JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL CHILD 33, 207-215 (1982) PSYCHOLOGY Developmental Differences in the Role of Detail in Picture Recognition Memory KATHY Claremont PEZDEK Graduate School AND HSUAN-CHIH University CHEN of Kansas Two experiments assess the effect of the amount of physical detail in pictures on picture recognition memory. Children and adults were presented simple and complex line drawings. A “same-different” recognition test followed in which the distractor items were original pictures from the presentation phase with the amount of physical detail altered. For second- and fourth-grade subjects, recognition sensitivity, measured in terms of d’, was similar for pictures in the simple and complex presentation conditions. For adults, however, recognition sensitivity was greater for pictures in the simple than complex presentation condition. This finding with adults was replicated in Experiment 2. Interpretations of this age difference in picture memory processing are discussed, as well as the constraints imposed by various dependent measures used in picture memory studies. It is now well accepted that individuals have an impressive ability to recognize (Nickerson, 1965; Standing, Conezio, & Haber, 1970) and recall (Bousfield, Esterson, & Whitmarsh, 1957) pictorial material. A number of developmental studies have reported that recognition of large numbers of pictures (Hoffman & Dick, 1976) and recall and recognition of visual objects (Dirks & Neisser, 1977; Mandler, Seegmiller, & Day, 1977) increase with age. The interesting developmental question here is the extent to which the younger and older subjects differ quantitatively We acknowledge the assistance of Joseph Muga, Nancy Reed, and Robert Bennett in collecting the data, and thank Gary W. Evans, Jean Mandler, and Ed Teyber for reading earlier drafts and offering perceptive comments. We are also grateful for the cooperation of the staff at Preston Elementary School in Rialto, California. Requests for reprints should be sent to Kathy Pezdek, Psychology Department, Claremont Graduate School, Claremont, CA 91711. 207 0022~0965/82/020207-09$02.00/O Copyright 0 1982 by Academic Press, Inc. All rights of reproduction in any form resewed. 208 PEZDEKANDCHEN in picture memory capacity, or qualitatively because they utilize different memory processing strategies (cf. Myers & Perlmutter, 1978). This issue is addressed in the present study by examining age differences in picture memory as a function of the amount of detail in the pictures. Several researchers have reported that memory for pictures is related to the amount of physical detail in the pictures. Examining age differences in picture memory as a function of the amount of detail in the pictures is thus one way of isolating the basis of age differences in picture memory. A second reason for varying the amount of detail in a picture to explore age differences in picture memory stems from suggestions by Elkind (1975) and Piaget (1969). They have reported that young children are more likely than older children and adults to “perceptually center” on the dominant aspect of a perceptual field, at the expense of processing peripheral information. Similarly, Mackworth and Bruner (1970) found that 6-year-old children scan pictures less systematically than adults and are less likely to utilize long eye-tracks that link together important areas of the pictures. These studies would suggest that memory of younger children might be less affected than adults by the amount of noncentral detail in a picture. This would perhaps account for the lower recognition memory performance by younger subjects. The research on the effect of the amount of detail in pictures on picture memory has produced mixed results. Although Reese (1970) hypothesized that the retention of visual stimuli is positively related to the amount of detail in the stimuli, the available data are less convincing. Support for Reese’s hypothesis comes from studies that have utilized recall measures (Bevan & Steger, 1971; Evertson & Wicker, 1974). Recognition measures, on the other hand, do not appear to be affected by the amount of detail presented in pictures. Nelson, Metzler, and Reed (1974) used a two-alternative forced choice test with adults and reported no difference in recognition among items presented as black-and-white photographs, complex, embellished line drawings, or simple, unembellished line drawings. Emmerick and Ackerman (1976) tested children in a paired-associate test and reported similar memory performance for items presented as simple line drawings and more detailed line drawings. In the present study, subjects from three age groups were presented a series of pictures, half of which were chosen from the complex, embellished line drawings used by Nelson et al. (1974) and half were simple, unembellished line drawings from the same study. At the time of test, recognition pictures were presented one at a time in a “same-different” test. Half of the simple and complex line drawings were tested in the “same” form that they had been presented in. The other half of the test pictures; that is, pictures previously presented items were “changed” in a simple form were tested with complex versions of the same pictures, and pictures previously presented in a complex form were tested with simple versions of the same pictures. The simple and complex versions PICTURERECOGNITION MEMORY 209 of each picture contained the same central information, but extra peripheral details, shading, and embellishment were added in the complex pictures. The present study thus scrutinizes age differences in picture memory by comparing recognition memory for pictures as a function of the amount of detail in the pictures. EXPERIMENT 1 Method Subjects and design. Subjects were selected from three age groups. Twenty second graders and 20 fourth graders participated from Preston Elementary School, Rialto, California. The adult subjects were 20 undergraduates who volunteered from classes at California State College, San Bernardino. Ten males and 10 females participated in each age group. All subjects viewed simple and complex line drawings in the presentation phase, and were tested with “same” and “changed” test items. The study thus can be described as a 3 x 2 x 2 factorial design with age as a between-subjects factor and presentation form and test form as within-subjects factors. Materials. Eighty-eight pictures were selected from the set of materials used by Nelson et al. (1974). Each of 44 basic pictures had been drawn in both a simple, unembellished line drawing form, and a complex, embellished line drawing form, for a total of 88 pictures. All drawings were black and white. The simple and complex forms of each picture contained the same central information, with extra shading and fine-grain details added to both the principal figure and the background in the complex pictures. The use of the materials by Nelson et al. (1974) strengthens this study by allowing a comparison of results with other studies without the confounding effect of different materials. The use of this standard set of materials is particularly important in this study because it is so difficult to quantify “amount of detail” in pictures. In this way, extraneous variables involving the stimulus materials are held constant in order to focus on the factors of interest in this study. Examples of stimulus pictures are shown in Fig. 1. Procedure. Subjects participated individually. They were presented a sequence of slides including 44 presentation pictures, followed by a 3-min delay task, and then 44 test pictures. The presentation pictures included 22 simple line drawings and 22 complex line drawings. In the presentation phase, subjects were instructed to study each picture carefully, as it would be important in a later part of the experiment. The pictures were presented on slides by a Kodak Carousel slide projector with a shutter attachment regulated by a msec timer. During the presentation phase, slides were presented for 8 set each. The test sequence consisted of 22 “same” pictures from the presentation phase-l 1 simple and 11 complex. The remaining 22 test pictures were “changed” versions of the presentation pictures. The 11 “changed” 210 PEZDEK SIMPLE AND CHEN COMPLEX r FIG. 1. Examples of pictures in both simple and complex form. versions of the simple pictures from the presentation phase were the complex versions of these pictures. The 11 “changed” versions of the complex presentation pictures were the simple versions of these pictures. Thus, each of the 44 presentation pictures was included once in the test phase, in either the “same” or “changed” form. In the test phase, subjects viewed pictures one at a time at a rate controlled by the experimenter. For each picture the experimenter asked, “Is this picture the same as a picture you saw before, or are there some changes in this picture?” Several practice slides were shown first to ensure that the subjects understood what types of changes constituted “changed” test items. The sequence of presentation and test slides was arranged in two orders. Half of the subjects in each age group were randomly assigned to each order. The intervening delay task was included between presentation and test to insure that the test that followed measured long-term memory. In the delay task subjects circled all of the number two’s on a random number sheet for 3 min. Results The data were scored in terms of the mean percentage correct for subjects in each age group, as a function of the condition of presentation and the condition of test. These data appear at the top of Table 1. As can be seen in Table 1, the pattern of results was very different for “same” and “changed” test items. This suggests that response bias as PICTURE RECOGNITION TABLE MEAN PERCENTAGE 211 MEMORY 1 CORRECTAND d’ VALUES IN EACH EXPERIMENTAL EXPERIMENT CONDITION IN 1 Percentage Correct Presentation condition: Test condition: Second graders Fourth graders Adults Simple “Same” Complex “Changed” 67.9 82.0 71.0 64.0 62.0 83.8 “Same” 73.4 83.8 68.7 “Changed” 58.5 59.2 66.5 d' Presentation condition: Second graders Fourth graders Adults Simple Complex 1.04 1.44 2.03 1.07 1.52 1.18 well as recognition sensitivity affected the percent accuracy data. The data were thus converted to the signal detection measure of d’. (See Banks, 1970, for an explanation of Signal Detection Theory.) The procedure suggested by Hochhaus (1972) was followed for calculating d’ values. The mean d’ values for each age group in each experimental condition are presented in the bottom portion of Table 1. A 3 (age) x 2 (presentation form) analysis of variance was performed on the d’ data. The only significant effect was the age x presentation form interaction, F(2, 57) = 4.75, MS, = S73, p -=c.OS. As can be seen in Table 1, the second and fourth graders were similarly sensitive to pictures in the simple and complex presentation conditions. The adults, however, were more sensitive to pictures in the simple than complex presentation condition. The result that adults were more accurate recognizing pictures in the simple than complex presentation condition is inconsistent with previous findings, especially those of Nelson et al. (1974). There are several procedural differences between the present study and previous studies that have reported different results. However, before considering these points, it was deemed necessary to replicate the finding with adults. Experiment 2 was thus conducted to test the replicability of the adult data in Experiment 1. EXPERIMENT 2 Method Subjects. Thirty-six adults volunteered to participate from classes at California State College, San Bernardino. Sex of subject was not specifically controlled for. 212 PEZDEK AND CHEN Materials, procedure, and design. The materials and procedure were the same as those used in Experiment 1. The design was a 2 (simple and complex presentation condition) x 2 (“same” and “changed” test condition) within-subjects design. To test the generalizability of the effect from Experiment 1, in Experiment 2 the assignment of presentation condition and test condition to each of the 44 pictures, and the order of presenting the items was generated using a different random order than was used in the first experiment. All other procedures were the same in the two experiments. Results The mean percentage correct data were calculated across subjects as a function of the condition of presentation and the condition of test. These results appear in Table 2. The more important results, however, for comparison with Experiment 1 are the d’ findings. As can be seen in Table 2, these adult subjects were more accurate recognizing pictures in the simple presentation condition (d’ = 3.07) than in the complex presentation condition (d’ = 1.92), t(35) = 4.98, p < .Ol. The direction of this effect clearly replicates the adult data in Experiment 1. DISCUSSION The direction of the significant age x presentation form interaction suggests that the children and the adults in the present study processed pictures in memory very differently, despite the absence of a significant main effect of age on recognition sensitivity. The result that second and fourth graders were similarly sensitive recognizing items presented as simple and complex pictures, is consistent with recognition data of Emmerick and Ackerman (1976) with 4- and Syear-old children. However, these results contradict hypotheses by Reese and others who have sugTABLE MEAN PERCENTAGE 2 CORRECT AND d’ VALUES IN EACH EXPERIMENTAL EXPERIMENT 2 CONDITION IN Percentage Correct Presentation condition: Test condition: Simple “Same” Complex “Changed” 88.8 87.8 d’ Presentation condition: Simple Complex 3.07 1.92 “Same” 81.8 “Changed” 74.2 PICTURE RECOGNITION MEMORY 213 gested that the retention of visual stimuli is positively related to the amount of detail in the stimuli. Nelson et al. (1974) have offered one explanation that handles the similar recognition sensitivity for simple and complex presentation pictures by the children in the present study. They suggested that a conservation of processing might be operating at the time of encoding information from pictures. That is, in a fixed amount of processing time, a constant amount of information is encoded, regardless of the amount of information in a picture. Thus, subjects would encode many details in a complex picture, but relatively less information about each detail. In a simple picture, subjects would encode fewer details, but more information about each detail. As a result, the total amount of information stored would be the same for simple and complex pictures, and recognition accuracy would not be affected by the extra detail in the complex pictures. Contrary to the results with children, adults in both Experiments 1 and 2 were more sensitive recognizing pictures in the simple than complex presentation conditions. As can be seen in the top portion of Table 1 and Table 2, the higher d’ value in the simple presentation condition by adults, can be accounted for by the higher recognition accuracy for changed simple presentation items as compared with changed complex presentation items. At the time of test, the additional detail in changed simple pictures was apparently more salient to adults than the deleted detail in changed complex pictures. Another interpretation of these results is that adults were more likely than children to thematically encode the pictures. Such thematic encodings would be expected to retain relevant details but omit irrelevant details. The greater recognition sensitivity to simple than complex pictures by adults fits this interpretation. The adult data is inconsistent with previous relevant findings, most notably Nelson et al. (1974). Although the stimulus pictures in the present study were drawn from the pool of items used by Nelson et al., there are two important procedural differences between the two studies. First, the distractor test pictures in the present study were changed versions of original pictures. Nelson et al. utilized completely new pictures as distractor items. Second, Nelson et al. utilized a two-alternative forced choice recognition test, whereas a “same-different” recognition test was used in the present study. Thus, the present study included a more difficult task and tested memory for the specific details in each picture, whereas Nelson et al. tested general familiarity with each picture. The absence of an increase in picture recognition accuracy with age in the present study is not consistent with previous findings (cf. Hoffman & Dick, 1976). The “same-different” recognition task in the present study specifically tests memory for the detail retained in each picture rather than simply testing general familiarity with the pictures. Picture 214 PEZDEK AND CHEN memory studies that have used recall tests or forced choice recognition tests with completely new distractor test items test general rather than specific picture memory. Whereas general picture memory accuracy has been reported to improve with age, the present study reports that this is not the case when memory for specific details in pictures is tested. One shortcoming of the present study occurred as a result of using the existing sets of simple and complex pictures of Nelson et al. (1974). The extra detail in the complex version of each picture had been added with the restriction that it be irrelevant to the central theme of the picture. However, in some pictures the extra detail was added primarily to the figure and in some pictures the extra detail was added primarily to the background context. An additional experiment would be necessary to examine the relative effects of relevant/irrelevant detail as compared with figure/context detail. The results of this study suggest that although adults and children are very accurate at discriminating between pictures they have seen before and those they have not seen, they actually do not remember a large portion of the information contained in any particular visual stimulus. Tests that demonstrate that subjects are able to distinguish completely new pictures from pictures that they have seen before simply reveal that the subjects retained enough information from old pictures to differentiate them from new ones. Exactly how much of the information in a visual stimulus is retained cannot be answered from much of the previous picture memory research. The results reported here concur with the few studies that have explored how much information in a picture is retained in memory. Nickerson and Adams (1979), for example, found that adults’ memory for the visual details on a common United States penny is remarkably poor. Similarly Mandler (cf. Mandler & Johnson, 1976) reported that subjects’ recognition for specific types of descriptive and spatial transformations in complex pictures was less than 67% correct. Together, these findings suggest that picture recognition memory is not “unlimited,” as described by Haber (1970), and further, that various measures of picture memory tap very different aspects of what is retained. This study concludes that adults and second- and fourth-grade children process and remember physical information in pictures qualitatively differently. Additional research in this area is necessary before a more specific account of the processing changes that underlie these developmental differences is forthcoming. These findings suggest that not only does the role of detail in picture memory differ developmentally, but it also varies with the specific dependent measure used. REFERENCES Banks, W. P. Signal detection theory and human memory. Psychological BuMin. 74, 81-99. 1970. PICTURE RECOGNITION MEMORY 215 Bevan, W., & Steger, J. A., Science, 1971, 172, 597-599. Bousfield, W. A., Esterson, J., & Whitmarsh, G. A. The effects of concomitant colored and uncolored pictorial representations on the learning of stimulus words. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1957, 41, 165-168. Dirks, J., & Neisser, U. Memory for objects in real scenes: The development of recognition and recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1977. 23, 315-328. Elkind, D. Perceptual development in children. American Scientisfs, 1975, 63, 533-541. Emmerick, H. J., & Ackerman, B. P. The effect of pictorial detail and elaboration on children’s retention. Journnl of Experimental Child Psychology, 1976, 21, 241-248. Evertson, C. M., & Wicker, F. W. Pictorial concreteness and mode of elaboration in children’s learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1974, 17, 264-270. Haber, R. N. How we remember what we see. Scientijc American, 1970, 222, 104-112. Hochhaus, L. A table for the calculation of d’ and p. Psycho/o&z/ Bulletin, 1972, 77, 375-376. Hoffman, C. D., & Dick, S. A developmental investigation of recognition memory. Child Development, 1976, 47, 794-799. Mackworth, N. H., & Bruner, J. S. How adults and children search and recognize pictures. Human Development, 1970, 13, 149-177. Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. S. Some of the thousand words a picture is worth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Leurning and Memory, 1976, 2, 529-540. Mandler, J. M., Seegmiller, D., & Day, J. On the coding of spatial information. Memory and Cognition, 1977, 5, 10-16. Myers, N. A., & Perlmutter, M. Memory in the years from two to five. In P. A. Ornstein (Ed.), Memory development in children. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978. Nelson, T. O., Metzler, J., & Reed. D. A. Role of details in the long-term recognition of pictures and verbal descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1974. 102, 184-186. Nickerson, R. S. Short-term memory for complex meaningful visual configurations: A demonstration of capacity. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1965, 19, 155-160. Nickerson, R. S., & Adams, M. J. Long-term memory for a common object. Cognitive Psychology, 1979, 11, 287-307. Piaget, J. The mechanisms of perception, New York: Basic Books, 1969. Reese, H. W. Imagery and contextual meaning. In H. W. Reese (Chm.). Imagery in children’s learning: A symposium. Psycho/ogical Buttetin, 1970, 73, 404-414. Standing L., Conezio, J., & Haber, R. N. Perception and memory for pictures: Singletrial learning of 2500 visual stimuli. Psychonomic Science, 1970, 19, 73-74. RECEIVED: February 7, 1980; REVISED: January 13, 1981, May 5, 1981.