Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Theoretical Biology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yjtbi The effect of dominance on polymorphism in Müllerian mimic species V. Llaurens a,n, S. Billiard b, M. Joron a a Origine Structure et Evolution de la Biodiversité, CNRS UMR 7205, Muséum National d'Histoire Naturelle, CP50, 45, rue Buffon, 75005 Paris, France Laboratoire de Génétique et évolution des populations végétales, CNRS UMR 8198, Université des Sciences et Technologies de Lille 1, Bâtiment SN2, 59655 Villeneuve d'Ascq Cedex, France b H I G H L I G H T S We investigate the influence of dominance on the maintenance of polymorphism in a Müllerian mimicry system in a spatially heterogeneous environment. Complete dominance was shown to extend the parameter space, and thus the breadth of ecological situations, where Müllerian mimicry polymorphism was maintained. Overdominance, which promotes polymorphism, can arise given certain levels of toxicity and predator discrimination accuracy. Dominant alleles were shown to reach lower frequencies than recessive alleles when selection on both homozygotes was symmetrical. art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t Article history: Received 17 May 2013 Received in revised form 7 August 2013 Accepted 9 August 2013 Available online 21 August 2013 Dominance controls the phenotype of heterozygous individuals, and plays an important role in the maintenance of polymorphism. Here we focus on the dominance acting on warning-pattern polymorphism in species engaged in Müllerian mimicry. Müllerian mimics are toxic species which display bright colour patterns used as a warning signal to predators and are subject to local positive density-dependent selection. Some Müllerian mimics are polymorphic due to a selection/migration balance in spatially heterogeneous communities of prey. Since heterozygotes at a locus controlling warning pattern might exhibit intermediate, non-mimetic heterozygous morphs, dominance is likely to influence the polymorphism at this locus. Using a deterministic model describing migration, density-dependent predation and reproduction, we investigated the influence of dominance on the dynamics of alleles at locus determining mimetic phenotype. Our results suggest dominance may interact with migration and selection and plays an important role in shaping the conditions of polymorphism persistence and the frequency of alleles at this locus. Our results thus highlight the important role of dominance in the dynamics of polymorphism at loci under balancing selection due to environmental heterogeneity. & 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Mimicry Balancing selection Spatial heterogeneity Aposematism 1. Introduction Dominance among alleles at the same genetic locus is a widespread phenomenon, described by Mendel in his famous study of pea crosses in the 19th century (Mendel, 1895). By modifying the phenotype of heterozygotes, dominance can have an important impact on the fitness of alleles. For instance, alleles with deleterious properties are generally found recessive or partially recessive to wild-type alleles (Orr, 1991). Dominance has been shown to influence the invasion of a new mutation arising in a population, more dominant mutations tending to get fixed more often than more recessive ones (i.e. Haldane ‘s sieve (Haldane, 1927)). n Corresponding author. Tel.: þ 33 01 40 79 38 61; fax: þ 33 01 40 79 33 42. E-mail addresses: llaurens@mnhn.fr, llaurens.violaine@laposte.net (V. Llaurens). 0022-5193/$ - see front matter & 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2013.08.006 Dominance also plays a significant role in the shape of allele frequency clines maintained by frequency-dependent selection: Mallet and Barton (Mallet and Barton, 1989) demonstrated that allelic dominance may result in the formation of asymmetrical clines and thus favour cline movement. Yeaman and Otto (Yeaman and Otto, 2011) also confirmed the impact of dominance in the invasion of new alleles in a classical two-population migration/ selection model. Since dominance only plays a role in the expression of the phenotype in heterozygous individuals, it is expected to play an important role in the evolution of polymorphic loci where heterozygotes are at high frequency. However, the influence of dominance on the persistence of polymorphism and on allele-frequency distribution has received little attention. Balanced polymorphism is maintained in well-documented regimes of selection such as heterozygote advantage, or negative frequency-dependent selection 102 V. Llaurens et al. / Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 (Charlesworth, 2006). However, the persistence of polymorphism due to spatial heterogeneity might also represent a widespread situation where balanced polymorphism is maintained (Spichtig and Kawecki, 2004). In previous theoretical studies on the effect of dominance on polymorphisms maintained by a migration/selection balance, dominance was modelled using a coefficient which directly modulates the fitness of heterozygotes (Yeaman and Otto, 2011; Otto and Bourguet, 1999; Orr and Betancourt, 2001), and, in turn, differences in invasion capacities for alleles with a positive vs. a negative effect on the fitness of heterozygotes (i.e. overdominance vs. underdominance). With this specific mathematical formalisation of dominance, the biological mechanism of dominance acting on the fitness of heterozygotes is not straightforward and this makes it difficult to confront theoretical predictions with empirical data. More importantly, predictions obtained by these models cannot be readily applied to cases (such as Müllerian mimicry) where fitness is frequency and density-dependent, i.e. when the fitness of heterozygotes depends both on the phenotype expressed by heterozygotes and on the overall distribution of the phenotypes in the population (and in the larger community). To address this limitation, we here consider a dominance coefficient acting on the phenotype and not directly on fitness. In this study, we focus on the role of dominance in shaping phenotypic variation and the maintenance of polymorphism in prey species engaged in Müllerian mimicry. Müllerian mimicry is the adaptive resemblance of multiple noxious prey species whose shared phenotype functions as a common warning signal to predators (Ruxton et al., 2004). Müllerian mimicry is a widespread phenomenon documented in many organisms from arthropods (Millipedes (Marek and Bond, 2009), Hemiptera, (Zrzavy and Nedved, 1999)) to vertebrates (amphibians (Symula et al., 2001), snakes(Sanders et al., 2006), birds (Dumbacher and Fleischer, 2001)…). Predators learn to avoid chemically defended, warningly coloured prey based on previous experience (Chouteau and Angers, 2011; Mappes et al., 2005) thus favouring the survival of prey bearing the commonest (most frequent) patterns in a local community (Pinheiro, 2003), a well-known example of positive frequency-dependent selection (Speed and Turner, 1999). Frequency-dependence operates locally and favours resemblance among co-occurring species, favouring the locally commonest phenotypes and selecting away rarer variants. However, selection on warning patterns may act in opposing directions in distinct localities, and mimetic communities indeed vary widely in their warning patterns across geographic areas or across habitats. The evolution and maintenance of warning-pattern polymorphism would not be predicted in the situation where a single, best-protected phenotype is expected to reach fixation, but polymorphisms may be maintained in the case of spatially variable selection, e.g. when the composition of the community of toxic prey changes spatially, as found for instance in the poison frog Ranitomeya imitator (Chouteau et al., 2011) or in the butterfly Heliconius numata (Joron et al., 1999). In Heliconius numata, multiple wing-pattern forms co-occur, each one being a precise mimic of a distinct species in the distantly related genus Melinaea. Finescale spatial variations in the relative abundances of the Melinaea species are positively correlated with the frequency of the matching wing colour pattern in Heliconius numata (Joron et al., 1999). The fine spatial heterogeneity in the mimetic community composition is thought to translate into variations in the direction of selection for warning pattern resemblance in distinct populations. This variable selection generates balancing selection on wing colour patterns at a larger spatial scale. In the fitness landscape of mimicry, shaped by frequencydependent selection, peaks correspond to common, mimetic phenotypes, and fitness valleys correspond to all rare, non-mimetic, and intermediate phenotypes which are strongly selected against. The persistence of polymorphisms will be influenced by the strength of selection acting on heterozygotes. By controlling the mimicry of heterozygous genotypes, dominance relationships among alleles at a locus controlling mimetic phenotypic elements are of particular relevance. For instance complete dominance of mutant alleles would allow full resemblance of heterozygote genotypes with this mutant allele to one of the mimetic homozygotes. Moreover, under strong balancing selection, heterozygote frequency is expected to be high, and dominance may play an important role in the selection on alleles, and on polymorphism maintenance. The evolutionary dynamics and genetics of mimicry have been well studied in butterflies. The genetic loci determining mimicry variation in the genera Papilio and Heliconius have received sustained attention, and both dominance and co-dominance relationships are observed among alleles coding for alternative mimetic phenotypes (see (Clarke and Sheppard, 1960) and (Clarke et al., 1985)). In Heliconius, alleles at homologous loci can have differently ordered dominance relationships in distinct species (e.g. Heliconius melpomene vs. Heliconius erato (Nijhout, 1991); Heliconius cydno vs. Heliconius melpomene (Naisbit et al., 2003)). Dominance can also vary within species: in Papilio dardanus, dominance tends to be stronger in crosses between individuals drawn from the same geographical area than in crosses involving individuals from different areas (Nijhout, 2003). Such variation in dominance level in natural populations underlines the lack of understanding of the role of dominance in the evolution and maintenance polymorphisms of these adaptive patterns. To fill this gap, we built a model based on a previously described theoretical haploid model (Joron and Iwasa, 2005) proposed to depict the evolution of warning-signal mimicry in unpalatable prey. We extended this model to a diploid species, in order to investigate the influence of dominance on warning-colour polymorphism. 2. Material and methods The model presented here is based on the previous model of Joron and Iwasa (Joron and Iwasa, 2005) which was built to describe Müllerian mimicry in a spatially distributed community. We extended this model to describe a diploid species with dominance relationships among alleles at the locus controlling mimetic colour pattern. We considered a system with two patches, 1 and 2, each one containing a single mimicry community (i.e. a number of species all mimicking each other and bearing a similar wing pattern signalling their toxicity to predators). The two patches differed in the mimetic pattern adopted by local species: the mimicry community exhibited morph A in patch 1 and morph B in patch 2. The two communities constituted the mimetic environment and were assumed to have a fixed abundance and a fixed warning colour pattern (A or B). We studied the polymorphism dynamics of a focal species evolving in this spatially distributed system. The focal species was a Müllerian mimic, mimicking morph A or B, bearing its own toxicity, and forming two distinct populations exchanging migrants between patches 1 and 2. This mimetic species was diploid and the mimetic morph A or B was assumed to be determined by a single locus with two segregating alleles a and b. 2.1. Modelling dominance Individuals of genotypes aa and bb displayed phenotype A and B respectively. The phenotype of the heterozygote ab depended on V. Llaurens et al. / Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 103 The survival of the individuals in a population not only depended on the community environment represented by model species but also on its own abundance in the community. Predator avoidance was thus assumed to depend both on the density of each morph and on the unpalatibility of the focal species, l. The shape of this function was similar to the function used in haploid model of Müllerian mimicry used by Joron and Iwasa (Joron and Iwasa, 2005). Homozygotes and heterozygotes could display similar colour patterns, and therefore benefited from their mutual abundance, to an extent which depends on the visual ability of the predator, i.e. depending on the generalization function, g(h), described above. The change in the density of homozygote genotypes aa and bb in population i due to predation, was given by Fig. 1. Shape of the generalization function according for different values of γ. X-axis exhibits the dominance coefficient h and y-axis the generalization function g. Dashed lines represent g(h) and plain lines g(1-h), black lines : γ ¼0.3, dark grey : γ ¼0.1, grey : γ ¼0.05, light grey : γ ¼0.01. the dominance coefficient h. The similarity between the homozygotes aa and bb and heterozygote ab morphs was described by a generalization function with a Gaussian shape, g(h), following equation: gðhÞ ¼ eðh1Þ =2γ with γ determining the width of the Gaussian function: 2 2 ð1Þ This shape corresponded to the function generally assumed for predator generalization, i.e. the range for which patterns are too similar to be discriminated by predators (Ruxton et al., 2008). The functions g(h) and g(1 h) represented the resemblance of the heterozygote ab to the homozygote aa and to the homozygote bb respectively. When h tended to 1, the heterozygote ab displayed exactly the same phenotype as the homozygote aa. Fig. 1 shows the effect of γ on the shape of the function with decreasing γ leading to steeper shapes, in which co-dominance (h ¼0.5) leaded to high dissimilarity of the heterozygote ab with respect to both homozygotes aa and bb. 2.2. Modelling allele dynamics We assumed a continuous time deterministic model where all events occurred simultaneously. Three types of events occurred in the populations 1 and 2: migration, viability selection due to death by predation, and density-dependent reproduction. The production of genotypes was assumed to follow Mendelian segregation and was modelled explicitly. The change in the density of each genotype with time during each event was detailed below. 2.2.1. Migration The change by time unit of Nuvi (density of genotype uv, i.e. aa, bb or ab, in population i) due to migration between populations i and j was given by dNuvi ¼ mðNuvjNuviÞ dt dN aai daai ¼ N ; dt 1 þ lðN aai þgðhÞN abi Þ aai ð3Þ dN bbi dbbi ¼ N ; dt 1 þ lðNbbi þ gð1hÞN abi Þ bbi ð4Þ The change of the density of heterozygote genotype ab in populations 1 and 2 after predation was given by dN ab1 gðhÞdaa1 þ ð1gðhÞdbb1 Þ ¼ N ; 1 þ lðN ab1 þ gðhÞN aa1 þ gð1hÞNbb1 Þ ab1 dt ð5Þ dN ab2 gð1hÞdaa2 þ ð1gð1hÞdbb2 Þ ¼ N : 1 þ lðN ab2 þ gðhÞN aa2 þ gð1hÞNbb2 Þ ab2 dt ð6Þ For the sake of simplicity, we assumed a symmetrical condition where daa1 ¼dbb2 ¼ d (1 s) and dbb1 ¼daa2 ¼ d (1 þ s) where d represented the mean predation risk and s the spatial heterogeneity due to the distribution of the two model species. 2.2.3. Reproduction The parameter r was set as the intrinsic per female capita growth rate, K the carrying capacity, assumed equal in both populations, and N i ¼ N aai þ N abi þN bbi the total density of individuals in population i. We also assume that the sex-ratio was balanced and that only females gave birth to new individuals. We assumed that reproduction was density-dependent and that the per capita growth rate in population i was for all genotype: ðr=2Þð1ðN i =KÞÞ. Since we aimed to explicitly model sexual reproduction, the rate at which each genotype was produced depended on the rate at which each type of cross occurs, i.e. on the density of each genotype. We assumed Mendelian segregation at the mimicry locus. The production rate of individuals with genotype uv in population i was then described as follows: r N 1 i f uvi ; ð7Þ 2 K with with ia j and m as the migration rate: ð2Þ f aai ¼ ðN abi þ 2N aai Þ2 ; 4Ni ð8aÞ 2.2.2. Survival within population We assumed daai and dbbi to be the death coefficient of the homozygotes aa and bb in population i. These death coefficients depended on the presence of the model species in the population considered. Hence, daa1 rdbb1 and daa2 Zdbb2 because the genotype aa (phenotype A) was favoured in population 1 and not in population 2 due to mimicry with model species at different abundances. f abi ¼ 2ðN abi þ2N aai ÞðNabi þ 2N bbi Þ ; 4N i ð8bÞ f bbi ¼ ðN abi þ 2Nbbi Þ2 : 4N i ð8cÞ Although colour patterns are likely to play a role in sexual selection, its influence on mating success was not included in our model for simplicity. 104 V. Llaurens et al. / Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 2.2.4. General equations for the population dynamics Combining the density change of each genotype with time due to the combination of migration, viability selection by predation and reproduction gave the total density change in a continuous time model. The dynamics were described by six ordinary differential equations, three for each population. The equations were for iaj: dN aai daai ¼ mðN aaj N aai Þ N dt 1 þlðN aai þ gðhÞN abi Þ aai r N ðN abi þ 2Naai Þ2 þ 1 i ; 2 K 4N i dN bbi dbbi ¼ m N bbj N bbi N dt 1 þlðN bbi þg ð1hÞN abi Þ bbi 2 r N ðN abi þ 2Nbbi Þ þ 1 i ; 2 K 4N i ð9aÞ ð9bÞ and dN ab1 g ðhÞdaa1 þ ð1g ðhÞdbb1 Þ ¼ mðN ab2 N ab1 Þ N 1 þ lðNab1 þ g ðhÞN aa1 þg ð1hÞN bb1 Þ ab1 dt r N 1 2ðNab1 þ 2Naa1 ÞðN ab1 þ 2N bb1 Þ þ 1 ; ð9cÞ 2 4N 1 K dN ab2 gð1hÞdaa2 þ ð1gð1hÞdbb2 Þ ¼ mðN ab1 N ab2 Þ N 1 þ lðNab2 þ gðhÞN aa2 þgð1hÞN bb2 Þ ab2 dt r N2 2ðN ab2 þ2N aa2 ÞðNab2 þ 2Nbb2 Þ þ 1 : ð9dÞ 2 4N 2 K 2.3. Analysis of the model 2.3.1. Stability analyses We first performed a stability analysis of these equations (Otto and Day, 2007). For a given equilibrium, eigenvalues were computed, the sign of which determined the conditions of its stability. Combining the analysis of several equilibria and their stabilities revealed the conditions for the persistence of polymorphism. We were able to obtain explicit expressions of the equilibria and their stability for two simple cases regarding migration: (1) when there is no migration, and (2) when migration rate is very high. In case (2), we assumed that the two populations behave as a single large population, where Naa1 ¼ Naa2 ¼Naa, Nab1 ¼Nab2 ¼Nab and Nbb1 ¼ Nbb2 ¼ Nbb. For all other value of the migration, we investigated the polymorphism maintenance by a numerical analysis detailed hereafter. 2.3.2. Numerical analyses of equilibria for intermediate migration rates To find the equilibria for any value of the migration rate m, we performed a numerical analysis using the FindRoot function in Mathematica. The first investigated equilibrium was for m¼ 0.01, specifying in the FindRoot function that the starting point was near the equilibrium values for m ¼0, as it had been computed in the previous section. We computed the second equilibrium for m ¼0.02 using the equilibrium found for m ¼0.01 as the starting value, and so on until m ¼0.4 with an increment of 0.01 between each computation. We found that the maintenance of polymorphism can depend on the initial state of the population. To approximately determine the size of the attraction basin, we performed additional numerical analyses to investigate the conditions for the maintenance of polymorphism for 1000 different initial conditions where the number of individuals of a given genotype was randomly drawn in a uniform distribution between 0 and 100. We iterated equations Eqs. (9a),(9b),(9c) and (9d) until the frequency change between two iterations was below 10 3 for both alleles. Three possible outcomes were possible: maintenance of polymorphism (both alleles a and b were present in the population at the end of the computation); allele a is lost; or allele b is lost. In the same way, we also computed the equilibrium for varying values of the dominance coefficient h for a given value of the migration rate m using the FindRoot function. Finally, we also investigated the effect of the toxicity, l, and the heterogeneity of the environment, s, on the maintenance of polymorphism for different dominance levels, h. For this, we performed a numerical analysis, starting with the initial conditions given by the equations from the equilibrium when m ¼ 0, we introduced a small quantity of heterozygotes to each patch so that the initial frequency of the heterozygotes was 0.001. We performed 1000 iterative computations of Eqs. (9a)–(9d) and checked if the densities of all genotypes were higher than 1 in both populations, which corresponds to a frequency higher than 10 3 in a 1000 individuals population. If so, we considered that the polymorphism was maintained. 2.3.3. Parameter values Unless otherwise stated, we used the following values for the parameters. The generalization function was chosen to be steep (γ ¼0.01, see Fig. 1) in the numerical analyses to simulate clear phenotypic distinction of the co-dominant heterozygote ab from either homozygote (aa and bb). We assumed a low value, because the few studies testing the ability of predators to discriminate the different morphs of Müllerian mimic species show that they were able to discriminate ‘local' versus ‘exotic' morphs quite accurately (Chouteau and Angers, 2011; Merrill et al., 2012). Computations were performed assuming a strict symmetry between the population 1 and 2, using an intermediate general predation risk d¼ 0.5 and a high spatial heterogeneity parameter s¼ 0.9, thus simulating a strong predation difference between populations. This assumption of a greater advantage provided by the model species with respect to the within-species density dependence is biologically relevant: several species can be involved in a mimicry ring leading the total of individuals to generally outnumber individuals from the focal species only. We also assumed toxicity l ¼0.0025, growth rate r ¼1 and carrying capacity K ¼1000. Note that since model species did not evolve, we used the Kl product as an estimate of the relative contribution of the mimetic species to the community toxicity. 3. Results 3.1. Polymorphism without migration First we analysed the case of two isolated populations exchanging no migrants (m¼ 0). Four possible equilibria were detected (Table 1). A first equilibrium corresponded to the extinction of both populations and the second equilibrium to two monomorphic populations, each one being fixed for the allele matching the local community. The other two equilibria described a monomorphic mimic species with the same allele invading both populations. However the latter two equilibria were always unstable when predation risk and spatial heterogeneity were positive (d 40 and s 40), meaning that these equilibria cannot occur with predation. In the absence of migration, polymorphism cannot be maintained within each population here, regardless of the level of dominance (h). V. Llaurens et al. / Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 105 Table 1 Description of the four possible equilibria obtained through the analytical analyses, assuming no migration (m¼ 0). Equilibrium Frequencies of genotypes Description of equilibrium Stability conditions 1 Nab1 ¼ Nab2 ¼ Naa2 ¼ Nbb1 ¼ Naa1 ¼ Nbb2 ¼ 0 Extinct populations r Stable when d 4 2ð1sÞ 2 Nab1 ¼ Nab2 ¼ Naa2 ¼ Nbb1 ¼ 0 ffi pffi pffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi 2 Naa1 ¼ Nbb2 ¼ ðKl1Þr þ r ðKl2lrþ 1Þ r8dKlð1sÞ p ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi ffi pffi 2 Naa1 ¼ ðKl1Þr þ r ðKl2lrþ 1Þ r8dKlð1sÞ ffi pffi pffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi 2 Naa2 ¼ ðKl1Þr þ r ðKl2lrþ 1Þ r8dKlð1 þ sÞ ffi pffi pffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi 2 Nbb1 ¼ ðKl1Þr þ r ðKl2lrþ 1Þ r8dKlð1 þ sÞ ffi pffi pffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi 2 Nbb2 ¼ ðKl1Þr þ r ðKl2lrþ 1Þ r8dKlð1sÞ Two monomorphic populations r Stable when d o 2ð1sÞ Allele a fixed in both population Always unstable for realistic range of predation parameters (d 4 0 and s 4 0) Allele b fixed in both populations Always unstable for realistic range of predation parameters (d 4 0 and s 4 0) 3 4 3.2. Polymorphism with infinite migration Secondly, we explored analytically the case where migration was high. There, the two patches were still occupied by different mimicry communities, but the focal species showed unlimited migration between the two populations, approaching the behaviour of a single population. Three equilibria were detected: the first one was the fixation of allele a, the second one the fixation of allele b, and the third one the extinction of both populations (supplementary Table 1). For each equilibrium, the sign of the leading eigenvalue could be examined to infer equilibrium stability and therefore the conditions under which polymorphism was maintained. However, these conditions were complex because many parameters were involved (for more detail see supplementary Table 2). Briefly, polymorphism maintenance under high migration depended mainly on spatial heterogeneity s and the shape of the generalization function γ: when both parameters were high, polymorphism was maintained. This means that polymorphism persisted when there was a sufficient spatial heterogeneity in the distribution of mimicry community (leading to disruptive selection on the mimetic pattern in the focal species) and when the generalization function was high enough to allow heterozygotes to be considered similar to the mimetic patterns. In this case, spatially heterogeneous selection was sufficient to maintain polymorphism. The dominance coefficient h played a significant role on polymorphism only when parameters s and γ are relatively small. This was because dominance has an influence on predation only if predators are able to distinguish heterozygotes from homozygotes (γ small). In this case, a tight equilibrium between dominance, demographic parameters (growth rate r and carrying capacity K) and toxicity could allow the maintenance of polymorphism by balancing the number of different phenotypes matching each prey community. In particular, the Kl product (the total contribution of the focal species to overall toxicity at the community level) modified the strength of selection exerted by the structure of predation given by parameters s and γ (i.e. spatial distribution of model species). When Kl was large, the selection regime switched from balancing selection due to spatial heterogeneity to directional selection due to within-species positive frequencydependent selection. Dominance thus plays a complex role in the persistence of polymorphism in a heterogeneous environment, by interacting with toxicity, community structure and demography. 3.3. Persistence of polymorphism with intermediate migration As previously shown in studies on spatially distributed populations, migration is a key parameter in the persistence of polymorphism. In this system, migration is balancing the effect of local directional selection and, therefore, polymorphism is generally maintained for low migration rates but becomes unstable above a critical value of migration (Joron and Iwasa, 2005). Numerical simulations were used to analyse the influence of migration on polymorphism in our two-population model. As shown in Fig. 2, polymorphism was maintained in both populations for all dominance levels when migration is relatively low. When m 40.3, the two populations started to behave like a single population, and for any dominance coefficient h, one allele became fixed while the other went extinct. However, the interaction between dominance and migration had an influence on the persistence of polymorphism. Indeed, dominance determined the strength of migration above which polymorphism was lost, and which allele becomes fixed (a or b). In the examples showed on Fig. 2, polymorphism was lost for m Z0.22 when allele a and b are co-dominant (h ¼0.5) (Fig. 2C), as compared to m Z0.26 when allele a was either recessive (h ¼0) or dominant (h¼ 1) (Fig. 2A and D respectively). Similarly, Fig. 3 highlighted that polymorphism started to be lost in some simulations for lower migration rates in intermediate dominance conditions as compared to complete dominance. For instance when h¼0.05, polymorphism was lost in some simulations when m 40.08 whereas strictly recessive alleles were lost only when m 40.25. This suggests that complete dominance allowed polymorphism to remain stable at higher rates of migration compared to intermediate dominance. The same trend was observed for spatial heterogeneity (s) with polymorphism observed at lower level of heterogeneity when alleles exhibited complete dominance as compared to co-dominance (see supplementary Figure 1). Altogether, this suggested that complete dominance allowed the persistence of polymorphism with lower levels of ecological and/ or population structure. 3.4. Influence of initial conditions and dominance on allele fixation For high rates of migration, the fixation of the same allele in both populations was observed for all dominance coefficients. Here, because of a perfectly symmetrical situation (opposing but identically scaled mimicry selection in the two populations), the identity of the allele becoming fixed depended mainly on the direction of dominance. In the case of co-dominance (Fig. 2C), roughly half of all simulations showed the fixation of allele a whereas the other half fixed allele b. There, the identity of the allele dominating at high migration rates was only determined by the initial frequencies of the alleles. In contrast, when dominance was complete, the dominant allele became fixed in the large majority of simulations where migration was high (allele b when h ¼0 and allele a when h¼1 respectively, see Fig. 2A and D). For migration rate m Z0.26, the strictly dominant allele reached a frequency close to 1 in 99% 106 V. Llaurens et al. / Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 Fig. 2. Simulations for recessive (h ¼ 0) (Fig. 2A), mildly recessive (h ¼ 0.005) (Fig. 2B), co-dominant (h ¼ 0.5) (Fig. 2C) and dominant (h ¼ 1) allele a (Fig. 2D), for a range of a realistic migration rates [0.01; 0.4] with an increment of 0.01. Each parameter set was replicated 1000 times. Grey: simulations where polymorphism was maintained, i.e. both alleles had a frequency Z 0.01. White: simulations where allele a was fixed, i.e. the frequency of a overall both populations was Z 0.99. Black: simulations where allele b was fixed, i.e. the frequency of allele a overall both populations was r 0.01. In all simulations, the shape of the generalization was chosen to be steep (γ ¼ 0.01), the global death risk was d ¼ 0.5, the predation risk linked to the different abundance of the model species in the two populations was s ¼ 0.9, toxicity was l ¼ 0.0025, growth rate in each population was r ¼ 1 and carrying capacity in each population was K ¼ 1000. Fig. 3. Frequency of allele a in population 1 (a) and 2 (b) most commonly observed out of 1000 simulations for a realistic range of migration rates [0.01; 0.3]. To simplify the visualisation, frequencies were plotted only in cases of polymorphism. The different lines represent values of the dominance coefficient h: red line: h ¼ 0 (the allele a is strictly recessive), Orange: h ¼ 0.005, Purple: h ¼ 0.01, Green: h ¼ 0.5 (the allele a is exactly co-dominant), Blue: h ¼ 1 (the allele a is strictly dominant). Note that the lines are interrupted as soon as polymorphism was lost. The shape of the generalization was chosen to be steep (γ ¼ 0.01), the global death risk was d ¼ 0.5, the predation risk linked to the different abundance of the model species in the two populations was s ¼ 0.9, toxicity was l ¼ 0.0025, growth rate in each population was r ¼ 1 and carrying capacity in each population was K ¼ 1000. of cases. Dominance thus had an important influence on the fate of the alleles. In case of partially recessive alleles (as for instance when h¼ 0.005), the most dominant allele became fixed in the majority of simulations although the fixation of the allele was also influenced to the initial conditions (Fig. 2B). This effect illustrated the interaction between an initial allelic density advantage and the phenotypic density advantage provided by dominance. This interaction would be modified in case of asymmetrical selection, e.g. when the total densities of the different mimicry communities are not equal. 3.5. Allele frequencies in polymorphic populations Fig. 3 showed that the interaction between migration and dominance was shaping the allele frequency in polymorphic populations. Allele frequencies were shown to depend closely on V. Llaurens et al. / Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 Fig. 4. Frequency of heterozygotes ab for a realistic range of migration rates, m : [0.01; 0.3], averaged across both populations. The different lines represent shapes of the generalisation function (γ): red line: γ ¼ 0.1, Orange: γ ¼ 0.3, Purple: γ ¼ 0.5, Green: γ ¼ 0.7, Blue: γ ¼ 0.9. Note that the lines are interrupted as soon as polymorphism is lost. The alleles were strictly co-dominant (h ¼ 0.5), the global death risk was d ¼ 0.5, the predation risk linked to the different abundance of the model species in the two populations was s ¼ 0.9, toxicity was l ¼ 0.0025, growth rate in each population was r ¼ 1 and carrying capacity in each population was K ¼ 1000. the level of dominance, with recessive alleles (0 r ho0.5) exhibiting higher frequencies than dominant alleles, resulting in a total frequency greater than 50%. This negative relationship between dominance and frequency, leading to a higher frequency for more recessive alleles held for wider shapes of the generalization function (see supplementary Figure 2). However, for very wide generalisation function (γ Z0.3), the generalisation curves, g (h) and g (1 h), overlapped (see Fig. 1) and intermediate heterozygotes ab (hE0.5) were perceived by predators as similar to both model species (and both homozygotes). Fig. 4 showed that the frequency of the intermediate heterozygote genotype ab increased with the width of the generalisation function, γ, leading to the persistence of the polymorphism in both populations, even for high migration rates. This overdominance effect depended on the discrimination capacities of predators with respect to the variable aposematic signal. 3.6. Migration load It was notable that, for all dominance levels, the total number of individuals overall both populations decreased when migration rate increased (for more details see supplementary Figure 3). This could be explained by a migration load which limits local adaptation (Lenormand, 2002). When there is no migration, all individuals in each population exhibit the matching phenotype of the local mimicry community, but as soon as migration increases, individuals are exchanged between populations, introducing nonmatching phenotypes which suffer higher predation and cause a decrease in the total number of individuals overall both populations. More interestingly, our diploid model showed that this decrease in population size depended on dominance: when h was close to 0.5 (co-dominance), the decrease was larger, presumably because heterozygotes did not match the mimetic pattern in any of the two patches, and suffered more predation in both populations. In the case of high dominance, heterozygotes matched the mimetic pattern of one of the two mimetic communities, leading to a higher total population size. Dominance thus also influenced the demography of both populations. 3.7. Influence of toxicity (l) and spatial heterogeneity (s) As previously described in the haploid model (Joron and Iwasa, 2005), the level of toxicity (l) and spatial heterogeneity (s) are key 107 parameters for local adaptation to spatially distributed mimicry communities. Briefly, under a haploid model polymorphism could be maintained when spatial heterogeneity was high due to the importance of local selection. Similarly, low values of toxicity were shown to favour polymorphism, because mildly defended prey gain high benefits from resemblance to the local mimicry community whereas the dynamics of highly defended prey are less sensitive to local mimicry. Here, we investigated more precisely the roles of parameters l and s in the interaction between dominance and migration (Fig. 5). Overall, our diploid model confirms the positive effect of spatial heterogeneity on the persistence of polymorphism whatever the dominance coefficient; it also highlights that for mildly defended preys, complete dominance tends to promote polymorphism. A particular case emerged for co-dominant alleles where polymorphism could persist when toxicity was very high (grey areas in bottom panels of Fig. 5). When h Z0.1, heterozygotes ab resembled neither homozygotes (and neither corresponding mimetic communities), but were produced in higher proportions than homozygotes due to Mendelian segregation. Since prey were highly defended, positive frequency-dependent selection was thus mainly influenced by the phenotypic composition of the focal species populations (as opposed to the local mimicry community). Since the commonest phenotype was carried by heterozygotes ab, a strong positive selection on heterozygotes promoted polymorphism. This overdominance effect explained the level of polymorphism observed at high values of toxicity when heterozygotes were distinct from either homozygote. Finally, for very high levels of toxicity, polymorphism was observed regardless of the spatial heterogeneity or dominance (see bottom of each graph on Fig. 5). In these cases, toxicity was so high that all individuals became protected irrespective of their aposematic pattern. 4. Discussion 4.1. Persistence of polymorphism As already demonstrated in the haploid model (Joron and Iwasa, 2005), the persistence of polymorphism was mainly driven by a balance between migration (here represented by migration rate, m) and selection (here represented by the overall predation risk, d, and spatial heterogeneity, s). However, our diploid model showed that complete dominance favoured the persistence of polymorphism for a larger range of parameters (m, d, s) than codominance. This is in accordance with the general model of Otto and Bourguet (Otto and Bourguet, 1999) investigating dominance in patchy environments, which shows that stable polymorphism is maintained for small values of h, i.e. in cases of complete dominance. In the case of Müllerian mimics, the generally observed positive effect of dominance on polymorphism maintenance was due to the higher levels of predation experienced by co-dominant alleles in both populations, because of their lack of resemblance to any mimicry community. In the case of partially dominant or co-dominant alleles, homozygotes had an important advantage over heterozygotes in both populations and selection against non-mimetic heterozygotes might thus favour the fixation of one of the two alleles, leading to the loss of polymorphism. The positive effect of dominance on the persistence of polymorphism in Müllerian mimics is supported by empirical data: for instance the toxic mimic butterfly Heliconius numata exhibits high local polymorphism and complete dominance among sympatric alleles controlling the wing-patterns forms (Joron et al., 2006). However, to demonstrate the crucial role of dominance on polymorphism, empirical estimations of the level of dominance and 108 V. Llaurens et al. / Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 Fig. 5. Conditions for Mullerian polymorphism maintenance as a function of the migration rate (m) (columns, from left to right: m ¼ 0.001, 0.005, 0.01, 0.1), dominance coefficient (h) (rows, from top to bottom lines: h ¼ 0, 0.005, 0.01, 0.1, 0.5), habitat heterogeneity (s) and unpalatibility (l) (note the logarithmic scale on the y-axis). In dark, polymorphism is maintained in both populations, in grey, polymorphism is also maintained in both populations with a higher frequency of heterozygotes compared to homozygotes (overdominance), in white polymorphism is lost. V. Llaurens et al. / Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 the level of colour pattern polymorphism in natural populations of Müllerian mimetic species would be required. Co-dominance also leads to the persistence of polymorphism in two special cases: (1) when the discrimination capacities of predators were not accurate, providing an advantage to intermediate heterozygotes through their resemblance to both mimetic communities, providing a multi-model advantage (Edmunds, 2000), and (2) when the focal species was highly toxic and thus protected independently of the mimicry community, favouring heterozygotes through simple density-dependent advantage. In such cases, the advantage of heterozygotes (overdominance) provides a balancing selective pressure, allowing the persistence of polymorphism in natural populations. Dominance thus appears to magnify the effect of heterogeneous selection and thus to modulate the dynamics of the migration/selection balance. However, the influence of dominance depends closely on the mechanism of selection acting on the functional locus and more precisely on the heterozygotes in both populations. For instance, behavioural studies have demonstrated that the physiological state of predators can affect their discrimination vs. generalization capacities (Barnett et al., 2012; Kokko et al., 2003; Halpin et al., 2012), and may thus limit selection on dominance. Generalization behaviour has also been shown to depend on the diversity of communities (Ihalainen et al., 2012), stressing the need to investigate the discrimination capacities of predators in natural populations of polymorphic Müllerian mimic species to characterize the selection on dominance more precisely. 109 environment, because it allows locally non-mimetic alleles to persist. Depending on the ecological context (i.e. the composition of the butterfly community, migration behaviour and relative toxicity), recessive alleles can thus reach high frequencies in mimetic species and persist over long evolutionary timescales. The capacity for invasion and persistence of migrant or new alleles with varying dominance levels thus needs to be investigated to distinguish the effects of population history, ecology and subdivision. For high migration rates, when predators had accurate discrimination capacities, selection led to the fixation of the dominant allele in both populations, assuming symmetrical spatial selection on homozygous phenotypes. In this situation, since the two model species provided the same advantage, the advantage provided to the most dominant allele was given by a positive numberdependent advantage because both homozygote and heterozygote display the same phenotype. This is equivalent to the situation where two distinct mimicry communities confer unequal advantages and lead to the fixation of the morph mimicking the more numerous community (Joron and Iwasa, 2005). This asymmetrical advantage due to dominance has previously been described by Mallet and Barton (Mallet and Barton, 1989): they showed that the movement of clines between different alleles of genes involved in mimicry is enhanced by dominance because the side of the cline where dominant alleles are most common is more likely to expand. Dominance is thus an important factor to consider to understand the evolution of wing colour patterns in mimic species. 4.2. Allele dynamics 4.3. Evolution of dominance In our model without migration, each allele became fixed in the population where it was locally adaptive, and dominance had no influence on polymorphism stability. However, as soon as migration was introduced, our model showed the key role of dominance in equilibrium allele frequencies. For moderate migration rates (lower than 0.2 in the example presented here), the equilibrium frequency was higher for the recessive allele than for the dominant allele. Since this corresponds to a two-population situation with strictly symmetrical selection acting on each homozygote, this might be similar to the situation found in parapatric mimetic communities where the relative abundance of mimicry communities could be similar on either sides of a suture zone. Empirical tests comparing allele frequencies with respect to dominance in localities where the abundances of the different mimic communities are similar would allow confirming this prediction of the model. Our diploid model also highlights that dominance can have complex interactions with overall prey community structure and with the toxicity of a species relative to other species in a mimicry community and may therefore modify the dynamics of polymorphism. In particular, relative toxicity with respect to other species of the mimetic community can lead to heterozygote advantage, and thus explain the polymorphism observed in several Müllerian mimic species. Finally, our model also predicted that the spatial distribution of the distinct mimicry communities can drive the direction of dominance. The traditional view of Haldane's sieve predicts that new establishing alleles are more likely to be dominant over ancestral alleles (Clarke et al., 1985), so that dominance could reflect the chronology of the evolution of the different alleles. In the case of balancing selection in subdivided populations, genetic drift is more likely to eliminate unexpressed advantageous alleles than dominant ones (Schierup et al., 1997), thus favouring the invasion of dominant alleles in a population with homogeneous selection. However, our model shows that recessivity can be advantageous in case of mimicry in a spatially heterogeneous Our model suggested that larger population size was observed in cases of complete dominance. This suggests that complete dominance may limit the mortality of intermediate non-mimetic heterozygotes. Complete dominance can either be due to (1) positive selection of dominant new or migrant alleles only (i.e. Haldane's sieve) or (2) evolution of expression levels towards strict dominance among alleles. This last possibility of evolution of dominance through natural selection, described by Fisher (Fisher, 1928), is still a debated topic. However, Billiard and Castric (Billiard and Castric, 2011) recently suggested that dominance is likely to evolve in many “special cases”, including loci under balancing selection. Indeed, in cases of balanced polymorphism, the frequency of heterozygotes is high enough to allow the evolution of dominance through natural selection (Otto and Bourguet, 1999). The possibility of the evolution of dominance in wing colour pattern genes has been suggested by Clarke and Sheppard (Clarke and Sheppard, 1960) based on the study of dominance in controlled crosses of sympatric versus allopatric morphs in the mimetic butterfly Papilio dardanus. In our diploid model, we highlighted the importance of dominance on adaptation of mimic alleles, suggesting that complete dominance would be a cost-effective way to express only mimetic phenotypes. Here, we assumed that each allele has a fixed dominance level, corresponding to a situation where dominance is an intrinsic property of the allele or is encoded by a gene tightly linked to the locus encoding colour pattern itself. This situation can arise in a supergene architecture where recombination is low as is observed in Heliconius numata (Joron et al., 2011). However, it has also been shown that the level of expression of major genes encoding wing colour pattern might be influenced by modifier genes which are not necessarily linked to these loci of major effect. For instance, improvement of mimicry has been suggested to rely on modifier genes with relatively small effect on wing colour pattern, as demonstrated in Heliconius numata (Jones et al., 2012) and Heliconius erato (Papa et al., 2013). These data raise the 110 V. Llaurens et al. / Journal of Theoretical Biology 337 (2013) 101–110 possibility that dominance at major genes could be modified by independent loci, allowing the rapid adaptation of heterozygotes to the local community of toxic butterflies. The influence of recombination between dominance modifiers and loci controlling aposematic colour pattern should be investigated in the future and could shed light on the mechanisms of the evolution of dominance. 5. Conclusions Our theoretical diploid model suggests that dominance can have an important impact on the dynamics of alleles responsible for aposematic pattern in mimetic species and, specifically, on the persistence of polymorphism. In polymorphic mimetic species, dominance interacts with the structure of the community of defended prey, migration capacities and demography, and thus influences the adaptation of aposematic patterns. Our results stress the importance of investigating the evolution of dominance in adaptive traits in heterogeneous environments. Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank A. Whibley for useful discussions on the manuscript. This work has been sponsored by the ATM Formes possibles, formes realisées from the National Museum of Natural History to VL, by the ERC starting grant MIMEVOL to MJ and the ANR BRASSIDOM to SB. Appendix A. Supporting information Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the online version at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2013.08.006. References Barnett, C.A., Skelhorn, J., Bateson, M., Rowe, C., 2012. Educated predators make strategic decisions to eat defended prey according to their toxin content. Behavioral Ecology 23, 418–424. Billiard, S., Castric, V., 2011. Evidence for Fisher's dominance theory: how many “special cases”? Trends in Genetics 27 (11), 441–445. Charlesworth, D., 2006. Balancing selection and its effects on sequences in nearby genome regions. PLOS Genetics 2, 375–384. Chouteau, M., Angers, B., 2011. The role of predators in maintaining the geographic organization of aposematic signals. American Naturalist 178, 810–817. Chouteau, M., Summers, K., Morales, V., Angers, B., 2011. Advergence in Mullerian mimicry: the case of the poison dart frogs of Northern Peru revisited. Biology Letters 7, 796–800. Clarke, C., Clarke, F.M.M., Collins, S.C., Gill, A.C.L., Turner, J.R.G., 1985. Male-like females, mimicry and transvestism in butterflies (Lepidoptera, Papilionidae). Systematic Entomology 10, 257–283. Clarke, C.A., Sheppard, P.M., 1960. The evolution of dominance under disruptive selection. Heredity 14, 73–87. Dumbacher, J.P., Fleischer, R.C., 2001. Phylogenetic evidence for colour pattern convergence in toxic pitohuis: Mullerian mimicry in birds? Proceedings of the Royal Society B 268, 1971–1976. Edmunds, M., 2000. Why are there good and poor mimics? Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 70, 459–466. Fisher, R.A., 1928. The possible modification of the response of the wild type to recurrent mutations. American Naturalist 62, 115–126. Haldane, J.B.S., 1927. A mathematical theory of natural and artificial selection, Part V: Selection and mutation. Proceedings of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 23, 838–844. Halpin, C.G., Skelton, J., Rowe, C., 2012. The relationship between sympatric defended species depends upon predators discriminatiry behaviour. PLOS One 7, e44895. Ihalainen, E., Rowland, H.M., Speed, M.P., Ruxton, G.D., Mappes, J., 2012. Prey community structure affects how predators select for Mullerian mimicry. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 279, 2099–2105. Jones, R.T., Salazar, P.A., Ffrench-Constant, R.H., Jiggins, C.D., Joron, M., 2012. Evolution of a mimicry supergene from a multilocus architecture. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 279, 316–325. Joron, M., Iwasa, Y., 2005. The evolution of a Mullerian mimic in a spatially distributed community. J. Theor. Biol 237, 87–103. Joron, M., Wynne, I.R., Lamas, G., Mallet, J., 1999. Variable selection and the coexistence of multiple mimetic forms of the butterfly Heliconius numata. Evol. Ecol. 13, 721–754. Joron, M., et al., 2006. A conserved supergene locus controls colour pattern diversity in Heliconius butterflies. Plos Biology 4, 1831–1840. Joron, M., et al., 2011. Chromosomal rearrangements maintain a polymorphic supergene controlling butterfly mimicry. Nature 477, 203–206. Kokko, H., Mappes, J., Lindström, L., 2003. Alternative prey can change modelmimic dynamics between parasitism and mutualism. Ecol. Lett. 6. (2068-1076). Lenormand, T., 2002. Gene flow and the limits to natural selection. Trends Ecol. Evol. 17, 183–189. Mallet, J., Barton, N., 1989. Inference from clines stabilized by frequency-dependent selection. Genetics 122, 967–976. Mappes, J., Marples, N., Endler, J.A., 2005. The complex business of survival by aposematism. Trends in Ecol. Evol. 20, 598–603. Marek, P.E., Bond, J.E., 2009. A Mullerian mimicry ring in Appalachian millipedes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 9755–9760. Mendel, G., 1895. In: Sherwood, C.S.a.E.R. (Ed.), The origin of genetics: a Mendel source book. W. H. Freeman, San Francisco. Merrill, R.M., et al., 2012. Disruptive ecological selection on a mating cue. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 279, 4907–4913. Naisbit, R.E., Jiggins, C.D., Mallet, J., 2003. Mimicry: developmental genes that contribute to speciation. Evol. Dev. 5, 269–280. Nijhout, F.H., 1991. The development and evolution of butterfly wing patterns. Smithsonian series in comparative evolutionary biology. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington and London. Nijhout, F.H., 2003. Polymorphic mimicry in Papilio dardanus: mosaic dominance, big effect and origins. Evol. Dev. 5, 579–592. Orr, H.A., 1991. A test of Fisher theory of dominance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 88, 11413–11415. Orr, H.A., Betancourt, A.J., 2001. Haldane's sieve and adaptation from the standing genetic variation. Genetics 157, 875–884. Otto, S.P., Bourguet, D., 1999. Balanced polymorphisms and the evolution of dominance. American Naturalist 153, 561–574. Otto, S.P., Day, T., 2007. in A biologist's guide to mathematical modeling in ecology and evolution. Princeton University Press. Papa, R., et al., 2013. Multi-Allelic Major Effect Genes Interact with Minor Effect QTLs to Control Adaptive Colour Pattern Variation in Heliconius erato. Plos One 8. Pinheiro, C.E.G., 2003. Does Mullerian mimicry work in nature? Experiments with butterflies and birds (Tyrannidae). Biotropica 35, 356–364. Ruxton, G.D., Sherratt, T.N., Speed, M.P. (Eds.), 2004. Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Aposematism, and Mimicry. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Ruxton, G.D., Franks, D.W., Balogh, A.C.V., Leimar, O., 2008. Evolutionary implications of the form of predator generalisation for aposematic signals and mimicry in prey. Evolution 62, 2913–2921. Sanders, K.L., Malhotra, A., Thorpe, R.S., 2006. Evidence for a Mullerian mimetic radiation in Asian pitvipers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 273, 1135–1141. Schierup, M.H., Vekemans, X., Christiansen, F.B., 1997. Evolutionary dynamics of sporophytic self-incompatibility alleles in plants. Genetics 147, 835–846. Speed, M.P., Turner, J.R.G., 1999. Learning and memory in mimicry: II. Do we understand the mimicry spectrum? Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 67, 281–312. Spichtig, M., Kawecki, T.J., 2004. The maintenance (or not) of polygenic variation by soft selection in heterogeneous environments. American Naturalist 164, 70–84. Symula, R., Schulte, R., Summers, K., 2001. Molecular phylogenetic evidence for a mimetic radiation in Peruvian poison frogs supports a Mullerian mimicry hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 268, 2415–2421. Yeaman, S., Otto, S.P., 2011. Establishment and maintenance of adaptive genetic divergence under migration, selection, and drift. Evolution 65, 2123–2129. Zrzavy, J., Nedved, O., 1999. Evolution of mimicry in the New World Dysdercus (Hemiptera: Pyrrhocoridae). Journal of Evolutionary Biology 12, 956–969.