989), a leading zoologist and researcher on the developing nervous system. Neural development and the chemoaffinity theory In the 1940s the prevailing state of knowledge was that the wiring of the nervous system largely came about through experience. His supervisor Weiss was a prominent advocate of this functional principle, which suggested that neurons had the capacity to adjust to the characteristics of associated muscles and/or organs. The concept was summarized in the saying, "function precedes form". Paul Weiss had developed a reputation for research on the developing nervous system through his work, which involved the transplants of limb buds in amphibians. Sperry's research utilized the laboratory rat where he developed skilled neurosurgical techniques. Sperry's initial experiments involved switching of flexor and extensor muscles in rat's hind limbs whilst further work involved crossing the nerves to the same muscles (Sperry, 1940, 1942). According to the functional theory, the laboratory animals would adjust to this change. His studies showed that in all cases the rats displayed the same awkward limb movements with no adaptation. These findings conclusively demonstrated that the motor system of the rat was predetermined and could not be modified by the transplants or with training or time. The significance of this work was soon apparent as it established that the central nervous system circuitry was indeed hard-wired for specific functions (Finger, 1999). https://www.ev ernote.com/edit/7c988bf 6-1320-476b-a886-f 3978916e591#st=p&n=7c988bf 6-1320-47… 1/6 Sperry received his PhD in 1941 and then continued post-doctoral studies with Karl Lashley (18901958) at Harvard University. Lashley was a leading psychobiologist who also shared the functionalist views of Weiss. In 1942, Sperry and Lashley worked at the Yerkes laboratory of primate biology in Florida where Sperry pursued his work on neuronal specificity. It was here that he performed his studies involving the rotation of eyes in amphibians. Previous work had found that sectioning of the optic nerves in newts could result in nerve regeneration when the eye was returned into its socket. Sperry designed a series of experiments in which the optic nerves were cut and the eyes rotated 180°. The question to be tested was whether their vision would return to normality after a period of adaptation or would the animals view the world as upside down. In all cases the results were unequivocal. The newts continued to act as if the world was upside down. When food was presented in the upper field, they looked down and vice versa. The responses remained the same even after a period of training (Sperry, 1945) (Figure 2). When Sperry traced the regenerated optic nerves into the brain; he found that the original pattern from eye to brain was maintained, suggesting that the nerves were guided in some way to their original places of termination (Sperry, 1948). The work was amplified by further studies in amphibia and on nerve-muscle and retino-tectal regeneration in fish. Figure 2: When the eye is rotated 1800, the frog's prey catching behavior is inverted (after Sperry, 1945). Sperry's experimental results led him to theorize on the nature of this nerve guidance in the developing or regenerating nervous system. The original concept of a chemical attractant secreted by target cells and guiding developing neurons was suggested by Santiago Ramón y Cajal (Cajal, 1909). Cajal's theory of neurotropism in embryonic nerve cell development was based on hypothetical grounds. Sperry cautiously developed a chemical theory for nerve guidance that culminated in his chemoaffinity theory (Berlucchi, 2006). He proposed the theory in a series of publications culminating in his paper in 1963 for the National Academy of Sciences (Sperry, 1963). When he was awarded the Gerard Prize of the Society for Neuroscience in 1979, the citation stated: "The chemoaffinity theory soon found wide recognition and is one of the few important general unifying principles in today's developmental neuro-biology despite the fact that its molecular basis is unknown" (Voneida, 1994). Sperry's work had an impact on the practice of neurosurgery during World War II. During the war, he worked with the Office of Scientific Research and Development (OSRD) in the Nerve Injury Project. His discoveries led to a change in the surgical management of servicemen with nerve injuries. Previously it had been common for surgeons to transplant nerves into antagonistic muscle groups and to submit the patient to retraining in an attempt to restore normal functioning. In clinical practice the results were unsatisfactory and Sperry's studies led to substantial changes in these treatment protocols. 12/15/12 Ev ernote Web The corpus callosum and split-brain experiments Sperry returned to the University of Chicago in 1946 where he worked first in the department of anatomy. In 1949 he married Norma Deupree in Wichita, Kansas. Shortly afterwards he developed tuberculosis from a laboratory monkey which he had used for nerve transplants. He was appointed an associate professor of psychology at the University of Chicago in 1952 but moved to the warmer climates of California in 1954 as the Hixon Professor of psychobiology at the California Institute of Technology (Caltech). Sperry's interest in the corpus callosum began as a graduate student. The function of this structure, also known as the great cerebral commissure connecting the two cerebral hemispheres, had remained largely unknown. In the early 1930s, Walter Dandy, neurosurgeon at Johns Hopkins University, split the corpus callosum to gain access to third ventricle tumors (Dandy, 1936). His observations suggested that patients had no observable ill-effects or mental changes from this procedure. In the early 1940s, surgical section of the corpus callosum for resistant epilepsy was performed by William Van Wagenen at University of Rochester medical school. Andrew Akelaitis performed behavioral and psychological tests on these patients after surgery and reported largely negative findings with no demonstrable or permanent deficits on cognitive tests (Akelaitis, 1941). To other investigators these findings were perplexing as it was known that the corpus callosum consisted of over 200 million nerve fibres connecting the cerebral hemispheres. This lack of known function led Karl Lashley to remark that the function of a corpus callosum "must be mechanical, to keep the hemispheres from sagging" (Voneida, 1994). In the early 1960s, Sperry had the opportunity to examine patients who had callosalectomies by Drs Philip Vogel and Joseph Bogen in Los Angeles. With his graduate students, Colwyn Trevarthen and Michael Gazzaniga, he devised a series of tests to examine patients after surgery. The experiments carefully examined language function as well as a range of cognitive and behavioral studies. Their work led to dramatic discoveries demonstrating differences between the hemispheres which had been previously unrecognized. The left-brain was superior in the fields of analytical and linguistic processing, whilst the right hemisphere was superior in visuospatial abilities (Gazzaniga, 1965, 1967). The clinical studies were augmented by laboratory experiments which involved sectioning the corpus callosum of cat's brain. Sperry and his graduate student Ronald Myers demonstrated that behavioral changes learned in one hemisphere were not transferred to the other (Myers, 1953). From this work Sperry remarked in a 1964 paper published in Scientific American: "It was as though each hemisphere were a separate mental domain operating with complete disregard, indeed, with a complete lack of awareness, of what went on in the other. The split-brain animal behaved in the test situation as if it had two entirely separate brains" (Sperry, 1964). The work was published in two seminal papers in Brain in 1965 and 1967 (Gazzaniga, 1965, 1967). Their observations showed that the right hemisphere also had language capabilities and in one sense, a person could literally have two consciousnesses. The general public became fascinated with these split-brain studies although some of the ideas became distorted in the media and elsewhere. The notion that the right hemisphere was for emotion and art and the left hemisphere for reasoning and intellectual pursuits was an over-simplification of Sperry's studies and papers (Figure 3). Nonetheless, these studies on functional specialization in the cerebral hemispheres brought him to wide public attention and secured his share of the 1981 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine. https://www.ev ernote.com/edit/7c988bf 6-1320-476b-a886-f 3978916e591#st=p&n=7c988bf 6-1320-47… 3/6 Figure 3: A schematic representation of certain specialized hemispheric functions (after Zaidel and Iacaboni, 2002). Conclusion Sperry continued at Caltech until his retirement in 1984. In his later life, much of his work was focused on the theories of mind and consciousness and the mind-brain problem. His first paper, which touched on the "central issue", was in 1965 in his chapter, New Views on the Nature of Man (Sperry, 1965). He proposed a "mentalist position", by which he meant that the pattern of brain dynamics directed and governed neuronal traffic. Thus, in Sperry's view, consciousness was generated by neural activity but was nonetheless separate from it. In a sense, consciousness emerged as an independent entity separate from the chemistry and biophysics of single nerve cells. He proposed an analogy with quantum physics in which interactions of subatomic particles result in emergent properties that do not resemble the particles from which they arose. Despite these distinctions, he remained a monist and did not accept the concept of Cartesian dualism or the "dualist interactionism" advocated by Popper and Eccles in 1977 (Popper, 1977). Although Sperry's writings had a strong philosophical element, his discoveries in the split-brain model showed that consciousness could be studied in the laboratory under controlled conditions. His work has been the impetus for the "cognitive revolution" in which scientists have looked at consciousness and mental processes in new ways. By nature Sperry was a shy and somewhat aloof person, but he was also known for his warm hospitality to his students and many international collaborators. In his latter years he suffered from a form of progressive lateral sclerosis which affected his mobility and speech, though his intellectual function was spared. His acceptance speech at the Nobel awards was read for him. From his early academic studies in English literature he had a life-long interest in the arts and was an accomplished sculptor and painter. His many scientific discoveries revolutionized foundational concepts and overturned prevailing theories in neuroscience. In addition to the Nobel Prize, he received numerous awards and honors including the Lasker award in 1979 and a National Medal of Science. As Viktor Hamburger stated at the award of the Ralph Gerard prize of the Society for 12/15/12 Ev ernote Web Neuroscience in 1979, "I know of nobody else who has disposed of cherished ideas of both his doctoral and his post-doctoral sponsor, both at that time the acknowledged leaders in their fields" (Voneida, 1994). Roger Sperry died in 1994 and was eulogized as an exceptional neuroscientist and person of high ethical standards. Reflecting on his career that had encompassed new ideas in neural development, split-brain syndrome and consciousness theory, David Hubel concluded: "His contributions to neuro-biology were titanic" (Hubel, 1994). Don Todman Department of Neurology School of Medicine University of Queensland, Australia drtodman@optusnet.com.au Selected quotations 1. "With few exceptions, the bulk of the collected lesion evidence up through the 1950's into the early 60's converged to support the picture of a leading, more highly evolved and intellectual left hemisphere and a relatively retarded right hemisphere that by contrast, in the typical right hand of brain, is not only mute and agraphic but also dyslexic, word deaf and apraxic, and lacking generally in higher cognitive function" (Nobel lecture, 1981). 2. "Where there used to be a chasm and irreconcilable conflict between the scientific and the traditional humanistic views of man and the world, we know perceive a continuum. A unifying new interpretive framework emerges with far-reaching impact not only for science but for those ultimate value-belief guidelines by which mankind has tried to live and find meaning" (Nobel lecture, 1981). 3. "I have never been entirely satisfied with the materialistic or behaviouristic thesis that a complete explanation of brain function is possible is purely objective terms with no reference whatever to subjective experience: ie, that in scientific analysis we can confidently and advantageously disregard the subjective properties of the brain process. I do not mean we should abandon the objective approach or repeat the errors of the earlier introspective era. It is just that I find it difficult to believe that the sensations and other subjective experiences per se serve no function, have no operational value and no place in our working models of the brain" (Discussion in the first conference on The Central Nervous System and Behavior, 1958). Bibliography Akelaitis AJ (1940) A study of gnosis, praxis and language following partial and complete section of the corpus callosum. Transactions of the American Neurologic Association, 66: 182-185. Berlucchi G (2006) Revisiting the 1981 Nobel Prize to Roger Sperry, David Hubel, and Torsten Wiesel on the Occasion of the Centennial of the Prize to Golgi and Cajal. Journal History of Neurosciences, 15, 4, 369-375. Cajal SR (1909) Histologie du Système Nerveux de l'Homme et des vertébrés. Tome 1. Paris, Maloine. Dandy WE (1936) Operative experience in cases of pineal tumor. Archives of Surgery, 33:19-46. Finger S (1999) Minds Behind the Brain: A History of the Pioneers and their Discoveries. New York, Oxford University Press. Gazzaniga M, Bogen JE, Sperry RW (1965) Observations on visual perception after disconnection of the cerebral hemispheres in man. Brain, 88:221-236. Gazzaniga M, Sperry RW (1967) Language after section of the cerebral commisures. Brain, 90: 131-148. https://www.ev ernote.com/edit/7c988bf 6-1320-476b-a886-f 3978916e591#st=p&n=7c988bf 6-1320-47… 12/15/12 Ev ernote Web Hubel D (1994) Obituary-Roger Sperry (1913-1994) Nature, 369:186. Myers RE, Sperry RW (1953) Interocular transfer of a visual form discrimination habit in cats after section of the optic chiasma and corpus callosum. Anatomical Record, 115:351-352. Popper KR, Eccles JC (1977) The Self and its Brain. New York, Springer. Sperry RW (1940) The functional results of muscle transposition in the hind limb of the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 73:379-404. Sperry RW (1941) The effect of crossing antagonistic muscles in the hind limb of the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 75:1-19. Sperry RW (1943) Visuomotor coordination in the newt (Triturus viridescens) after regeneration of the optic nerve. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 79: 33-55. Sperry RW (1948) Restoration of vision after crossing of optic nerves and after contralateral transplantation of eye. Journal of Neurophysiology, 8:17-28. Sperry RW (1963) Chemoaffinity in the orderly growth of nerve fiber patterns and connections. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 50:703-710. Sperry RW (1964) The great cerebral commissure. Scientific American, 210:42-52. Sperry RW (1965) Mind, brain and humanist values. In Platt JR (Ed) New Views of the Nature of Man. Chicago, Chicago University Press. Sperry RW (1981) Nobel Lecture. http://nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/medicine/laureates/1981/sperry-lecture.html Voneida T (1994) Biographical memoirs, Roger Wolcott Sperry. National Academy of Sciences. http://www.nap.edu/html/biomems/rsperry.html Zaidel E Iacoboni M (2002) The Cognitive Neuroscience of the Corpus Callosum. Boston, MIT Press. https://www.ev ernote.com/edit/7c988bf 6-1320-476b-a886-f 3978916e591#st=p&n=7c988bf 6-1320-47… 6/6