CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION John Bowlby, a British psychoanalyst, formulated attachment theory and in the process revolutionised thinking about the mother -infant relationship and about the importance and function of close relationships. Attachment theory has emerged as a major field of inquiry among researchers interested in human development and has generated an enormous body of research that focus on understanding the social, emotional, and interpersonal development of children. It has been valued for having the potential for prognostic power, based on findings of continuity between early quality of attachment and later socio-emotional development (Senior, 2002). Bowlby took issue with the emphasis that psychoanalysis placed on the child’s inner fantasy world, rather than on actual life events and interactions. He thus compared psychoanalysis to a single-track railway, insisting on the inevitable path toward examining early developmental phases – whereas ‘multi-track’ attachment theory allowed for a myriad web of possible tracks (Fonagy, 2001). Bowlby chose to emphasise external reality and the importance of environmental influence in psychological development, as he felt that within psychoanalysis there was a detachment from external realities (Gullestad, 2001). Unlike Bowlby and like so many psychoanalysts, Klein, for example, placed emphasis on the child’s inner fantasy world, at the expense of looking at actual life events and interactions. She saw mental structures as arising out of a variety of internal objects (phantasies about people in the infant’s life), which changes in character in unconscious phantasy as the child develops from infancy (Watts, 2002). The infant’s phantasies are modified by actual experiences of interaction with the environment (Fonagy, 1999). Klein made a fair contribution to psychoanalysis, but Bowlby did more than any other psychoanalyst to change social policy 1 and inform government thinking about the needs of children and families (Gomez, 1997). He questioned the Kleinian view that children’s emotional problems are mainly due to infantile fantasies generated from internal conflicts related to aggressive drives, rather than to environmental failure and trauma. Most importantly within the context of this thesis, he focussed our attention on the importance of intergenerational transmission of attachment relations and to the importance of maternal deprivation and separation (Gullestad, 2001). Attachment theory began with Bowlby’s attempt to understand the psychopathological effects of maternal deprivation by studying the normative course of the ontogeny of this earliest relationship. His hope was that if we better understood this normative course, we would be in an improved position to understand the effects of its disruption (Fonagy, 1999). This will then be the focus of this thesis. It seeks to explore the attachment patterns of children who have suffered from maternal deprivation, separation, attachment abuse and who were institutionalised as a result. 1.1 Motivation for the study A number of events motivated this thesis. The researcher’s interest in attachment theory was initially stimulated when she was exposed to the theory in her undergraduate studies in Psychology. Among others, the theory elucidated for the researcher, the function of close relationships and how and why the parent-child relationship plays a central role in the child’s psychological development. It further clarified for her how these relationships are formed and what impact they have on future relationships. Attachment theory moved away from the psychoanalytical emphasis on the child’s inner fantasy world by incorporating actual life events and interactions in what Bowlby considered to be the most important relationship, which is the relationship between the child and his caregiver. An additional stimulating force for the researcher was A.H. Bowley’s ‘Psychology of the unwanted child’ published in 1947. 2 This book gave an account of the adjustment struggles of children of varying ages orphaned during World War II, to the children’s homes and institutions they were placed in. It thus elucidated for the researcher, the implications of maternal deprivation on the development of the child’s relationship patterns. What further prompted the researcher’s interest in this field of study was her practical placement in a children’s home. She was in a position to view children of differing ages and cultures in the context of a children’s home - how they interacted with each other, with strangers, and how the care workers (caregivers) and staff interacted with them. These children appeared to be ‘love starved’ and uninhibited. Many would indiscriminately attached to any person that gave them attention. Some of the children at the researcher’s practical placement were placed in the children’s home merely because their parents rejected and abandoned them and they had nowhere else to go, while others came to the home as a measure of protective care against neglect and/ or abuse. According to Goldberg (2000), research done in the 1940s on children in orphanages indicated that the children were developmentally delayed and that their social and emotional behaviour was unusual. These children did not seem to form close relationships, and were instead described as inappropriately friendly towards everyone, including strangers (Goldberg, 2000). This study was done in an effort to highlight the importance of the attachment relationship and how attachment abuse and maternal deprivation, which in some instances land children in protective care (i.e. children’s homes), can be detrimental to the forming of close relationships. 1.2 Conceptual framework for the study The study is rooted in a qualitative approach and aims to describe the nature of attachment patterns in institutionalised adolescents. The emphasis will be on exploring the 3 participants’ experience of maternal deprivation, attachment abuse and institutionalisation and examining how these issues have affected their attachment patterns. Three 16-year-old adolescents were chosen to participate in the study. The central reason relates to the researcher’s interest in exploring what will happen in terms of attachment if adolescents are separated from their families and placed in a different environment, such as a children’s home. The adolescent tries to answer the question, “Who am I, and what is my place in society?” A key task of adolescence is to develop autonomy so as no longer to need to rely (as much) on parents’ support when making one’s way through the world. The growing push for autonomy from parents may create healthy pressure to begin to use peers as attachment figures, so that attachment needs can be met while establishing autonomy in the relationship with parents (Steinberg, 1990). Self-chosen values and occupational goals lead to a lasting personal identity. The negative outcome if these values and goals are not reached is confusion about future adult roles. Young people generally work on establishing intimate ties to others. Because of earlier disappointments and negative life events, some individuals cannot form close relationships and remain isolated from others. 1.2.1 The research question The study is primarily concerned with the following question: What are the attachment patterns of children who have suffered from maternal deprivation, separation, attachment abuse and who were institutionalised as a result? 1.2.2 The method The study was conducted by means of a qualitative methodology. Qualitative research is concerned with exploring everyday perceptions of the world and uncovering the experiential, subjective dimensions of people’s worlds. Inherent in qualitative research are 4 assumptions about the nature of social life, knowledge, research objectives and ways to deal with data (Neuman, 1997). A clinical interview was conducted with the three participants. In addition to the interview, two projective drawing tests and two projective story telling tests were conducted with each participant. The transcribed interviews and the stories and drawings of the projective tests constitute the units of analyses. Interview data and projective story-telling tests were analysed using the thematic content analytical process, while the projective drawing tests were analysed by means of classification systems specifically designed for such purpos es. The choice of method was motivated by its compatibility with the research question, which has as its focus, real people and their real life experiences. 1.3 The study process Chapters 2, 3 and 4 provide the theoretical framework for the study. Chapter 2 starts with a discussion of “normal” child development from infancy to adolescence. The relevance of this chapter is to create a benchmark against which we can gauge any straying from the developmental path that is considered ‘normal’ in the child deve lopment field. Attention is given to the contributions of traditional theorists/theories to the field of developmental psychology, specifically child development. In addition to this, a brief outline will be given of some of the more modern theories and the importance of the family context for development. This is followed by a detailed delineation of the developmental stage of adolescence, with specific reference to cognitive development and emotional and social development. Chapter 3 offers a broad overview of attachment theory. It starts with the definitional complexities around attachment and ultimately focuses on the contributions of John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth to the field of attachment theory. Included here is a discussion of the psychoanalytical view on attachment. This is done primarily because it serves to better 5 understand Bowlby’s attachment theory, which ultimately provided the theoretical framework on which the study is based and because Bowlby essentially developed his theory as a result of disagreements with psychoanalytical theories in general and more specifically, the Kleinian views on attachment. Attention is also given to Bowlby’s discussion on the phases of the development of attachment and attachment behaviour, as well as Ainsworth’s “Strange Situation” and types of attachment patterns that were delineated from this procedure. This is followed by a discussion of the quality of maternal responsiveness to the infant as well as Bowlby’s elucidation of the internal working model or mental representations. Attachment in the family system is also expanded on, looking specifically at attachments to fathers and siblings and how the marital relationship contributes to children’s behaviours and the attachment patterns formed. The focus then shifts to attachment in adolescence followed by an outline of traumatised and abused children and a discussion on attachment abuse. In chapter 4 various aspects of institutionalisation that are pertinent to the study are outlined, starting with a discussion of the essentials for good personality development as well as effective childcare practices. This is followed by an outline of what residential care is and what it offers to children that are placed there. Attention is given to the situations that encourage institutionalisation, namely maternal and other forms of deprivation, rejection, neglect, separation and abuse. In addition to this, the consequences of institutional rearing and factors that negatively affect the impact of institutionalisation are re flected on. Finally, the importance of parental involvement in institutional placements is highlighted. Chapter 5 entails a detailed delineation of the empirical study. Chapter 6 contains the presentation of the results, while chapter 7 entails a discussion of the results by means of contextualizing these within the broader literature. In chapter 8 the study is concluded. 6 CHAPTER 2 THE DEVELOPING CHILD 2.1 Introduction Developmental psychology is usually described as the study of human development over the entire lifespan - from conception to death (Louw, 1991). Developmental psychologists aim to identify the determinants of development and to determine the extent and nature of their influence precisely (Hook, 2002a). This enables us to explain a wide variety of psychological changes, both cognitive and social, which occur between birth and death. These various psychological changes are multifarious, and include insights into how children and adults learn, perceive, understand, recall, and process aspe cts of the world (Hook, 2002a). The focus of this research is on the developmental stage of adolescence - 16 year olds in particular. However, the first part of the chapter will give a broad overview of child development, starting at infancy. The relevance of this chapter is to create a benchmark against which we can gauge any straying from the developmental path that is considered ‘normal’ in the child development field. It will thus look at development starting at infancy and not just adolescent developme nt because the aim of the study is to explore the nature of the participants’ attachment patterns. According to Fonagy (2001), attachment theory is a developmental theory that emphasises infancy and how it influences or impacts on later development. It is therefore important to see how early experiences influence and shape the type of person the infant becomes. Child development is a field of study devoted to understanding all aspects of human growth from conception through adolescence (Berk, 1996; Louw, 1991). Social pressures to better the lives of children have also stimulated research about child development. For example, the beginning of public education in the early part of this century led to a demand 7 for knowledge about what and how to teach children of different ages (Santrock, 1999). The interest of paediatricians in improving children’s health required an understanding of physical growth and nutrition (Santrock, 2001). The social services profession’s desire to treat children’s anxieties and behaviour problems required information about personality and social development (Mussen, Conger, Kagan, & Huston, 1990). 2.2 Theories of child development Child development theories seek to explain three basic questions. • The first is whether the child is an organismic or mechanistic being? In other words, are children active beings with psychological structures that underlie and control development, or are they passive recipients of environmental inputs? In the organismic theory put forward, children are viewed as active, purposeful beings that make sense of their world and determine their own learning and the surrounding environment supports development (Berk, 1996). In contrast, the mechanistic theory focuses on relationships between environmental inputs and behavioural outputs. The mechanistic theory regards the child as a passive reactor to environmental inputs (Berk, 1996). • The second basic question is whether development is a continuous process, or whether it follows a series of discontinuous stages? In ot her words, is child development a matter of cumulative adding on of skills and behaviours, or does it involve qualitative, stage wise change? Continuous development views development as a cumulative process of adding on more of the same types of skills that was there to begin with (Hook & Cockcroft, 2002; Louw, Louw, & Schoeman, 1995). In contrast to this view, the discontinuous development theory holds that new and different ways 8 of interpreting and responding to the world emerges at particular time periods (Hook & Cockcroft, 2002; Louw et al., 1995). • The third question is whether development is primarily determined by nature or nurture, that is, whether genetic or environmental factors are the most important determinants of child development and behaviour? The nature theory of development (inborn biological givens) is the hereditary information we receive from our parents at the moment of conception that signals the body to grow and affects all our characteristics and skills (Hook & Cockcroft, 2002; Moshman, 1999). According to the nurture theory of development, children are formed and affected by the complex forces of the physical and social world they encounter in their homes, neighbourhoods, schools and communities (Hook & Cockcroft, 2002; Moshman, 1999). Though many theories have enriched the field of child development over the decades, this research will only focus on the main traditional theories of child development as well as some contemporary perspectives. The scope of this research does not allow for an exhaustive history on child development. 2.3 Traditional approaches Modern theories of child development are the result of centuries of change in Western cultural values, philosophical thinking about children, and scientific progress (Berk, 1996). These theories of child development have roots extending far back into the past. In medieval times children were thought of as miniature adults, a view called preformationism (Santrock, 2001). By the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries, childhood became a distinct phase of the life cycle. Children came to be seen as people in their own right, experiencing emotions and being influenced by what happens in society (Santrock, 2001). 9 2.3.1 The psychoanalytic perspective In the 1930s and 1940s, child guidance professionals turned to the psychoanalytic perspective for help in understanding children with emotional problems (Fonagy, 1999; Hook, 2002a). The psychoanalytic perspective is an approach to personality development introduced by Sigmund Freud. It assumes that child ren move through a series of stages in which they confront conflicts between biological drives and social expectations. The way these conflicts are resolved determines psychological adjustment – person’s ability to learn, get along with others, and cope with anxiety (Berk, 1996; Hook & Watts, 2002). 2.3.2 Freud’s psychosexual theory Freud argued that infants and young children are sexual beings and that the way they are permitted to express their impulses lay at the heart of their adult behaviour (Berk, 1996; Freud, 1977; Hook & Watts, 2002). Freud constructed his psychosexual theory of development on the basis of adult remembrances. It emphasises the fact that how parents manage their child’s sexual and aggressive drives in the first few years of life is crucial for healthy personality development (Hook & Watts, 2002). Freud’s psychosexual theory highlighted the importance of family relationships for children’s development. It was the first theory to stress the importance of early experience for later deve lopment (Fonagy, 1999). In Freud’s psychosexual theory, children move through five stages, during which three portions of the personality – id, ego, and superego – become integrated (Hook & Watts, 2002; Kaplan & Sadock, 1998). The id - the largest portion of the mind, is inherited and present at birth. It is the source of basic biological needs and desires. The id seeks to satisfy its impulses head on, without delay (Freud, 1991). The ego (the conscious), rational part of the personality, emerges in early infancy to ensure that the id’s desires are satisfied in accordance with reality 10 (Freud, 1991). Between the ages of 3 and 6 years, the superego (the seat of conscience), the third portion of personality, appears. It contains the values of society and is often in conflict with the id’s desires. The superego develops from interactions with parents, who eventually insist that children control their biological impulses. Once the superego is formed, the ego is faced with the increasingly complex task of reconciling the demands of the id, the external world, and the conscience (Berk, 1996; Hook & Watts, 2002; Louw, 1991). Strong critique of Freud’s psychosexual theory indicated that the theory is culturally biased and that he emphasised the role of the unconscious in determining behaviour. Freud prioritised the sexual level of development. Erikson by contrast, focussed on the adaptive abilities of the ego and prioritised the social level of development. 2.3.3 Erikson’s psychosocial theory Erikson’s psychosocialtheory builds on Freud’s theory by emphasising the development of culturally relevant attitudes and skills and the lifespan nature of development (Hook, 2002b). Although Erikson accepted Freud’s basic psychosexual framework, he expanded the picture of development at each stage. For Erikson, the psychosexual stages concerned zones for relating to objects, and they entailed unconscious expressions of the child’s needs (McCarthy, 2000). In his psychosocial theory, Erikson emphasised that social experiences at each Freudian stage do not just lead to an embattled ego that mediates between id impulses and superego demands. The ego is also a positive force of development. At each stage, it acquires attitudes and skills that make the individual an active, contributing member of society (Berk, 1996; Hook, 2002b). A basic psychological conflict, which is resolved along a continuum from positive to negative, determines healthy or maladaptive outcomes at each stage (Moshman, 1999). 11 Erikson’s first five stages parallel Freud’s stages. However, Erikson did not regard important developmental tasks as limited to early childhood. He believed that they occurred throughout life. Unlike Freud, Erikson pointed out that normal development must be understood in relation to each culture’s unique situation (Moshman, 1999). Erikson postulated eight psychosocial stages: 2.3.3.1 Basic trust versus mistrust (birth – 1 year) From warm responsive care, infants gain a sense of trust, or confidence, that the world is good. Mistrust occurs when infants have to wait too long for comfort and are handled harshly. This stage involves developing, or failing to develop a basic sense of trust in the world. 2.3.3.2 Autonomy versus shame and doubt (1 – 3 years) Using new mental and motor skills, children want to choose and decide for themselves. Autonomy is fostered when parents permit reasonable free choice and do not force or shame the child. This stage involves developing a sense of oneself as an autonomous agent. 2.3.3.3 Initiative versus guilt (3 – 6 years) Through make-believe play, children experiment with the kind of person they can become. Initiative – a sense of ambition and responsibility – develops when parents support their child’s new sense of purpose and direction. The danger is that parents will demand too much self-control, which leads to overcontrol, or too much guilt. This stage involves developing a sense of initiative and ambition. 2.3.3.4 Industry versus inferiority (6 – 11 years) At school, children develop the capacity to work and cooperate with others. Inferiority develops when negative experiences at home, at school, or with peers lead to feelings of incompetence. This stage involves developing a sense of industry and competence. 12 2.3.3.5 Identity versus identity diffusion / role confusion (adolescence) The adolescent tries to answer the question, “Who am I, and what is my place in society?” Self -chosen values and vocational goals lead to a lasting personal identity. The negative outcome is confusion about future adult roles. 2.3.3.6 Intimacy versus isolation (early adulthood) Young people work on establishing intimate ties to others. Because of earlier disappointments, some individuals cannot form close relationships and remain isolated from others. The central task of this stage is the development of a capacity for intimate relationships. 2.3.3.7 Generativity versus stagnation (middle adulthood) Generativity means giving to the next generation through child rearing, caring for other people, or productive work. The person who fails in these ways feels an absence of meaningful accomplishment. This stage focuses on the development of generativity, a commitment to future generations. 2.3.3.8 Ego integrity versus despair (old age) In this final stage, individuals reflect on the kind of person they have been. Integrity results from feeling that life was worth living as it happened. Old people who are dissatisfied with their lives fear death. This stage is concerned with the formation of a sense of integrity with respect to one’s life. (Berk, 1996; Hook, 2002b; Moshman, 1999). At the same time that Freud and Erikson gained in prominence, a very different perspective, behaviourism, also influenced child study. 2.3.4 Behaviourism Behaviourism is a psychological theory of human development that propose that humans can be trained, or conditioned, to respond in specific ways to specific stimuli and that 13 given the correct stimuli, personalities and behaviours of individuals, and even entire societies, can be codified and controlled (Kaplan & Sadock, 1998; Staddon, 1993). Edward Thorndike (1874-1949) initially proposed that humans and animals acquire behaviours through the association of stimuli and responses (Staddon, 1993). He advanced two laws of learning to explain why behaviours occur the way they do: the Law of Effect specifies that any time a behaviour is followed by a pleasant outcome, that behaviour is likely to recur. The Law of Exercise states that the more a stimulus is connected with a response, the stronger the link between the two (Staddon, 1993). John Watson championed the cause of modern behaviourism early in the 20th century (Rachlin, 1991). He claimed that psychology was not concerned with the mind or with human consciousness. Instead, psychology would be concerned only with behaviour. In this way, people could be studied objectively (Rachlin, 1991). Watson's work was based on the experiments of Ivan Pavlov, who had studied animals' responses to conditioning (Rachlin, 1991). In Pavlov's well-known experiment, he rang a bell while he fed some dogs several meals. Each time the dogs heard the bell they knew that a meal was coming, and they would begin to salivate. Pavlov then rang the bell without bringing food, but the dogs still salivated. They had been "conditioned" to salivate at the sound of a bell. Pavlov believed, as Watson was later to emphasise, that humans react to stimuli in the same way (Rachlin, 1991). Watson was reacting to emerging Freudian psychoanalytical theories of development. His scheme rejected the entire hidden, unconscious, and suppressed longings that Freudians attributed to behaviours and proposed that humans respond to punishments and rewards (Rachlin, 1991). Behaviour that elicits positive responses is reinforced and continued, while behaviour that elicits negative responses is eliminated. Behaviourism is associated today with Skinner, who made his reputation by testing Watson's theories in the laboratory (Nye, 1992; Todd & Morris, 1995). Skinner's studies led 14 him to reject Watson's almost exclusive emphasis on reflexes and conditioning. People respond to their environment, he argued, but they also operate on the environment to produce certain consequences. He developed the theory of "operant conditioning," the idea that we behave the way we do because this kind of behaviour has had certain consequences in the past (Nye, 1992; Todd & Morris, 1995). Like Watson, however, Skinner denied that the mind or feelings play any part in determining behaviour. Instead, our experience of reinforcements determines our behaviour (Nye, 1992; Todd & Morris, 1995). In the 1950s, how ever, the popularity of behaviourism began to decline. Cognitive development was the next focus of inquiry. In contrast to behaviourism, the cognitivedevelopmental theory emphasised an active child with a mind inhabited by rich structures of knowledge. 2.3.5 Piaget’s cognitive-developmental theory Jean Piaget introduced the cognitive-developmental theory. It holds that children are actively building psychological structures and cognitive development as taking place in stages (Cockcroft, 2002). Piaget believed that just as the structures of the body are adapted to fit with the environment, so the structures of the mind develop over the course of childhood to better fit with, or represent, the external world (Berk, 1996). This can be achieved if the parent-child relationship is secure and the child feels safe enough to explore his environment and incorporate this into his cognitive structure. Piaget argued that children move through five stages, beginning with the infant’s sensorimotor action patterns and ending with the elaborate, abstract reasoning system of the adolescent. Piaget suggested four stages of cognitive development: 15 2.3.5.1 Sensorimotor (birth - 2 years) From the outset, biology and experience blend to produce learned behaviour. A stimulus is received, a response elicited, accompanied by awareness. As children become more mobile, these experiences build on one another. 2.3.5.2 Preoperational thought (2 - 7 years) Children use symbols and language more extensively, but thinking and reasoning are intuitive. Children are unable to think logically or deductively. 2.3.5.3 Concrete operations (7 - 11 years) Egocentric thought is replaced by “operational” thought, which involves attending to information outside. Children are able to understand another’s point of view. Children in this stage can serialise, order, and group things according to common characteristics. 2.3.5.4 Formal operations (11 years – end of adolescence) Children can think abstractly, reason deductively, and define abstract concepts. Not all children enter this stage at the same time or to the same degree. Children that have had no sensory stimulation, as is the case in abusive and neglectful family homes and in some children’s homes, might tend to struggle with formal operations (Cockcroft, 2002). Piaget’s stages of cognitive development have sparked a wealth of research on children’s conceptions of themselves, other people, and human relationships – this relates to Bowlby’s internal working model, which will be discussed in the following chapter. New ways of understanding the child are constantly emerging – questioning, building on, and enhancing the discoveries of earlier theories. 2.4 Current perspectives The field of child development continues to seek new directions, one of which is the information processin g approach. It views the mind as a complex, symbol-manipulating 16 system, operating much like a computer (Cockcroft, 2002). This approach helps investigators achieve a detailed understanding of what children of different ages do when faced with tasks and problems. Three modern theories place special emphasis on contexts for development. Ethology stresses the adaptive, or survival, value of behaviour and its origins in evolutionary history (Cockcroft, 2002). In ecological systems theory, nested layers of the environment, which ranges from the child’s immediate settings to broad cultural values and programs, are seen as major influences on children’s well-being (Berk, 1996; Hook, 2002c). Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory has enhanced our understanding of cultural influences, especially in the area of cognitive development. Through cooperative dialogues with mature members of society, children acquire culturally relevant knowledge and skills (Cockcroft, 2002; Morgan, 1999). According to Vygotsky (in Morgan, 1999), it is the internalised voice of the other that becomes an aid in thinking and a way of directing one’s behaviour. From a cognitive and developmental perspective Vygotsky’s notion of mediated thought describes how the mother acts as a social and cultural guide in extending her child’s current level of actual development in thinking to the child’s potential level (Morgan, 1999). Just as complex as the heredity that sets the stage for development, is the child’s environment – a many-layered set of influences that combine with one another to help or hinder the course of growth. 2.5 Importance of family context for development The early relationship environment is crucial not only because it shapes the quality of subsequent relationships but because it serves to equip the individual with a mental processing system that will subsequently generate mental representations, including relationship representations (Fonagy, 2001). Environments can enhance growth or create risks for children. When a vulnerable child – a youngster with physical or psychological problems – is exposed to unfavourable child -rearing contexts, development is seriously 17 threatened. The context that fosters optimal growth is, in most cases and ideally, the family (Hook, 2002d). The family introduces children to the physical world through the opportunities it provides for play and exploration of objects (Senior, 2002). It also creates bonds between people that are unique. The attachments children form with parents and siblings usually last a lifetime, and they serve as models for relationships in the wider world of neighbourhoods and schools (Goldberg, 2000). Within families children also experience their first social conflicts. Discipline by parents and arguments with siblings provide children with important lessons in compliance and cooperation and opportunities to learn how to influence the behaviour of others. Within families children learn the language, skills, and social and moral values of their culture (Berk, 1996; Goldberg, 2000). Naturally the ideal then would be to place children in an environment where all these needs are met should the family fail in this task. This is the motivation behind the children’s home. It is meant to be a substitute environment where the child is able to achieve optimal development in the absence of the family. Children’s homes however often fail to live up to these expectations. The major characteristics associated with institutional care are: general intellectual retardation, retardation in language functions, a social ‘personality’ disturbance, chiefly disturbances centring on the capacity to establish and maintain close personal relationships (Bowlby, 1952). In the report by Bowlby on Maternal Care and Mental Health, he argued that maternal separations were a clear risk factor for mental illness, and that institutional care was very damaging to children unless it provided them with a true “mother substitute” (Feeney, Hohaus, Noller, & Alexander, 2001). In the institutional setting, moreover, there is less opportunity for the child who has learnt the process of abstraction and mental organisation to exercise them. In the family, the young child is, within limits, encouraged to express himself both socially and in play (Bowlby, 1952). 18 2.6 Adolescence The age at which adolescence begins varies from 11 to 13 years because of individual and cultural differences and the age at which it ends varies from 17 to 21 years (Louw, 1991). Adolescence is the developmental stage between childhood and adulthood. The term ‘adolesce nce’ means ‘to grow up’ or ‘to grow to adolescence’ (Louw, 1991, p. 377). Adolescence is characterised by profound biological, psychological, and social developmental stages. The biological onset of adolescence is signalled by the rapid acceleration of skeletal growth and the beginnings of physical sexual development. The psychological onset is characterised by an acceleration of cognitive development and consolidation of personality formation. Socially, adolescence is a period of intensified preparation for the coming role of young adulthood (Kaplan & Sadock, 1998). 2.6.1 Cognitive development in adolescence Piaget attempted to demonstrate that over the course of childhood and early adolescence individuals show qualitative changes in the nature of their cognition, and such changes are internally directed, and such changes are progressive in the sense that later cognitive structures represent a higher level of rationality than earlier ones (Moshman, 1999). The adolescent’s cognitive ability develops both quantitatively, in the sense that the adolescent is able to master intellectual tasks more easily and more effectively, and qualitatively in the sense that changes take place in the nature of the thought processes and the cognitive structure (Cockcroft, 2002). The adolescent thus reaches, according to Piaget’s theory, the level of formal operations. At the beginning of adolescence, thinking usually becomes abstract, conceptual, and future oriented. Many adolescents show remarkable creativity (Kaplan & Sadock, 1998). Formal operations allow the adolescent, for example, to understand and evaluate reality as the realisation of a particular possibility. At the level of 19 formal operations, there is a radical reversal of perspective present during the concrete operations level: rather than considering possibilities with respect to reality, reality is considered with respect to possibilities. The formal thinker spontaneously and systematically generates possibilities, and reconstrues realities in the light of those possibilities (Moshman, 1999). 2.6.2 Emotional and social development in adolescence Adolescents, while proceeding through this period, are faced with the numerous tasks that they must master before they become adults (Gitter, 1999). The developmental task of attaining autonomy, becoming independent in one’s thoughts and opinions as well as actions, has long been thought of as one of the central processes of adolescence (Hill & Holmbeck, 1986). This process is mostly easily steered in the context of a close relationship with parents rather than at the expense of this relationship (Gitter, 1999). In addition to this, the major task of adolescence is to achieve a secure sense of self, to form an identity. For adolescents, identity is both a matter of determining who one is and a matter of deciding who one will be. According to Moshman (1999), identity is, at least in part, an explicit theory of oneself as a person. Identity is generally seen as related to the self, with the understanding that neither term is easy to define and that the relationship of the two concepts is far from clear (Ashmore & Jussim, 1997). Although identity formation is a challenging process even under the best circumstances, problems in earlier development (early attachments) may render it even more difficult and decrease the likelihood of positive outcomes (Moshman, 1999). Identity diffusion is a failure to develop a cohesive self or self-awareness. Adolescent identity crisis is partly resolved by the move from dependency to independence. The initial struggles often revolve around the established concepts of sex roles and gender identification (Kaplan & Sadock, 1998). Erikson’s psychoanalytic psychology of adolescence was based on the 20 twofold importance of identity formation and the ego’s adjustment to the drives and to society. His writings on adolescence addressed both the anxiety inherent in the process of identity formation, and the analytic task of helping the adolescent to assess values and choices from the point of view of identity synthesis (McCarthy, 2000). Besides developing physically, cognitively and morally, the adolescent has to achieve social maturity. The adolescent’s ability to handle the developmental tasks associated with social development (e.g. the development of indepe ndence), will, to a large extent, be determined by his physical and cognitive maturity, but also by the complexity or level of modernisation of the society in which the adolescent grows up; the characteristics of his subculture (ethnic and socio-economic) and the attitudes and reactions of society to his subculture; and the family structure and parental influence (Louw, 1991). 2.7 Conclusion In this chapter a broad overview of the theoretical contributions made to the field of child development was given. The investigation focussed on ‘normal’ child development as a benchmark against which we can gauge any straying from the developmental path. The developmental stage of adolescence was discussed under the headings of cognitive development and emotional and social development. Chapter 3 will look at Attachment Theory and the contributions that were made to this field of study. 21 CHAPTER 3 AN OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY 3.1 Introduction The growth of psychological knowledge has shown that children need a stable home and the love and care of two parents if they are to achieve optimal emotional development (Chesser, 1912-). They need to experience real affection, appreciation and recognition from other people. Bowley (1947) asserts that without this, children receive no assurance that they are good, worthy, loveable, and wanted. Bowley (1947) further emphasised the importance for the child’s emotional development that children should have this assurance to offset their inner fears of badness, unworthiness, unlovableness, and their sense of rejection. Attachment theory has focused on the processes whereby infants and young children develop confidence in their parents’ protection (Goldberg, 2000). This chapter presents a review of attachment theory and related issue s. The focus is mainly on the ideas of John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth, as well as those who have influenced their ideas. Before doing this, an operational definition of attachment will be provided. 3.2 Definitions of attachment Attachment refers to the enduring ties that children form with their primary caregivers; it includes a desire for proximity to an attachment figure, a sense of security derived from the person’s presence, and feelings of distress when the person is absent (Cassidy, 1999; Gomez, 1997; Morgan, 1999). Although parent -child relationships are not the only determinant of children’s behaviour, researchers believe that a developmental perspective based on people’s past and present socio-emotional 22 experiences, particularly within close relationships, offers a powerful insight into human personality, styles of caregiving and the character of interpersonal life (Howe, Brandon, Hinings, & Schofield, 1999). Relationships provide the key experience that connects children’s personal and social worlds (Kaplan & Sadock, 1998). The recognition by the individual that social life involves a constant interplay between self-reflexive minds possessed of their own feelings, motives, thoughts, beliefs and intentions is a central feature of attachment theory (H owe et al., 1999). 3.3 Bowlby’s attachment theory Attachment theory was originally introduced by John Bowlby in the nineteen fifties and early sixties and can be regarded as the joint work of Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth (Bretherton, 1991). The theory was further elaborated through the work of others such as Mary Main and Peter Fonagy (Gullestad, 2001). However, although attachment theory is currently the most acceptable theoretical explanation of attachment, psychoanalytic theory was the first theory that a ttempted to explain how attachment takes place. Therefore, in order to fully understand attachment theory, it is necessary to examine the psychoanalytical explanations of Sigmund Freud and especially Melanie Klein, as well as other theorists, as attachment theory essentially emerged in response to perceived shortcomings in those theoretical explanations. 3.3.1 Bowlby and Psychoanalysis In the psychoanalytic perspective, attachment patterns can be seen as mechanisms of defence, mastered by the child to cope with the idiosyncratic styles of interaction of his caregivers. Patterns of attachment are habitual ways of relating, developed by the ego to minimise anxiety and maximise adaptation (Fonagy, Steele, 23 Moran, Steele, & Higgitt, 1992). Psychoanalytic explanations are based on the premise of ‘I love you because you feed me’, and Bowlby’s attachment theory is based on the premise of ‘I was born to love you’ (Fonagy, 2001). According to Colin (1996), attachment theory continues to share many important views with psychoanalytic theories. Firstly, early adaptations have profound and long-lasting effects on the individual’s personality, social relationships, thoughts, feelings, and behaviour. Secondly, much of human motivation is unconscious. Thirdly, development reflects a coherent underlying organisation, even when surface manifestations in behaviour change with age, state, and situation. The focus is on whole people interacting in intimate and committed relationships, not on single variables (Colin, 1996). 3.3.1.1 Sigmund Freud There are a number of similarities between Bowlby’s attachment theory and the Freudian psychoanalytical view of attachment. Both theories concern themselves with unconscious processes and the psychological consequences of significant early deprivation (Fonagy, 1999). Freud’s conflict themes concerning wish and reality and internal and external reality remained essential building blocks for Bowlby and other attachment theorists. Bowlby pointed out that Freud was aware of the importance of attachment to the mother; this was however developed late in his research (Fonagy, 1999). He also noted that Freud’s observation indicated that abandonment and isolation distressed infants at 18 months of age. Freud understood that anxiety is rooted in the fear of losing the mother, he however understood it to be a fear of ungratified instincts. Freud also acknowledged that the child’s relation to the mother is unique and laid down unalterably at an early stage to become the prototype for all later love relations, and that there is more to this love relationship than food, and that 24 the experience of being cared for relates directly to self -esteem (Fonagy, 1999). Other than the points mention above, there seem to be no other points of contact in both theories (Fonagy, 1999). Freud and Bowlby differ radically in their image on man. Whereas Freud emphasises a monadic individual driven by untamed passion in a battle with the norms and requirements of society, Bowlby’s ethological model focuses on an interacting system, consisting of the child and his caregiver in which each partner adapts to the other through a mutual signalling system (Gullestad, 2001). In other words, whereas psychoanalysis emphasises a more intrapersonal focus on the individual, attachment theory giv es priority to the interpersonal and contextual dynamics of the individual. 3.3.1.2 Melanie Klein In Klein’s theory she regards the human psyche as having two basic positions: the paranoid-schizoid and the depressive (Gomez, 1997; Fonagy 1999). In the paranoid-schizoid position the relationship to the object (the caregiver) is to a part object split into a persecutory and idealised relationship, and the ego (the self) is similarly split (Fonagy, 1999, 2001). In the depressive position the relation is to an integrated parental image. The individual recognises his destructive wishes towards the object. This brings with it a certain characteristic sadness, but correspondingly, the ego is more integrated. In the paranoid-schizoid position the person moves between states of exaggerated perceptions of goodness (idealisation of both self and other) and similarly extreme persecutory perceptions of both self and other as bad (Fonagy, 1999, 2001). In the early depressive position primitive envy represents a particularly malignant form of innate aggression. This is because unlike other forms of 25 destructiveness, which are turned against bad objects already seen as persecutory, envy is hatred directed to the good object and arouses a premature expression of depressive anxiety about damage to the object (Fonagy, 1999, 2001). According to Bowlby, a major shortcoming in psychoanalysis was its emphasis it placed on internal conflict at the expense of the environment (Gomez, 1997). Bowlby was even more critical of the Kleinian approach to attachment. Bowlby was trained as a psychoanalyst at the British Psychoanalytical Institute where he had been exposed to Kleinian ideas through his training analyst, Joan Riviere and through supervision with Melanie Klein herself. As previously stated, although Bowlby acknowledged Kleinian thinking for its emphasis on object relations, he was dissatisfied with Klein’s contention that children’s emotional problems are mainly due to infantile fantasies generated from internal conflicts related to aggressive drives, rather than environmental failure (Gullestad, 2001). He further took issue with Klein’s understanding of development, which was based on extrapolations from clinical experience rather than on observations of normal children (Bretherton, 1995). Thus Bowlby was profoundly sceptical about the Kleinian detachment from external reality (Holmes, 1995). As a result he found an alternative theoretical model of attachment, which was rooted in ethology and based on the concept of imprinting, implying that bond formation need not be tied to feeding (Gullestad, 2001). 3.3.1.3 Erik Erikson Like Bowlby, Erikson saw early development as a continuous process, starting within the first minutes of postnatal experience and extending throughout life, taking different forms at different times. According to Erikson, the quality of the maternal relationship determines the sense of trust that mothers create in their children, which 26 then persists throughout life. The most important link he made was between trust and identity, which draws on the notion of coherence so central to Mary Main’s framework for adult attachment (Fonagy, 2001). Although Erikson stressed the importance of social agents for the facilitation of psychological development and for the ongoing articulation of the individual ego, his emphasis was not on the social relationship but rather on the previous circumstances and consequences of the attainment of self-identity. Like other psychoanalytic developmental theorists, he placed separation-individuation ahead of social involvement (Fonagy, 1999). His emphasis remains psychoanalytic, focussing on the individual rather than on the relationship. Identity consolidation is the central goal and attachment plays a secondary role. Erikson is less concerned with the movement from a secure, trusting dependency to a mature interdependence with a capacity to tolerate intimacy (Fonagy, 2001). 3.3.1.4 Margaret Mahler Carlsson and Sroufe (1995) referred to Mahler’s practicing subphase (9-17 months) of the separation-individuation process and the infant’s tendency to return for “emotional refuelling” as clearly similar to Bowlby’s “secure base” phenomenon. Similarly Mahler’s proposed link, between histories of well-regulated relationships with the caregiver and the smooth transition towards more autonomous functioning by the age of two years, is common ground between her theory and attachment theory (Fonagy, 2001). Mahler sees the infant as a concrete experience of the physical world, however according to Bowlby, the infant is biologically prepared for the establishment of early social relationships. In her framework, it is the disengagement from attachment that would be considered the hallmark of progress and the 27 enrichment of the self. Thus, although Mahler considers emotional availability of the caregiver to be essential, this is viewed as a precursor and precondition of the separation-individuation process. She thus also sees the development of the representation of the body as a function of the infant-caregiver relationship (Fonagy, 2001). 3.3.1.5 Donald Winnicott Winnicott worked within the Independent School of British Psychoanalysis where they had abandoned the libidinal driven structural model and developed a “selfobject” theory in which parts of the self are seen in dynamic interaction with each other and complementary internal and external objects (Gomez, 1997). British object relations theory focus primarily on attachment and attempt to understand the development of the individual as a unit in interaction. The self is seen as being comprised of, and its integrity and continuity being maintained by the gestalt of past and present interpersonal relationships (Fonagy, 2001). Winnicott postulated an inherent desire to develop a sense of self – a desire that can be hidden or falsified. He saw the child as evolving from a unity of mother and infant. Three functions of this unity facilitate healthy development: holding leading to integration of sensorimotor elements; handling – facilitating personalisation (autonomy); and object relating – resulting in the establishment of a human relationship (Fonagy, 1999; Gomez, 1997). What is described as “attachment” in Bowlby’s terms is “ego relatedness” in Winnicott’s terms. Relatedness is born of the experience of being allowed to be alone in the presence of somebody else (Winnicott, 1971). For the infant to be able to be alone, a sense of safety must be associated with experiencing the inner world. For the infant to learn about and represent the 28 experience of distress, it must be met by a combination of external feedback (e.g., the mother’s face and tone) that “explains” to the infant what he is feeling and a simultaneous communication of having coped with the distress (Gergeley & Watson, 1996; Winnicott, 1971). According to Winnicott, the mother has to be “good-enough”, but her failure is expectable and is in fact the major motivation of growth. This is consistent with the observation of attachment researchers that moderate degrees of maternal involvement are preferable to highly dependent responses (Fonagy, 1999). The lack of “goodenough” mothering causes distortions or deviations in mental functioning and prevents the establishment of an internal environment that could become the essence of the self (Fonagy, 2001). A second major aspect of Winnicott’s theory relevant to attachment theory concerns environmental failure and the infant’s reaction to it. On the one hand, internal and external impingements and the lack of a holding environment can lead to aggression and antisocial behaviour. T his is characterised by the use of physical action as self-expression, a lack of concern for the other, and a definition of self in opposition to the environment. On the other hand, external impingement and the substitution of the gestures of the other for the gestures of the self engenders a “false self”, which appears real, performs and complies, and may be true in highly selected aspects or based on wholesome identification with the object (Fonagy, 1999). From the point of view of attachment theory, the two categories of environmental failure may be seen as alternative coping strategies to deal with insensitive caretaking (impingement) (Fonagy, 1999). In summary, Winnicott believed that the true self could only evolve in the presence of an unobtrusive other who will not interrupt the continuity of its experience of itself. Thus, he does not consider relationships to be independent from 29 instincts and has an integrated formulation where instincts and object relationships are intricately interwoven (Fonagy, 2001). 3.3.2 Attachment behaviour Bowlby defined attachment behaviour in observable terms as any form of behaviour that results in a person attaining or retaining proximity to some other individual from the self (Bowlby, 1988). This is usually an older larger figure. So long as the attachment figure remains accessible and responsive the behaviour may consists of little more than checking by eye or ear on the whereabouts of the figure and exchanging occasional glances or greetings. In certain circumstances, however, following or clinging to the attachment figure may occur and also calling or crying which are likely to elicit caregiving (Morgan, 1999). When the attachment behavioural system is in its goal state (i.e. there is adequate proximity and contact for the environmental conditions), attachment behaviours are not evident, but if threats to safety are perceived, attachment behaviours are activated (Goldberg, 2000). Attachment behaviour brings infants into close proximity to their primary caregivers. It is within these close relationships that children learn about themselves, other people and social life in general (Howe et al., 1999). According to Bowlby, the goal of the child is not an object (e.g., the mother) but rather a state – maintenance of the desired distance from the mother, depending on the circumstances (Cassidy, 1999; Goldberg, 2000). Bowlby theorised that attachment serves an evolutionary purpose, enhancing the survival of the species. Humans, like other primates, possess an evolutionary adapted behavioural system, the goal of which is maintaining proximity to a caregiver, particularly when danger threatens, and thereby ensuring the vulnerable infant’s protection from predators and ultimate 30 psychological and physical survival (Senior, 2002) . Bowlby envisioned the attachment system operating in the context of other behavioural systems (e.g., exploration, sociability) with its ‘set goal’ adjusted to fit the context. The goals of the attachment system are also modified as the child develops, so that longer separations and greater distances are deemed to be ‘safe’ (Goldberg, 2000). 3.3.3 Phases of the development of attachment Bowlby (1969) postulated four phases in the development of an infant’s attachment: 3.3.3.1 Preattachment (ages 0 – 2 months) Indiscriminate social responsiveness - Infants however orient to their mothers, follow them with their eyes over a 180-degree range, and turn toward and move rhythmically with their mother’s voice. 3.3.3.2 Attachment-in-the-making (ages 3 – 6 months) Discriminate social responsiveness - Infants become attached to one or more people in the environment. 3.3.3.3 Clear-cut attachment (ages 7 months – 3 years) Active initiative in proximity and contact - Infants cry and show other signs of distress when separated from the caregiver or mother. This phase may occur as early as 3 months in some infants. When the infant is returned to the mother, the infant stops crying and clings, as if to gain further assurance of the mother’s return. Sometimes, seeing the mother after a separation is sufficient for crying to stop. 3.3.3.4 Goal-corrected attachment (ages 3+ years) Infant begins to understand mother’s point of view, feelings, plans and motives and makes inferences about her behaviour. The mother figure is seen as 31 independent, and a more complex relationship between the mother and child develops. 3.4 Ainsworth’s assessment of attachment security and insecurity by means of the “Strange Situation” Mary Ainsworth’s great contribution to attachment theory was to develop a procedure that attempted to measure attachment security in infants. This experiment called the “Strange Situation” is a research protocol for assessing the quality and security of an infant’s attachment (Kaplan & Sadock, 1998). The purpose was to study the effects of separation from the mother in an unfamiliar environment (Goldberg, 2000). Bowlby’s theoretical genius was therefore nicely complimented by Ainsworth’s empirical creativity (Howe et al., 1999). Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall (1978) endeavoured to empirically investigate whether the quality of maternal responsiveness is directly tied to patterns of infantile behaviour, particularly comfortseeking and contact maintenance. The “Strange Situation”, involved eight brief (3 minute) episodes that provided the opportunity to observe a variety of the 12- to 24month-old infant’s responses to the stress of a new environment and of separation from an attachment figure. As attachment behaviour is activated when the individual experiences anxiety, all assessment procedures involved introducing mild levels of attachment-related emotional distress to those under study. The way in which the individual responds to and handles this distress is taken to reveal the strategy that he typically uses to regulate arousal and raised affect (Goldberg, 2000; Howe et al., 1999; Solomon & George, 1999). In the “Strange Situation”, the mother, infant and experimenter (stranger) settle into a playroom, and the mother then leaves the room for a few minutes. The 32 infant’s reaction to this separation, and the mother’s and infant’s responses when the mother returns, are noted. The experiment is used to assess and examine the motherinfant relationship and the infant’s ways of coping with separation. The response also shows whether the individua l sees the self as worthy and effective, and whether other people are seen as caring and available. Thus, the measures allow investigators to determine the individual’s internal working model and attachment style. Ainsworth also confirmed that attachment serves the purpose of reducing anxiety (Senior, 2002; Solomon & George, 1999). 3.5 Types of attachment patterns From this experiment Ainsworth was able to distinguish three primary attachment classifications: “secure”, “resistant” and “avoidant” (Howe et al., 1999). These patterns were linked to caregivers’ success or failure in responding to, and meeting, the infant’s needs (Goldberg, 2000). In later research, Main and Solomon described a third insecure category, the “disorganised/ disoriented” category (Goldberg, 2000; Morgan, 1999; Senior, 2002; Solomon & George, 1999). 3.5.1 Secure attachment The “secure” infant readily separated from the caregiver in the laboratory procedure and became easily absorbed in exploration. The infant was upset by the separation and demanded and received care from the caregiver when she returned, but continued explorative play thereafter. The securely attached infant develops the confidence that the caregiver will be available, responsive and helpful, should he be in a frightening situation (Gomez, 1997; Morgan, 1999; Senior, 2002). 33 3.5.2 Insecure-avoidant attachment The “insecure-avoidant” infant appeared uninvolved with the caregiver when she was present and was not overtly upset when she left and ignored her on her return, but watched her acutely and was unable to play freely. Main (in Morgan, 1999) highlighted that attachment behaviour is virtually absent throughout the “Strange Situation” in the avoidant infant and is replaced with active avoidance (looking away, moving away, turning away and leaning out of arms) as well as persistent attention to the inanimate environment. Avoidant behaviour was seen in infants who appeared less anxious during the separation and snubbed the caregiver on her return, avoiding eye contact and/ or using toys to distract their attention away from the caregiver. Avoidant coping interferes with the development of feelings of emotional connectedness (e.g., affection, empathy, dependency) and fosters a self-promoting and inflated selfconcept (“compulsive self-reliance”), leading to a focus on satisfying one’s own needs with little regard for those of others. The result is externalising behaviours, such as exploitation and aggression (Finnegan, Hodges, & Perry, 1996). 3.5.3 Insecure-ambivalent (resistant) attachment The “insecure-ambivalent” (resistant) infant was panicked by the separation and simultaneously clung to the caregiver and fought her off when she returned (Bowlby, 1988). The infant was also unable to return to his own activities. The resistant infant is uncertain whether the caregiver will be available or responsive or helpful when called upon. As a result of this uncertainty, the infant is always prone to separation anxiety and is anxious about exploring the world (Bowlby, 1988; Senior , 2002). This pattern tends to be promoted by inconsistent caregiving, i.e. a caregiver being available and helpful on some occasions, but not others. Because ambivalent coping inhibits exploration and mastery of the environment and interferes with the 34 development of age-appropriate strategies for regulating affect during even minor stressors, it renders the child vulnerable to fear responses and to self-perceptions of weakness and helplessness (Finnegan et al., 1996). 3.5.4 Disorganised/ disoriented attach ment The disorganised/ disoriented infant had no coherent strategy whatsoever to deal with the experience of separation and showed disorganisation and dissociation upon reunion (Bowlby, 1988). The disorganised/ disoriented infant was confused and chaotic, with bizarre patterns of repetitive movements or frozen paralysis expressing his bewilderment (Gomez, 1997). Some instances of disorganised attachment are seen in infants known to have been physically abused and/ or grossly neglected by the parent (Crittenden, 1985), in infants of mothers who are still preoccupied with mourning a parental figure lost during the mother’s childhood and in infants of mothers who themselves suffered physical or sexual abuse as children (Main & Hesse, 1990). 3.6 Quality of maternal responsiveness to infants The kind of attachment shown by infants was closely linked with the mothers’ responsiveness to them during their first year (Bowlby, 1988; Gomez, 1997). The mother’s expressed attitude towards her infant is the overwhelmingly deciding factor in how secure the infant will be at one year, a pattern that holds true even for infants who are easily upset in their first few months. Maternal sensitivity and responsiveness are the main determinants of secure attachment (Senior, 2002). If the mother receives help in changing her feelings and behaviour towards her infant, the infant can develop a secure attachment from an insecure starting point (Kobak, 1999; Weinfield, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, 1999). Another factor that may play a s ignificant role in 35 facilitating the mother’s responsiveness to her infant is the existence of support systems – family, friends, physicians, and nurses (Berk, 1989). The mothers of secure infants are the most attuned and sensitively responsive to their infants, interacting with them freely and with enjoyment, picking up their signals accurately and responding to their distress promptly (Goldberg, 2000). They are described as emotionally expressive and flexible in dealing with their infants (Weinfield et al., 1999). The insecure-avoidant infants are likely to have mothers who interact with them less, are slow to respond to distress, are uncomfortable with close body contact, and hold a practical rather than personal attitude towards them. Their positive feelings about the infant are often overcome by anger and irritation (Goldberg, 2000). They are minimally expressive, relatively rigid in dealing with their infants, and often interfere unnecessarily with their infants’ activities (Weinfield et al., 1999). The mothers of insecure-ambivalent infants tend to respond unpredictably and inconsistently, and are rather insensitive to their infants’ signals but are less rejecting than mothers of insecure-avoidant infants (Goldberg, 2000). They are more likely to be inept in physical contact with their infants, and they show little spontaneous affection (Kobak, 1999). The insecure -disorganised infants generally come from profoundly disturbed backgrounds involving abuse, severe neglect or psychosis (Goldberg, 2000). It is thus clear that how mothers respond to their infants, has a huge influence on the type of attachment patterns that the child will develop. 3.7 Internal working models/ mental representations A basic assumption of attachment theory is that critical variations in the quality of one’s early experiences with caregiving figures shape the formation of mental representations or internal working models of close relationships (Senior, 36 2002). Internal working models are cognitive structures in which the individual mentally represents the self, others and relationships. They are thus schematic internal representations of external experience (Howe et al., 1999; Morgan, 1999). These representations are influenced by experience and are subject to change as new experiences accumulate (Goldberg, 2000). The “Strange Situation” observes the relationship as manifested in both the child and mother’s behaviour rather than something that belongs only to the mother or only to the child. The child’s internal working model reflects the nature and structure of this relationship and the kind of care he has received (Morgan, 1999). Internal working models have two dimensions: (a) self model – containing perceptions of one’s own worth and lovability, and (b) other model – containing expectations regarding the essential goodness, trustworthiness, and dependability of important others in one’s social world. Those who experience sensitive and emotionally available caregiving develop a sense of others as dependably available and supportive , a sense of themselves as competent and worthy of attention and affection, and generally positive expectations of intimate relationships (Senior, 2002). The secure child has an inner representation of a lovable self and responsive other, with enjoyable interactions alternating with exciting explorations in an interesting world (Gomez, 1997). According to Zeanah, Mammen and Lieberman (1993), when the caregiving is inadequate, the individual develops deficiencies in feelings about self and others, and a negative set of expectations regarding relationships. The insecure-ambivalent child, on the other hand, has a picture of a self, which is not lovable, and an unpredictable other that has to be manipulated or coerced into caring. The insecure-avoidant child ha s an internal model of a self which is not worthy of care and an other who does not care, forcing the child to repress his longing 37 and his anger in order not to drive the other even further away. In this pattern of detachment the child disowns his anger, needs and anxiety and the awareness of his caregiver’s rejection in what Bowlby termed “defensive exclusion” (Gomez, 1997). According to Bowlby (1973, 1988) a person’s internal working model, once formed in early childhood, tend to persist and serve as a template for his subsequent close relationships. Bowlby further asserted that internal working models allow individuals to anticipate the future and make plans, thereby operating most sufficiently. By 3 or 4 years of age, physical separations no longer present as serious a threat to a child, and consequently do not produce the same kinds of emotional reactions. In large part, the child’s developing capacities to represent an absent parent, talk about impending separations, and plan for reunions with the parent reduce the problem posed by separations from an attachment figure (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999). 3.8 Attachment in the family system Thus far the focus has been on the development of a relationship between an infant and a primary caregiver, usually the mother. However, such dyads do not exist in a vacuum, but are nested within a family environment that influences the development and functioning of infants and their primary caregivers. Family members other than parents also serve as attachment figures, and a network of surrounding relationships influences each specific attachment. 3.8.1 Fathers Although fathers are capable of serving as attachment figures, most fathers have limited opportunities to respond to attachment-relevant situations, and may be 38 more oriented to the role of ‘playmate’ than that of ‘protector’. Based on their observations, Clarke-Stewart; Cohen and Campos; and Lamb found that older infants and toddlers displayed more affiliative behaviour towards fathers and more attachment behaviour towards mothers (Goldberg, 2000). This gave rise to the suggestion that fathers and mothers adopt different roles and influence different aspects of their children’s development. Although there are numerous studies documenting the ability of fathers to carry out the activities and responsibilities traditionally performed by mothers, few fathers are primary caregivers of infants, fathers are less available than mothers during infants’ waking hours, and fathers are more likely than mothers to read and watch television and to play than to engage in caregiving activities when they are with their infants (Goldberg, 2000). When engaged in similar activities, father -infant and mother-infant interactions are often qualitatively different. Fathers choose to engage in arousing playful interactions, while the activities of mothers are more likely to be directed towards reducing arousal and soothing the infant (Goldberg, 2000). During the rapproachment phase in the separation-individuation framework of Margaret Mahler (in Kaplan & Sadock, 1998) in which she describe how young children acquire a sense of identity separate from their mothers, the father’s availability is crucial for individuation, as mothers are rediscovered as separate individuals. Coming from outside the symbiotic fusion, fathers represent a stabilising force that offers toddlers a relationship uncontaminated by ambivalence (Goldberg, 2000). They provide support and ‘a breath of fresh air’ whenever toddlers are terrified by the idea of being reengulfed into symbiosis (Geiger, 1996). Interactions with fathers also shape children’s attachment security, though possibly to a lesser degree than those with mothers. In addition, fathers contribute to their children’s development in other vital ways, such as engaging in 39 physical play (Feeney et al., 2001; van Ijzendoorn & De Wolff, 1997). Fathers can also influence children’s adjustment indirectly by impacting on the mother’s sensitivity to her offspring. 3.8.2 Siblings Parents’ time and energy are increasingly divided as subsequent births add to the family. Depending on the age of the older children when these additions occur, some caregiving responsibilities are carried out by siblings (Goldberg, 2000). Attachment theorists have suggested a number of ways in which the older child’s attachment experiences influence their ability to care for a younger sibling. For example, internal working models of attachments are thought to include representations of relationships with caregivers, as well as the role played by each participant (child and caregiver). Thus, as children mature, they are predisposed to reenact elements of the kind of care that they themselves experienced. Children learn how to behave towards others, including younger family members, by following the behavioural models that their caregivers provide (Goldberg, 2000). Those who experienced care that generated a secure attachment should thus be more capable of providing similar care for siblings. In addition, the confidence that a child has developed regarding pa rental availability in times of distress may affect the extent to which new siblings are viewed as a threat to their own well being. Children who are unsure of the parent’s availability and have developed insecure attachment strategies may be more likely to react negatively to a new sibling (Goldberg, 2000). 40 3.8.3 Marital relationships and child attachment Parents may develop similar styles of response to their infants by influencing each other’s pattern of childcare. There are many opportunities for this to occur as couples discuss childcare, share the experience of rearing children, and learn through observation of each other. Parents may develop behaviour patterns that are complementary rather than similar. For example, one parent may relieve the other from infant care when exhaustion or preoccupation might lead to episodes of insensitive care. Similarly, one parent may take over non-parental tasks in order to free the other from distractions or stresses that interfere with sensitive care (Goldberg, 2000). Marital discord affects children’s experiences within the family and can have an influence on children’s behaviour. Marital discord can be described as marriages full of disagreements, quarrelling, and harsh sounds and includes conflict, disharmony, and lack of agreement between currently married parents, including separated (but not divorced) parents (Goldberg, 2000). Another aspect that might have an influence on children’s behaviour is the six family patterns as illustrated by Little (1982). • The first family pattern described by Little (1982) is the fragile bond marriage, where couples follow traditional roles – women spend most of their time and energy in the home caring for the children, while men develop careers. Within this marriage, there is an avoidance of confrontation on troublesome issues and the couple drifts apart emotionally but cling to the shell of their marriage. • The second is the fractured family. Couples also follow traditional roles in this type of marriage, where men avoid their wives and are verbally and sometimes physically abusive to them. Both women and children in these families 41 eventually have problems requiring professional attention and within these homes relationships are fractured not by divorce itself, but by the destructive behaviours exhibited in daily living. • The third family pattern is the doll’s house marriage where men and women are emotionally committed to their marriage and value family life. When there is a shift, where women are more successful then men in the workforce, women begin to perceive their marriage as a stifling prison and then they (women) eventually decide to leave the marriage. • The fourth is the stalemate marriage, where men characteristically avoid strong emotional confrontations and withdraw from their families and women on the other hand move aggressively against this withdrawal by pleading, demanding, criticising and finally threatening their husbands. A stalemate position, as in chess, is reached where neither partner can move and the game ends in a draw. Women, who are more dissatisfied with the unproductive communication patterns, leave the home. • The fifth family pattern described by Little (1982) is the perfect model marriage. These marriages appear more egalitarian than others. Illusions of a perfect model of marriage enable these men and women to maintain an emotional distance in their relationship. Men move away from the home as they become restless and dissatisfied with their wives. • The last family pattern described is the unformed family where women appear unready for marriage and have some difficulty forming bonds with their husbands and children. These brief marriages are characterised by women’s withdrawal and flight from the home and men’s confusion and complaints about their wives’ coldne ss and avoidance of intimacy. When these women 42 leave their families, the men feel confused and angry with the women for abandoning their roles as mothers and wives (Little, 1982). Types of parenting styles can also influence children’s attachments to their parents and the presence of behaviour problems. Michael Rutter (in Kaplan & Sadock, 1998) described four types of parenting styles: • Authoritarian – characterised by rigidity and strict rules, which can lead to depression in children; • Permissive – characterised by indulgence and no limit setting, which can lead to poor impulse control; • Indifferent – characterised by neglect and lack of involvement, leading to aggressive behaviour; and • Reciprocal – characterised by shared decision-making with behaviour directed in a relational manner, which results in a sense of self-reliance. 3.9 Attachment in adolescence While the importance of attachments may be fairly stable from infancy to old age, the organisation of attachment behaviour and the nature of attachment relationships change with age (Colin, 1996). Protection and security are the hallmarks of attachment throughout the life span, but the conditions that activate higher intensity levels of the attachment system, the type of attachment behaviour used, and the degree of proximity (or “contact”) that terminates attachment behaviours change with age (Colin, 1996). As discussed in the previous chapter, the key task of adolescence is to become autonomous and be less dependent on parents in making decisions. There is a pressure then to use peers as attachment figures in order for attachment needs to be met. From this perspective, adolescence is not a period in which attachment needs 43 and behaviours are relinquished, rather, it is one in which they are gradually transferred to peers (Steinberg, 1990). The central function of the adolescent’s attachment relationship with parents may be to provide an emotional secure base from which the adolescent can explore the wide range of emotional states that arises when he is learning to live as a relatively autonomous adult (Allen & Land, 1999). Close relations with parents foster the growth of adolescent self -reliance and individuation (Steinberg, 1990). According to Allen, Kupermine and Bell, (in Allen & Land, 1999) in adolescence, security of the parent-adolescent attachment should be reflected in a capacity of both the parent and the adolescent to assert autonomy and yet to maintain a sense of closeness or relatedness. Adolescent behaviour problems and symptoms thereof, may serve not only as expressions of distress or of psychopathology, but also as attempts to change the nature of interactions within a parent-adolescent dyad (Allen & Land, 1999). The implications of all this is especially severe for children, who have been neglected, maltreated and/ or abused. 3.10 Abused and traumatised children Most children ending up in care have been neglected, maltreated and/ or abused. Abuse and neglect is defined as the physical or mental injury, sexual abuse or exploitation, negligent treatment, or maltreatment of a child at the age specified by the child protection law of the state in question, by a person who is responsible for the child’s welfare to the extent that the child’s health or welfare is harmed or threatened (Erickson & Egeland, 1996). Children experience different forms of maltreatment. The term ‘maltreatment’ covers a variety of conditions under which children receive inadequate care (Goldberg, 2000). The American Humane Association (in Erickson & Egeland, 1996) describes emotional neglect as ‘passive or passive-aggressive 44 inattention to the child’s emotional needs, nurturing, or emotional well-being’. These definitions may vary somewhat depending on cultural contexts. Neglected children, if they survive physically, often fail to develop the confidence, concentration, and social skills that would enable them to succeed in school and in relationships (Erickson & Egeland, 1996). The Proceedings of the 1983 International Conference on Psychological Abuse of Children and Youth, highlighted psychological maltreatment and defined it as consisting of acts of omission and commission which are judged on the basis of a combination of community standards and professional expertise to be psychologically damaging (Hart, Brassard, & Karlson, 1996). Such acts are committed by individuals, singly or collectively, who by their characteristics (e.g., age, status, knowledge, organisational form) are in a position of differential power that renders a child vulnerable (Hart et al., 1996). Such acts damage immediately or eventually the behavioural, cognitive, affective, or physical functioning of the child. Examples of psychological maltreatment include acts of rejection, terror, isolation, exploitation, and missocialisation (Hart et al., 1996). At a generic level, many experts have considered psychological maltreatment to embody the repeated pattern of behaviour that conveys to children that they are worthless, unloved, unwanted, only of value in meeting another’s needs, or seriously threatened with physical or psychological violence (Brassard, Hart, & Hardy, 1991). Sexual abuse is the most prevalent form of abuse that children experience. Sexually abused adolescents report higher rates of emotional and behavioural problems than their nonabused peers (Berliner & Elliott, 1996). Sexually abused children tend to be less socially competent, more aggressive, and more socially withdrawn than nonabused children. Maternal distress and lack of support for the child appear to be associated with reporting higher levels of child behaviour problems 45 (Berliner & Elliott, 1996). Maternal belief in the child’s disclosure of the abuse and support following disclosure has a significant impact on later functioning. The highest risk of failure to support is found when the offender is a stepfather or the mother’s live-in boyfriend (Berliner & Elliott, 1996). Childhood sexual abuse by parents or stepparents may tap into ambivalent attachments in which the child clings to a caregiver who alternates between being sexually intrusive and threatening or rejecting (McCluskey & Hooper, 2000). The most important variable predicting out-of-home placement (placement in a children’s home) is whether the mother believes and supports the child (Hunter, Coulter, Runyan, & Everson, 1990). The increased symptomatology noted in children who are taken into protective custody may be secondary to the lack of caregiver support that provoked the placement. 3.11 Attachment abuse Studies of maltreated children identified patterns of attachment that are not generally seen in children with low -risk, and when the disorganised classification was available, a substantial number of maltreated children fell into this category and tended to remain disorganised on subsequent assessment (Goldberg, 2000). Mainly abuse, whether physical, psychological or sexual, is the main causal factor for children being placed in children’s homes. Children who are exposed to abuse or extreme forms of punishment are not only faced with threats to their attachment figure’s availability, they must also manage a more profound dilemma when their attachment figures are potential sources of danger (Kobak, 1999). In most instances in the children’s home and in this study, the children were abused by attachment figures (i.e. attachment abuse) and as a result were removed from the family and placed in care. Attachment abuse is defined as hurting the 46 feelings or the body of an attachment figure to construct a more tolerable, if momentary, experience of the self (Stosny, 1995). It begins with the abuser’s need for someone else to relieve the fear of being overwhelmed by his feelings and the need for someone else to set limits on his feelings. Attachment abuse results from the abuser’s inability to maintain tolerable self -constructions (set of beliefs, feelings, and behaviours about the self that form the perspective from which individuals construct meaning) and his desperate illusion that manipulating the mirror-reflection provided by attachment figures will fill-in the cracks and holes in his sense of self (Stosny, 1995). From Kleinian psychoanalysis came the idea that parents are not just the object of the child’s first love, but also of their hatred, as was expanded on earlier. Intrinsic aggression and inevitable frustration of desire lead to murderous hatred in the infant, which, as with libido, is ideally contained and detoxified by the caregiver, so that gradually the child learns to cope with feelings of disappointment and deferments of gratification (McCluskey & Hooper, 2000). In the case of physical abuse this process is perverted: rather than absorbing and buffering the hatred, the adult (caregiver) retaliates, using the child as a container of their own deprivation and pain (McCluskey & Hooper, 2000). The unique self-building (and potentially selfdestroying) capacity of the attachment bond forms the heart of all intimate relationship functions and dysfunctions. That which impairs, subverts, distorts, or damages the self-building nature of attachment interaction is rightly regarded as attachment abuse (Stosny, 1995). Abusive parents show strong emotional involvement with their children, while neglecting parents are more likely to be withdrawn and detached (Goldberg, 2000). 47 Parents with attachment deficits tend to lack social support systems, are more vulnerable to stress and more susceptible to disorganised family living, all factors known to contribute to child abuse (Berk, 1989). Maltreating parents are often characterised by a lack of understanding of the emotional complexity of human relationships, especially the parent-child relationship. They have difficulty seeing things from the child’s perspective or understanding behaviour in terms of the child’s developmental level and the context or situation (Berliner & Elliott, 1996). The perverse paradox of abuse from an attachment perspective arises from the vicious circle in which an adult who is a caregiver can be both the attachment figure to whom the child turns for protection, and the source of threat which gives rise to the nee d for that protection. The more frightened or in pain the child becomes, the more the child clings to the perpetrator (McCluskey & Hooper, 2000). The abused child seeks contact with the abusing caregiver because, paradoxically, the predictable, familiar, but adverse experience (which includes a clear representation of the child’s role) generates a greater sense of safety than an unfamiliar, nonabusive one (for which the child has no role relationship representations) (Weinfield et al., 1999). The home envir onments of maltreated children include emotional and physical rejection, hostile attempts at behaviour management, verbal and emotional assault, or an absence of a synchronist interactional style between caregiver and child (Crittenden & Ainsworth, 1989). Maltreated children in lower socio-economic groups are significantly more likely to form insecure attachment relationships with their mothers or other primary caregivers (Cicchetti, 1989). 48 3.12 Conclusion Attachment theory concerns itself with early caregiving relationships and the way that these relationships support the child’s subsequent development. The nature of the parent-child relationship during early childhood is believed to be one of the central causal factors in personality development and in terpersonal functioning, as well as having implications for psychopathology (Senior, 2002). Bowlby (1952) outlined instances in which the natural home group fail to care for the child. Firstly, the natural home group never established because the child is illegitimate. Secondly, the natural home group is intact, but is not functioning effectively because the breadwinner is unemployed with consequent poverty, chronic illness or incapacity of parents and instability or psychopathy of parents. Thirdly, the natural home group is broken up and therefore not functioning because of social calamity, death of the parent, illness requiring the parent to be hospitalised, imprisonment of the parent, desertion by one or both parents, separation or divorce, employment of the father elsewhere or the full time employment of the mother. Attachment theory has not been immune to criticism. Kagan (1984) highlights a number of these. The first criticism related to the reliability/ stability of research on which attachment theor y is based. It is argued that some research shows shifting of attachment upon retest. The role of temperament has further been questioned. Critics contend that temperament may explain behaviour in the “Strange Situation”. In other words, a securely attache d child with an easy-going temperament may not get upset when encountering a stranger or separation from his mother. Furthermore, the meaning of crying or the absence of crying is overrated. Finally it is argued that the “Strange Situation” is artificial a nd does not relate to the real world of the child. 49 Notwithstanding these criticisms, there seems to be overwhelming support for attachment theory. Mostly because attachment theory provides a sophisticated set of ideas for making sense of our feelings and behaviours in times of need and in the context of close relationships (Howe et al., 1999). It is one of the most thoroughly researched constructs in social and personality development and it has influenced childcare and public policy more than any other theory. In the following chapter, attention will focus on the various aspects of child institutionalisation (i.e. placement in children’s homes). 50 CHAPTER 4 INSTITUTIONALISATION 4.1 Introduction This chapter provides the context in which the study is grounded. In terms thereof the focus is on attachment of the unwanted child in a children’s home. The unwanted child is often mistreated, abused and/ or rejected and as a result institutionalised, i.e. placed in children’s homes. The relevance of this in the present study derives from the inextricable link between social action or experience and the context in which it is generated (Neuman, 1997). The Child Care Act of 1986 provides the legal and constitutional framework for the separation of children from their parents. Authority for the Act lies with the Departments of Welfare and Justice. In terms of the Act, a child can be removed from the custody of his parents if it is determined that the parents are unfit to have custody of the child. These criteria were later adapted with the promulgation of the Child Care Amendment Act (1996), which reverted to the ‘child in need of care’ concept as the criterion for determining whether a child should be removed from his parents’ care. This change was made in order to bring the laws in accordance with the child-centred focus of the June 1995 United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child (Committee on the Rights of the Child, 1999). If a parent is deemed unfit, a children’s court may then decide to place a child either in foster care under the supervision of a social worker or in a children’s home. In this study the placement of children in children’s homes constitute institutionalisation. There are close to 200 registered children’s homes in South Africa and while the Child Care Act (1983) provides for inspections at the behest of the Minister of Welfare, these are neither regular nor mandatory (Committee on the Rights of the Child, 1999). Although efforts are made to ensure that the needs of children in children’ s homes are met, 51 circumstances such as overcrowding and staff shortages makes this difficult and in many cases impossible. As a result, increasing numbers of professional social workers prefer foster homes to institutional settings for children in need of placement (Hilzinger, 2002). In the middle of the 20th century, psychoanalysts observed that children reared in large institutional homes with minimal stimulation and no consistent contact with a loving caregiver, often became emotionally unstable, lacked in conscience, or were mentally retarded (Westen, 1999). Although a number of factors could account for this, it is generally assumed that what is possibly lacking in the lives of these children is sufficient attachment. This is based on the fact that human beings are not designed to live in isolation from one another. Strong and permanent bonding is essential for the survival of all, especially the young and the vulnerable (Gomez, 1997). Human development is seen as a process of creating and maintaining attachments towards the primary attachment figure and other significant people (Howe et al., 1999; Senior, 2002). This chapter focuses on institutionalisation as the context in which study participants’ attachment patterns are explored. This focus will however be preceded by a discussion of two related issues, i.e. the essential requirements for good personality development and fundamentals for effective childcare. This will serve to further broaden our understanding of the relevance of institutionalisation within this thesis. 4.2 Essentials of good personality development To a large extent, the physical and mental health of a child is determined by the social, emotional, and moral milieu surrounding him prior to his birth (Safonova & Leparsky, 1998). Perhaps the first primary factor in ensuring sturdy growth is firm and stable roots. Children flourish best in an environment that is affectionate and secure (Bowlby, 1952). They need above all stability, safety, security, and affection. Children suck their thumbs more often 52 and for longer if they have received neither sufficient nourishment nor affection in childhood. The unwanted child is usually bottle -fed and hastily weaned (Bowley, 1947). What follows are fundamentals for effective childcare as set out by Dreyer (in Mudaly, 1985). 4.3 Fundamentals for effective childcare According to Dreyer (in Mudaly, 1985), the following are fundamentals for effective childcare: • Providing the opportunity for the child to experience basic physical care largely in the form of clothing, food, shelter, health care – areas in which the child has most likely suffered some measure of deprivation, • Providing the opportunity for the child to experience consistency and stability particularly in his immediate life-world, • Providing the opportunity for the child to experience spontaneous freedom to discover the world about him so as to foster personal growth within the constraints of his lifeworld, • Providing the opportunity for the child to communicate with and relate to any number of persons and in a variety of situations so as to learn appropriate ones – they need opportunities to talk about and act out their feelings and anxieties of separation, about their future, home, parents and family, • Providing a better future for the child by ensuring an adequate and challenging educational programme, • Providing for the child the right to self-determination, i.e. the child must learn to see options and choose from among them and take responsibility for the consequences of his decisions, and 53 • Providing the opportunity for the child to realise, appreciate and accept that people and circumstances change, and that he will have to come to terms with his own personal growth from a child to a self -sufficient adult. The familial context is an important element of the child’s social and emotional world because it is here that the child’s essential needs are catered for and the child learns to interact with others and form significant relationships. For the child to develop a good, healthy personality, his biological as well as emotional needs should be met. When these needs are not met the child may become socially inadequate, have feelings of being unwanted and struggle establishing peer- and relationships with significant others (Colin, 1996). Thus the logic behind legislation such as the Child Care Act of 1986 is to make provision for the placement of the child in a setting where the above-mentioned needs are met and the path towards healthy development is cleared. 4.4 Residential childcare Community-based group homes are today widely accepted as a resource for the care and treatment of homeless, psychologically traumatised and socially maladjusted children. Residential childcare programs usually have clearly stated goals, which include “…building on children’s strengths, helping them overcome problems and helping them leave in a better condition that they were when they came in” (Van Staden & Nieuwoudt, 2001, p.1). Van Staden and Nieuwoudt (2001) also note that two key professional issues have to be addressed by the children’s home: (a) the need to maintain order and quality of life in the homes, and (b) the need to ascertain that the work of the home is incorporated in a plan that involves preparing the children for the time when they leave the home. Residential care is usually only made available by the state to the individual, as a last resort, when his ‘natural’ care and support systems have failed in some way. This has been 54 outlined by Bowlby and is expanded on in the previous chapter. According to Davis (1981), residential care can function in two ways, which produce diverse outcomes. On the one hand residential care can be a positive response to situations in which the family is no longer able or willing to provide care for one of its members. It can offer a refuge from, or substitute for, family life. Indeed for some, it can offer some opportunities that were never part of the child’s own family situation. On the other hand, the transfer of responsibility for caring from the family to the residential unit can be problematic. It is argued that once the family hands over its caring responsibilities, the individual concerned is ‘cast out’ of society and placed in a world which can never be more than ‘second best’; a world in which the shame of social rejection shapes the relationships between staff and residents (Davis, 1981). Thus the transfer of responsibility for caring from the family to the residential unit can lead to the stigmatisation of those placed in residential care. Notwithstanding this Carlebach (in Davis, 1981) states that the right kind of residential provision for deprived and delinquent children could save them from moral delinquency and, at the same time, could save society from the consequences of their activities. Thus residential care can be seen not just as a means of ‘saving’ children from inadequate families and mothers, but also as a solution to the problem of inadequacy itself. 4.5 Factors that motivate institutionalisation A number of factors result in children being placed in residential care. These include inter alia maternal and other forms of deprivation, rejection and neglect, separation, and last but not least, abuse. 55 4.5.1 Maternal deprivation Bowlby’s critical contribution was his unwavering focus on the infant’s need for an unbroken (secure) early attachment to the mother. He thought that the child who does not have such a provision was likely to suffer from maternal deprivation (Fonagy, 2001). Maternal deprivation is thus defined as a situation in which an infant or young child is reared for a more or less prolonged period under conditions in which he receives inadequate maternal care and thus has insufficient interaction with a mother figure (Bowlby, 1952). Bowlby (1952) distinguished three forms of maternal deprivation on the basis of the extent of the deprivation suffered by the child. Firstly, partial deprivation occurs when the child lives with a mother (or permanent mother -substitute), including a relative, whose attitude towards him is unfavourable. Partial deprivation brings in its train acute anxiety, an excessive need for love, powerful feelings of revenge, and, arising out of these, guilt and depression. Secondly, complete deprivation occurs when the child loses a mother (or permanent mother-substitute) by death, illness, or desertion, and has no familiar relatives to care for him. Thirdly, complete deprivation occurs when the child is removed from his mother (or permanent mothersubstitute) to strangers by medical or social agencies, as a result of abuse and/ or neglect. Complete deprivation has even more far-reaching effects on character development and may entirely cripple the capacity to form relationships (Bowlby, 1952). 4.5.2 Other forms of deprivation In institutional settings other types of deprivation, each with potentially different implications, can be distinguished. For the purpose of this study, the most relevant forms are emotional and psychological deprivation. The term emotional deprivation can be restricted to characterise an environment with neutral feeling tone or without variation in feeling tone (Bowlby, 1952). Emotional deprivation symptoms that may occur in any combination and to 56 any degree are: (a) behaviour disturbances resulting from the immaturity of the ego and inadequate superego development, (b) impulsive behaviour, i.e. lack of self-control, (c) lack of anxiety and guilt, (d) antisocial, aggressive behaviour, (e) low achievement motivation, (f) lack of goal directedness, (g) lack of affect, i.e. lack of ability to make a one-to-one meaningful, lasting relationship, affectionless character, repression of all need for mother or friendships, shallow or nonexisting relationship formation, and (h) behaviour called psychopathic, sociopathic, or antisocial by various sources (Bowlby, 1952). Psychological deprivation, according to David (1992), refers to a condition produced by life situations in which a person is not given the opportunity to satisfy some basic (vital) psychological needs sufficiently and for a long enough period. As a result, appropriate actualisation and development are obstructed and distorted. Psychological deprivation is thus a characteristic inner end product of the prolonged impact of an impoverished environment – a psychological state resulting from a persistently restricted and/ or distorted interaction with the environment (David, 1992). Infants in institutions characterised by low staff-to-infant ratios and frequent turnover of personnel tend to display marked developmental retardation, even with adequate physic al care and freedom from infection (Kaplan & Sadock, 1998). Children tend to feel rejected and neglected as a result of the deprivation they experience. 4.5.3 Rejection and neglect According to Main and Goldman (in Morgan, 1999), neglect seems to refer to a passive disinterest in the child, while rejection refers to an active hostile or cold response to the child. When a child is unwanted, there is a danger that the child may be rejected and deprived of sensory and emotional stimuli (Safonova & Leparsky, 1998). Cold, passive neglect may cause retarded emotional growth and distrust of affection coupled with a need for affection, which may cause the child to be unable to achieve happy normal relationships 57 in later life (Chesser, 1912-). In some instances mothers reject their children when they fear that they may loose their husband or boyfriend’s affections because of a very strong allegiance to the child (Safonova & Leparsky, 1998). Another cause of rejection is when the child is unsatisfactory in some way. He may have some disfiguring birthmark, some physical defect, or some degree of mental defect (Safonova & Leparsky, 1998). In the cases discussed in this research, this is not so. These children are normal in all respects, physically, psychologically and mentally. A father’s negative attitude toward the child and psychological stress in the home is more common among unwanted children. An unwanted pregnancy (in combination with a number of psychosocial factors, i.e. single parenting, frequent family conflicts, negative attitude of the father, low utilisation of medical services, and tobacco and alcohol abuse) should be considered a risk factor that affects the emotional development of a child (Safonova & Leparsky, 1998). Unwantedness in early pregnancy has a detrimental effect on children’s psychosocial development (David, 1992). 4.5.4 Separation Separation refers to the physical separation of the infant or young child from his mother, whether permanently, or temporarily for longer or shorter periods (Bowlby, 1952) as illustrated in the “Strange Situation”. Separation from the person to whom the infant is attached precipitates separation anxiety (Kaplan & Sadock, 1998). An infant cannot survive and develop without intimate, committed, and consistent care of a caregiver. Appropriate care involves satisfaction not just of physical needs but also of emotional needs (Gomez, 1997). Attachment from the infant’s view refers to the specific affiliative tie of the infant to the mother, or father, which generally begins soon after six months of age (Cassidy, 1999). Once cumulative everyday experience has resulted in the infant’s developing an internal working 58 model of an attachment figure, separation from that figure is painful and the loss can be devastating (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999). Lengthy separation is particularly damaging for a child between six months and three years, when strong and specific attachments have developed but before the child is able to understand that the parent’s absence is temporary (Gome z, 1997). Typical reactions to separation in this age group can be divided into three phases. The first phase is protest. When the child has come to the end of his capacity to tolerate separation, he will do everything in his power to bring his attachment figure back. The protest stage can last up to a week. If the separation then ends, the child is likely to greet parents with anger, relief and anxious clinging. After protest comes despair. The child gradually loses hope that his lost person will return. This phase is followed by an apparent recovery, which Bowlby describes as detachment. The child represses or disinvests in his relationship with the lost person and begins to attach himself to an alternative figure. This can lead to considerable difficulties if the child is then reunited with his parents (Gomez, 1997). 4.5.5 Abuse Many children are placed in protective care in children’s homes as a result of abuse that is perpetrated in their family homes by an attachment figure. This topic has been discussed in detail in the previous chapter under the headings of 3.10 and 3.11, so no further elaborations will be made here. 4.6 Consequences of institutional rearing Children reared in institutions are said to suffer from serious personality defects. Although institutions have improved as a result of criticism about the adverse conditions that characterise them, those aspects of the institutional environment that Bowlby (1951) 59 considered most detrimental to mental health, remain unchanged (Tizard & Rees, 1975). Children brought up in institutions tend to suffer from two disabilities: stunted individuality caused by habitual conformity with institutional rules; and retarded emotional growth caused by ‘love starvation’ (Tizard & Rees, 1975). Children in institutions, according to Roberts (in Mudaly, 1985) come from deprived home circumstances because the primary family is no longer able to provide adequate protection and nurturing for the child. Childcare is a form of substitute care for children that generally necessitate total separation of the child from his biological family and his adjusting to a wholly new and unfamiliar environment. From his first encounter with care, the child lives in a new world of untested experiences and relationships (Mudaly, 1985; Van Staden & Nieuwoudt, 2001). The placement of children in substitute care is done in circumstances where the particular child’s needs cannot be adequately met in his own family or in fosterfamily care. The child in need of care is no longer the orphan or simply the child of the poor, but now mostly the victim of family breakdown, the victim of family neglect and abuse. Whittaker (in Mudaly, 1985), asserts that there is the substantial view that successful residential child care programmes will be those that actively focus on the growth and development in the child’s total life sphere (i.e. the expanded therapeutic milieu, embracing the family, peer group, school and community), rather than on the amelioration of psychiatrically defined syndromes or the ext inction of certain problematic behaviour. 4.7 Factors that negatively affect the impact of institutionalisation A number of factors affect the impact that institutionalisation will have on children. These include the age and time of placement, duration of institutionalisation, and traumatic conditions preceding or concomitant with institutionalisation. 60 4.7.1 Age and time of placement Research indicates that infants placed in institutions from birth to six months old were consistently less vocal than children reared by their own families, the difference being clearly discernable before the age of two months old (Batchelor, 1998; Safonova & Leparsky, 1998; Tizard & Rees, 1975). This backwardness of “talking” is especially characteristic of the institution child of all ages (Safonova & Leparsky, 1998). Infants under the age of six months who have been in an institution for some time tend to present with outstanding features of listlessness, emaciation, relative immobility, quietness, unresponsiveness to stimuli like a smile or a coo, indifferent appetite, failure to gain weight despite adequate diets, frequent stools, poor sleep, unhappy appearance, and the absence of sucking habits (Bowlby, 1952). This presents clear evidence that age and time of placement can negatively affect the impact that institutionalisation has on children placed in residential care. 4.7.2 Duration of institutional care The longer a child is institutionalised the more negative consequences are expected (Batchelor, 1998). Infants reared in institutions undergo an isolation type of experience, resulting in an isolation type of personality. The more complete the deprivation in the early years the more isolated and asocial the child, whereas the more the deprivation is interspersed with satisfaction, the more ambivalent and antisocial he becomes. Children placed in institutions for short periods after the age of two years do not develop this isolation type of personality or show the same behaviour patterns (Batchelor, 1998). 4.7.3 Traum atic conditions preceding or concomitant with institutional placement Some children are placed in care because of severe neglect, physical and/ or sexual abuse. These experiences are extremely traumatic in and of themselves and this is 61 compounded by the fact that the traumatised child now has to adjust to a different environment, namely residential care. For many children, this presents such a daunting task that they prefer to endure the abuse as opposed to having to move away from the only caregivers they know (Stosny, 1995). 4.8 Parental involvement in institutional placements Current research suggests that an effective program for the placement of a child outside his parents’ home must rely on the parents’ involvement and cooperation (Eisikovits & Beker, 1986). Littner (in Eisikovits & Beker, 1986) listed four primary reasons that explain why a child’s ties with his biological parents remain important even when the child is placed with foster parents or in institutions. Firstly, the child may feel the absence of the parents and miss them even if their relations are disturbed and difficult, because he is still dependent upon them for providing him with a source of security. Secondly, a child removed from his home could have no understanding of the reasons why he has been made to leave his parents. This may lead to a series of irrational explanations such as, for example, that he has been taken out of his home because he was a bad boy. Thirdly, the child is likely to identify with his parents in such a way that any criticism directed toward them, justified or not, is viewed by the child as an attack on himself. A fourth reason is that while separated from parents, the child may develop an incorrect image of them, whether positive or negative, and of himself as a consequence. Parental ties are also known to be necessary for the child’s development and achievements in the new environment, for working on the possibility and timing of the child’s return to the home, and the methods by which the institutions can contribute to the improvement of parental functioning (Van Staden & Nieuwoudt, 2001). Children whose parents visited the institution regularly and participated in institutional life and activities 62 reached higher academic achievements, a more strongly developed self-concept, and stronger ties to their biological families, and a better capacity to establish friendships (Van Staden & Nieuwoudt, 2001). Matsusima argues that preventing parent-child ties and encounters can only harm the child’s capacity to benefit from the institutional care (in Eisikovits & Beker, 1986). A lack of attention to parent-child ties harms both the child’s capacity for adaptation to the institution and his ability to benefit from the opportunities available because of the trauma of separation upon entrance and the repeated need for separation at each visit coupled with ignorance of the need to shape a new parent -child relationship. 4.9 Conclusion This chapter serves as an overview of the most pertinent aspects related to children’s placements in residential care, children’s homes, and institutions. The importance of this chapter derives from the fact that human experience does not take place in a vacuum but in a specific context. Thus a clear understanding of experience can only be arr ived at if consideration is given to the context in which these experiences are generated. This chapter therefore contextualizes the attachment experiences of the study participants. The chapter that follows will look at the methodology that is utilised in the study. 63 CHAPTER 5 METHODOLOGY 5.1 Paradigmatic approach The focus of the current inquiry is on children’s (16 year olds) subjective experience of attachment within the context of a children’s home. For this reason the research was approached from a qualitative paradigm. Qualitative research is concerned with exploring everyday perceptions of the world and uncovering the experiential, subjective dimensions of people’s worlds (Mouton & Marais, 1996). Inherent in qualitative research are assumptions about the nature of social life, knowledge, research objectives and ways to deal with data. It is on the basis of these assumptions that qualitative research can be distinguished from positivist orientated quantitative research (Mouton & Marais, 1996). Qualitative research is contrary to most of the core assumptions and goals of quantitative research. The goals in qualitative research are defined as describing and understanding (verstehen), rather than explaining and predicting human behaviour as is the case in quantitative research (Babbie & Mouton, 2001). According to Neuman (1997) qualitative research emphasise the importance of the social context for understanding the social world. Implicit in this is the assumption that meaning always occurs within a certain context and can as such not be separated from the context in which it is generated. The meaning of social action or experience is therefore inextricably linked to the context in which it is generated. “When a researcher removes an event, social action, answer to a question, or conversation from the social context in which it appears, or ignores the context, social meaning and significance are distorted” (Neuman, 1997, p.331). Within the context of the present study, the implication will thus be that the attachment experiences of children raised in a children’s home due to abuse perpetrated by 64 their parents would be significantly different from those of children reared by their nonabusive parents because of the specific features that characterises the two contexts. Tools utilised by qualitative researchers therefore differ from traditional quantitative tools such as variables, statistics, hypotheses, etc. Typical qualitative tools include categories, themes and interpretations that are responsible for the rich descriptions that qualitative data yields. It is these descriptions that allow us to generate new insights into our social worlds and allows us to get closer to the understanding of our social realities. They yield rich and colourful detail, which allows us to better understand how people make meaning of their social lives. Hammersly (1996) points out that there is considerable differentiation of theoretical premises and methods within the qualitative tradition. However, despite the fact that there are different forms of qualitative methodologies, what all these share is an emphasis on subjectivity and contextualization of human experience. 5.2 Aims of the study This study aims to explore and develop a better understanding of the nature of attachment patterns of institutionalised adolescents by looking at how attachment abuse, maternal deprivation and institutionalisation can be detrimental to forming close relationships. As mentioned previously, this study is exploratory in nature. Its value can be that of identifying early problematic patterns related to the attachment of unwanted children who have been institutionalised. The study can also direct us towards priorities for future research. It can further be used to inform policy development and formulation in terms of the care and needs of unwanted children. 65 5.3 Sampling The sampling method used in the present study is consistent with the assumptions of qualitative research. According to Kuzel (1992), citing Patton, qualitative sampling is essentially concerned with information-richness. That is because the aim of qualitative research is to understand social life. This is in stark contrast to quantitative research, which is characterised by a preoccupation with generalizability and representativeness (Mouton & Marais, 1996). Qualitative research is driven by the desire to illuminate the questions under investigation and to increase the scope or range of data exposed – to uncover multiple realities, not to explain relationships between variables by reducing them to measurable entities (Mouton & Marais, 1996). This focus on information-richness therefore justifies the use of in-depth relatively small, even single case samples rather than large samples chosen randomly. F or this reason a purposive sampling strategy was employed in the present study. According to Neuman (1997) purposive sampling occurs when an investigator wants to identify particular cases for in-depth study in order to gain a deeper understanding. Sample selection in the present study was therefore based on the extent to which participants could help us understand the phenomenon under investigation and not the extent that we will be able to generalize findings. 5.3.1 Accessing participants Accessing participants for the study involved a number of steps. Initially a list of children’s homes in Johannesburg was drawn up. Contact details of these were acquired through a social worker linked to Childline. The Johannesburg Children’s Home was incidentally the first institution on the list. Contact was made with the director of the Children’s Home. The goals and nature of the research was outlined to her. She was satisfied with the explanations and gave permission for the research to be conducted. At a meeting 66 with one of the social workers at the Children’s Home, the proposed selection criteria were discussed. This enabled the social worker to provide the researcher with the details of children who meet the selection criteria. Meetings were then set up with the care workers of the selected children in order to make arrangements for the data collection phase of the study. 5.3.2 Selection criteria A number of criteria guided the participant selection. A limitation to the selection criteria was the specified age of the participants – the pool chosen from was small. Participant selection was guided by the following criteria: • Participants had to be sixteen years old. • They should have been admitted to institutional care as a measure of protection fr om abusive or neglectful caregivers for a period exceeding two years. • They should have remained in institutional care without interruption of moving back and forth to their parents (caregivers) or temporary foster care for more than one year. 5.3.3 Profile of participants Three 16-year-old participants were purposefully sampled from the Johannesburg Children’s Home. The participants had been in the children’s home the longest. Two participants were female and one male and two were black and one was white. Adolescents were selected essentially at this developmental stage, as they are confronted with the task of identity formation and because they trying to answer the question, “W ho am I, and what is my place in society?” Answers to these questions can only be achieved through an exploration of the social world. Thus during adolescence, the child starts forming their own relationships while they are individuating from childhood spaces. It is the researcher’s belief 67 that this relates to issues of attachment and thus ties in with the aims of the study. The characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 5.1. Table 5.1 Characteristics of participants Participant Age Race Gender Length of stay Language 1 16 Year White Female 4 Years English 2 16 Years Black Male 4 Years Zulu 3 16 Years Black Female 5 Years Tswana 5.3.3.1 Participant One This participant is the eldest child with two younger siblings, a brother and a sister. She had believed that her stepfather was her biological father and was only told at the age of 12 years that he was her stepfather. She never met her biological father and was not told anything about him other than his name. Her two younger siblings are the children of her mother and stepfather and are thus her half-siblings. She has been at the children’s home for four years after she was removed from her family home and was placed at a place of safety due to sexual abuse/ indecent assault allegedly perpetrated by her stepfather from when she was 6 years old until she was 11 years old. She disclosed the abuse to her mother on several occasions and was promised that something would be done about the situation. She was asked by her mother to keep the abuse secret. When nothing was done to remedy the situation, she disclosed the abuse to a trusted teacher at the school she attended at the time. 68 The principal was informed and he in turn called in a social worker that removed the child from her family home that same day. Charges were laid at the Child Protection Unit (CPU) in Johannesburg of indecent assault against the stepfather. The participant was then placed in a place of safety and a few months later was admitted to the children’s home, as she was too old to be adopted and she has a biological family. 5.3.3.2 Participant Two This participant is one of four siblings, and he is the second eldest. His parents got married in December of 1983. The problems started when his father physically abused his mother and also had extra-marital affairs. His mother was requested to leave the family home in June 1996, when the participant was 10 years old. The two younger siblings accompanied her to a woman’s shelter for six months while the two older siblings, the participants and his eldest sister, were left in their father’s care. His parents separated when his mother left for the shelter, after 12 years of marriage. His mother was hospitalised for 3 weeks in 1998 for depression and a month later the participant was admitted to the children’s home because his parents were not in a position to provide for his material and emotional needs. He appeared to be in need of a stable home and family environment. He alleges that his father physically abused his sister and himself while they were in his care. All four children were placed in the children’s home. However, the eldest sibling is now living on her own as she no longer falls in the age category of the children’s home resident. The children still get to see their parents during school holidays and on some weekends. 5.3.3.3 Participant Three This participant was placed along with her older brother in the care of her maternal uncle when she was six years old after it was reported that their mother died in May 1992 in 69 Inkhata battles at a hostel she was living at the time. In 1996 their guardian’s (maternal uncle) neighbour reported that the children were still neglected and without care even though they had been placed in his care, which allegedly resulted in her (participant) being sexually abused by a neighbour’s son on four different occasions. The children were thereafter removed and placed in a place of safety. The place of safety was a temporary arrangement that ended, as she (participant) allegedly was no longer trusted after stealing money and posing additional problems. She was admitted to the children’s home in 1997, as she was too old to be adopted and still has biological family. 5.4 Data collection A number of data collection methods were used. These include a clinical interview conducted with each participant as well as psychometric tests, which were administered on all the participants. The choice of data collection is motivated by its consistency with the philosophical assumptions of the qualitative paradigm. The content that was focused on in the data collection was the children’s subjective experience of attachment. The data collection methods were further motivated by the fact that adolescence is a complex stage in which the individual could, for example, have problems with authority figures. The research topic is also extremely sensitive and some individuals might be able to talk matter -of-factly about issues, while others might hold back. Others might even fantasise about ideal family circumstances and allow these fantasies to take over the narrative that they relate in the interview (Batchelor, 1998). To compensate for this, the researcher decided to include psychometric tests in the data collection because these allow unconscious thoughts and feelings to be projected. The interviewing and testing took place at the Johannesburg Children’s Home, in the children’s respective cottages where they reside, with the permission of the management staff of the home and the children’s respective care workers. All the data 70 collection methods were conducted in English despite the fact that two participants were not English first language speakers. These participants did however display an exceptional level of English competence and had no problem understanding the researcher’s interview questions and the tests’ questions and were able to provide responses to these. 5.4.1 Clinical interview A clinical interview is a method in which the researcher uses a flexible, conversational style to probe for the participant’s point of view (Berk, 1996). The clinical interview has two strengths. First, it permits people to display their thoughts in terms that are as close as possible to the way they think in everyday life. Second, the clinical interview can provide a large amount of information in a fairly brief period of time. A major limitation of the clinical interview however has to do with the accuracy with which people report their thoughts, feelings, and experiences (Berk, 1996). In Appendix 1 at the end of the study, the questions posed in the clinical interview are set out. This process was followed with all three of the participants. The clinical interview took 30 to 45 minutes, depending on the length of the participants’ responses. These interviews were tape -recorded and transcribed. The questions posed in the clinical interview had as its aim, to firstly access how children feel about being placed in the children’s home; and secondly, to gain insight into the nature of their attachment related experiences both in the children’s home and prior to their placement in the home. Most of these children were placed in the children’s home as a result of some form of abuse and/ or neglect. It is argued that the quality and character of relationships from infancy onwards provide powerful clues to the possible origins of current internal working models, behaviours and interpersonal conduct (Howe et al., 1999). Hence the type of caregiving provided will affect children’s personality and attachment styles. 71 5.4.2 Psychometric tests Psychometric tests are one of the most popular data collection methods in psychological research. According to Hammond (1997), there are two main reasons for the popularity of these types of tests. Firstly, psychometric tests have been designed to measure a broad range of mental characteristics therefore providing researchers with a wide variety of measurement tools, which makes a large number of psychological variables accessible for research. A second reason for the popularity of psychometric methods is the relative ease with which it is possible to collect large amounts of data. The researcher chose the above mentioned testing technique because of the reasons expounded by Hammond (1997) and because adolescents tend to be guarded when asked directly about issues. Hammond (1997) however also reminds of the plethora of sub- optimal studies in psychological research literature whose main flaw is the ill-advised use of psychometric methodologies. He argues that the unconvincing nature of these studies has to do with the fact that the interest in the use of psychometric tests is not matched by an equal interest in the technicalities and sophistication of the psychometric principles that underlie their proper use. In addition to this there are also issues pertaining to the psychometric properties of psychometric tests. The fact that psychometric measurement relies on estimation rather than direct measurement, it is difficult to establish reliability and validity of these tests (Hammond, 1997). Encouraging though is the fact that studies carried out by Parker, Hanson and Hunsley (1988) suggest that a number of these psychometric tests show high levels of validity and reliability. There are different types of psychometric tests that can be distinguished on the basis of their data elicitation strategy. The only type of psychometric tests employed in the present study is projective tests. 72 5.4.3 Projective tests Projective tests are specific types of psychometric tests that have in common the utilisation of ambiguous stimuli or tasks designed to provide a wide range of responses from examinees (Hammond, 1997). They are intended to be depth orientated and focused on the unconscious, covert characteristics of personality. In other words, they are designed to produce indirect measures of an individual’s mental state (Hammond, 1997). Projective tests are based on the assumption that the more unstructured the stimuli, the more the examinees reveal about their personality. Thus the use of ambiguous stimuli eliminates or greatly reduces defensiveness and other conscious attempts to distort test results (Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). Two types of projective tests were used in the study, that is, those that focus on drawings and those that focus on story telling. 5.4.3.1 Projective drawing tests Projective drawings are expressive techniques in that they suggest aspects of the person while he is performing some activity (Groth-Marnat, 1997). According to Beek (in Lezak, 1995), each person perceives external stimuli through a reflection of his attitudes, understandings, and perceptual and response tendencies, and interprets the compounded percept as external reality. In the past, interpretation of drawings (and projective testing in general) drew heavily on psychoanalytic theory (Groth-Marnat, 1997). One of the central assumptions of this procedure is that, because many important aspects of personality are not available to conscious self -report, questionnaires and inventories are of limited value (GrothMarnat, 1997). To obtain an accurate view of a person’s inner world, one must somehow circumvent unconscious defences and conscious resistances. From a psychoanalytical perspective, then, an indirect approach, such as through projective drawing, is essential (Groth-Marnat, 1997). Through symbolical creation, an individual depicts important themes, 73 dynamics, and attitude s. These idiosyncratic expressions of inner dynamics are most likely to occur when the person draws something on a blank sheet of paper (Groth-Marnat, 1997). With regard to attachment, it is argued that children’s family drawings purportedly examine self-concepts and perceptions of interpersonal relations within families (Pianta & Longmaid, 1999). According to Bowlby (1969), individuals develop ‘representational models’ or ‘internal working models’ of the properties, characteristics, and behaviours of attac hment figures, the self, and their relationship. The individual uses these models to evaluate and guide behaviour in new situations and relationships. The quality of these representational models of attachment relationships within the family is related to the caregiver’s sensitivity and responsivity, consistent availability, emotional acceptance, and ability to serve as a secure base for exploration (Pianta & Longmaid, 1999). Representations consistent with observed attachment security are marked by a view of the self as competent and deserving of care and that family relationships will be rewarding and fulfilling. In contrast, representations consistent with observed insecurity contain views of others as rejecting, hostile, or unavailable and of the self as lacking competence and being unworthy of others’ affection (Pianta & Longmaid, 1999). In order to gain insight into the attachment patterns of the participants, the Draw -a-Person Test (DAP) and the Kinetic Family Drawing Test (KFD) were used. The motivation for using these two tests is largely due to the researcher’s belief that they supplement each other. a) The Draw -a-Person Test (DAP) The Draw -a-Person Test (DAP) grew out of the work of Karen Machover (1949). Although originally developed to assess children’s intelligence, its use was extended to assess personality and diagnose psychopathology. The basic hypothesis underlying the development of the DAP was that examinees project into human figures that they are asked to draw. These 74 reproductions are believed to reveal their impulses, anxieties, and other internal emotional states (Groth-Marnat, 1997; Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). The DAP consists of having examinees draw two human figures (Machover, 1949). Firstly they are instructed to simply draw a person and, following completion of the task, they are told to draw a person of the opposite sex from that of the first drawing. While the examinee goes about these tasks, the clinician observes such things as the sequence of body parts drawn, verbalisations made by the examinee, and other factors associated with the actual drawing process. Occasionally, a period of questioning occurs after both drawings have been completed. During the inquiry, information is elicited about the figures, such as age, occupation, and family relationships (Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). b) The Kinetic Family Drawing Test (KFD) The DAP focus on individual dynamics. However, many persons involved in assessment and therapy have been interested in understanding the structure of and relationships within families. Family drawings provide a less threatening means of revealing this information than direct questioning. Family drawings were originally suggested by Appel (1931) and Wolff (1942), but were later more fully developed as the Draw-A-Family Test (DAF) by Hulse (1951) and Harris (1963). A more recent and quite popular variation of the DAF is Burns and Kaufman’s Kinetic Family Drawing (KFD). The authors have criticised the DAF as producing fairly rigid, low-energy, noninteractive drawings, and have attempted to correct this result by requesting that the examinee draw his family “doing something”. These instructions are usually supplemented by asking the examinee to include himself in the drawing. After completing the drawing, the examinee is requested to describe it and/ or tell a story about it. This aids the interpreter by clarifying the interactions, characters, and underlying message of the drawing (Groth-Marnat, 1997). 75 5.4.3.2 Projective story-telling tests In infancy, the only way to try to understand how children are representing the availability of the mother and the worthiness of the self (internal working model of attachment) is to observe their behaviour in various low -stress situations (Senior, 2002). With older children it becomes pos sible to examine internal working models using measures that seek to elicit more directly ways in which the individual is mentally representing the self and the other (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999). A common technique used in early and middle childhood is to get children to ‘project’ themselves into a story in which there are attachmentrelated events. The stories may be facilitated by the use of dolls, pictures or opportunities to draw (Howe et al., 1999). Differences in the content and process of the stories indicate the way in which the child typically handles anxiety and distress. In turn, these defensive methods of dealing with anxiety provide information about the child’s internal working model of attachment. Secure children develop stories in which fears and separation anxieties are deliberately raised before finally being resolved, usually by the return of responsive, loving parents (Howe et al., 1999). Avoidant and ambivalent children tend not to let their attachment behavioural systems become fully activated. The stories they develop do not allow anxieties and fears to threaten safety and security (Howe et al., 1999). Avoidant-defended children tell matter-of-fact stories that appear casual and nonchalant. Nothing out of the ordinary seems to happen. The attachment system is immobilised. Distressing possibilities are defensively excluded (Howe et al., 1999). Ambivalent-dependent children tell busy, happy stories about what children do in their parents’ absence. Brief ‘sentimental laments’ are often introduced about how much parents are loved (Howe et al., 1999). Attachment anxieties are dealt with by a form of defensive ‘splitting’ – the bad, distressing bits are temporarily ignored or denied, 76 while the good bits are exaggerated. Cognitive disconnection separates positive and negative information, attention being diverted to positive events. It seems difficult for ambivalent children to integrate and concurrently acknowledge both anxious and positive feelings. They can only deal with one emotional perspective at a time. Things are either all wonderful and good, or all terrible and bad (Howe et al., 1999). Disorganised-controlling children introduce fear and danger into their stories but seem to have no way of resolving the growing crises. Parents remain physically and emotionally unavailable or even abusive. Chaos and helplessness mount, the self and the family disintegrate, and stories are ended, often abruptly, with no resolution (Howe et al., 1999). In the present study, the Sentence Completion Test and the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) were used. a) The Sentence Completion Test The incomplete-sentence method of assessing personality has its roots in the work of Ebbinghaus around the beginning of the 20th century (Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). As is the case with other projective methods of personality assessment, the underlying assumption is that examinees project their wishes, desires, fears, and attitudes into sentences that they create. While examinees responding to tests like the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) are rarely aware of what they are revealing about themselves, due in part to the ambiguous nature of the test items, examinees responding to the Sentence Completion Test are generally more aware of the feelings, attitudes and fears that they are disclosing because the task is more structured (Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). b) The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) Henry Murray (1938) developed another major projective instrument using pictures rather than inkblots. The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) consists of 31 picture cards that 77 provide stimuli for examinees to create stories concerning relationships or social situations suggested by the pictures (Groth-Marnat, 1997; Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). Murray postulated that the examinees would project into their stories information concerning their needs, emotions, conflicts, attitudes, and emotional difficulties. Particular attention is paid to the protagonist of each story and the environmental stresses that impinge on him. The hero is presumed to represent the examinee (Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). 5.4.4 Reliability and validity of the projective tests used The consistency of test scores is of considerable importance in evaluating a test as a measurement instrument. A reliable test is one that yield consistent scores when a person takes two alternative forms of the test or when he takes the same test on two or more different occasions (Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). Validity is the degree to which inferences made on the basis of test scores are correct (Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). The responses examinees give to the TAT and the Sentence Completion Test involve complex, meaningful verbal material. Exact quantitative analysis is difficult because of the complexity of this material, and interpretations are typically based more on a qualitative than a quantitative analysis of story content. Most methods of determining reliability therefore become problematic (Groth-Marnat, 1997). A further complication is the fact that, in actual practice, clinicians rely primarily on intuitive clinical judgment, use different sets of instructions, and vary the number, type, and sequence of cards from one examinee to the next. Thus, reliability in clinical contexts is likely to be considerably lower than under experimental conditions. Another difficulty in determining reliability lies in the wide variability among different stories (Groth-Marnat, 1997). Review of the TAT’s validity has shown wide variability. This variability can be partially accounted for by differing 78 interpretations of the data and partially influenced by slight variations in instructions (GrothMarnat, 1997). Notwithstanding these complications, there seems to be sufficient evidence that the reliability of the TAT and the Sentence Completion Test is fairly satisfactory with both having an acceptable degree of inter-rater reliability (Chandler & Johnson, 1991). There are conflicting opinions regarding the validity of the TAT, while the Sentence Completion Test has a high degree of face-validity although it is regarded as the projective test most vulnerable to faking (Mortensen, 1991). Establishing the reliability and validity of projective drawings present special challenges, given the fluctuating conditions between one administration and the next; the underlying, often difficult-to-prove (or disprove) assumptions behind the procedure; and the frequent richness and complexity of the productions (Vass, 1999). Several rigorous analyses of the psychometric properties of projective drawings have ge nerally failed to demonstrate that the drawings are valid indicators of personality (Groth-Marnat, 1997). However, projective tests yield invaluable information that is sometimes absent when using other tests. Kaplan and Main (1985) were the first to suggest that children’s drawings might be a fruitful way of capturing attachment representations. The nonverbal nature of drawings may free the child to express emotions and attitudes that are otherwise difficult to assess. There has been considerable support for the use of drawings as a valuable method for assessing children’s attitudes, needs, and conflicts (Fury, Carlson, & Sroufe, 1997). According to Bowlby (1973), drawing is a natural mode of expression. Long before youngsters can put their feelings and thoughts into words, they can express both conscious and unconscious attitudes, wishes, and concerns in drawings. Drawing is a non-verbal language, a means of communication. It thus seems plausible that representations of attachment experiences would be revealed in 79 drawings, and specifically, that the child’s inner working models of self, caregivers, and self with caregivers would be manifest (Fury et al., 1997). 5.5 Data analysis 5.5.1 Analysis of the interview data and projective story telling tests Interview data was tape recorded and transcribed verbatim. Both the data from the clinical interviews, the Sentence Completion Test and the Thematic Apperception Test were subjected to a thematic content analytical process. In order to increase the reliability of the data, the researcher as well as another researcher conducted separate content analyses of each of the three data sources. Themes generated were compared and only those on which agreement was reached were included in the results of the study. The underlying aim of the content analytical approach was to elicit information that will contribute to the understanding of the development of attachment patterns of children in children’s homes. This would be facilitated by the richness of the data gathered by means of these methods. These methods yielded words and sentences which constituted the units of analyses and thus lend themselves to a content analytical method. Content analysis can be qualitative or quantitative. This study utilised a qualitative or thematic content analytical approach. According to Holsti (in Babbie & Mouton, 2001), the advantage of this approach is that it focuses on the manifested as well as latent content. Thematic content analysis, also referred to as conceptual content analysis by Palmquist (in Babbie & Mouton, 2001), is the term used to describe a more clearly interpretative application of the method in which the focus of analysis is upon thematic content which is identified, categorised and elaborated upon on the basis of systematic scrutiny (Eagle, 1998). Thematic content analysis tends to be more subjective and emphasise meaning rather than quantification. 80 A ‘thematic analysis’ is a coherent way of organising or reading some interview material in relation to specific research questions. According to Strauss, (in Eagle, 1998) the categories researchers use in a content analysis can be determined inductively, deductively or by some combination of both. Abrahamson (in Eagle, 1998) indicates that an inductive approach begins with the researcher’s ‘immersing’ themselves in the documents (i.e. the various messages) in order to identify the dimensions or themes that seem meaningful to the producers of each message. In a deductive approach, researchers use some categorical theme suggested by a theoretical perspective, and the documents provide a means for assessing the hypothesis. In many circumstances, the relationship between a theoretical perspective and certain messages involves both inductive and deductive approaches. Thematic content analysis assumes a relation between the text and the author of the text or its audience; based in a realist or structuralist understanding of the world (Eagle, 1998). Thus, it is assumed that inferences about inner states, intentions and cognitions, for exam ple, can be made from the words or actions that people produce (Eagle, 1998). The researcher used both the inductive and deductive approaches in order to generate as many themes as possible that would add to the richness of the data collected and that would also illuminate the aims of the study. 5.5.2 Analysis of the KFD The KFD was analysed using Kaplan and Main’s (1986) classification system for children’s family drawings. Kaplan and Main (1986) suggested that attachment organisation is reflected in representations of family relationships in children’s family drawings through eight dimensions or constructs: degree of movement present in family figures, individuation of figures, completeness of human form, quality of smiles, size of figures, centeredness on the page, overall impression of invulnerability, and overall impression of vulnerability (Fury et al., 1997; Pianta & Longmaid, 1999). 81 As described by Kaplan and Main (1986), the overall impression of family drawings made by children classified as having secure representations is that the family (or the child) portrayed in the drawing is essentially happy. These drawings contain realistic elements and depict humans, often interacting with one another. The image projected by the child in the drawing is one of comfort with others and realism. The overall impression characteristic of drawings classified as reflecting insecure avoidant representations is a strong emphasis on invulnerability. These drawings depict an image of unrealistic happiness, such as when all figures have the same very large and brightly coloured smile and often a lack of individuation, such as when all are drawn the same size, shape, or with the same clothing. Family members are not depicted interacting with one another. Drawings characteristic of insecure-ambivalent or resistant representations display an emphasis on vulnerability. In these drawings of families, children may appear alone, isolated from the group, or looking worried and fearful (Fury at al., 1997; Pianta & Longmaid, 1999). Kaplan and Main state that drawings reflecting disorganised representations tend to have ominous or foreboding elements such as dark clouds or blood, the child (or the human figures) may be missing from the drawing, or the drawing may be uninterpretable (Fury at al., 1997; Pianta & Longmaid, 1999). According to Kaplan and Main (1986), disorganised drawings include the following features of disorganisation: unfinished objects or figures; scratched out figures or restarts; overall impression of vulnerability, and to a lesser extent overbright excessive sweetness. Drawings classified as secure and avoidant include the following features: essentially happy, solid, centred, and realistic. Drawings classified as ambivalent include features of vulnerability, extreme proximity of figures, unpleasant exaggeration of facial features, and extremely tiny or huge figures (Fury at al., 1997; Pianta & Longmaid, 1999). 82 5.5.3 Analysis of the DAP Scoring and interpreting the DAP are complex, subjective tasks. Machover (1949) outlined a number of structural and formal aspects of the drawings that are important in the interpretation process. These include such aspects as the size and placement of the figures on the paper, the amount of action depicted in the drawings, the systematisation followed in doing the task, erasures, shading, and differential treatment of male and female figures. Attention is also given to specific body parts (Murphy & Davidshofer, 2001). The DAP tests were analysed qualitatively, using various literature on interpreting human figure drawings. No quantitative analyses were used. 5.6 Ethics Ethical concerns were addressed throughout the course of the study. Because participants were minors in the care of a children’s home, permission for the research was obtained from the relevant authorities, i.e. the governing body of the home, which also included the director of the home as well as the care workers, and from the children themselves. Participants were informed about the nature and purpose of the study in order to get their informed consent. According to Kvale (1996), informed consent entails informing the research participants about the overall purpose of the investigation and the main features of the research design, as well as any possible risks and benefits that might result from participation in the research project. Informed consent further requires obtaining voluntary participation of the subject, with the full understanding that he may withdraw from the study at any given time. This is aimed at counteracting undue influences and potential coercion (Kvale, 1996). The participants were assured that their participation was voluntary and that they could terminate their participation at any time if they so wish. The well being of the participants was of central importance, especially given the sensitive nature of the research. 83 Therefore every effort was made to ensure that the well being of the participants was respected at all times. For instance, the children were tested in the environment they were familiar with, the children’s home. In cases where participants were unable to be interviewed and tested on a specific date, arrangements were made for them to be interviewed and tested at a later date. Participants, who were too emotional during the interviewing, were given the option of terminating the interview and to resume on a later date if they still wished to participate in the study. Participants who appeared to be adversely affected by their participation were referred to a mental health professional(psychologist or social worker) at the children’s home for support and containment and follow -up sessions at the children’s home if necessary. The relevant information was then obtained from the children’s background histories (case files). Assurances wer e given that information obtained will remain strictly confidential and the anonymity of all participants will be observed. Confidentiality means that any private data that may compromise the participants’ identity will be removed from the research report (Kvale, 1996). A brief feedback session will be held with the governing body as well as the children’s care workers at the children’s home within a few months after the thesis is approved. The children will then be informed by the social worker at the home of the results of the study after it has been discussed with her at the feedback session held. 5.7 Conclusion In this chapter a detailed summary is given on the methodology the researcher followed and the epistemology used. The following chapter discusses the results of the study yielded with the use of clinical interviews and the psychometric tests performed with the three participants. 84 CHAPTER 6 RESULTS 6.1 Introduction This chapter presents the results of the study. The results for each participant will be presented separately and will be organised as follows: the results for each data source will first be presented. Emphasis will be placed not only on the attachment related themes generated from each of the data sources, but also themes that could possibly provide insight into the current attachment patterns. In the results of the interview data and the story tellingbased projective test (the Sentence Completion Test), the researcher will use the verbatim words of the participant in order to highlight how a specific attachment-related theme is represented. The individual presentation of each data source results will be followed by an integration of the results from the different data sources in order to enhance the depth and complexity of insight that can be deduced from the results. This will be followed by the presentation of a table, which details the different themes generated from each data source for the particular participant. At the end of this, a brief summary of the results for the participant is provided. 6.2 Participant One 6.2.1 The clinical interview The clinical interview for Participant One was dominated by discussions of her family and their treatment of her. The data revealed that the participant experienced a sense of abandonment and rejection by her family. She felt she was of lesser importance to her mother than were her siblings and that her mother treated them favourably. She appears to believe that their preferential treatment stems from the fact that she and her siblings did not share the 85 same biological father. She also felt that her mother aligned herself with her husband and their children and that she was excluded from the family as a result. She was always made to feel different from everyone else in the family and she believed that she was victimised because of her father. “If it wasn’t for these people, my brother and sister and I wouldn’t have family cause they’ve all, like, just thrown me in the gutter because of my dad”. “It’s always been just like difficult cause she always ch ose her other two kids over me. Whenever there was a fight she would turn around and say, ‘You just like your father’. She always said that. She would never say that about her other kids”. The participant verbalised intense anger and hatred towards her mother, with whom she cannot get along. All of these feelings stem from feeling betrayed by her mother’s silence and lack of support after her disclosure of abuse perpetrated by her stepfather. She was forced to leave her home due to her mother not protectin g her. “My mother and I don’t actually get along, we worst of enemies. I hate my mother because she went and took her husband’s side. The reason why I’m in the home, I was actually sexually abused by him”. She had a strained and distant relationship with her family because she isolated herself from them as a result of the abuse and subsequent abandonment and rejection she experienced. She did not feel part of the family due to the fact that her siblings share the same mother and father while she has a dif ferent father. “I also isolate myself from her and her kids. I know they half brother and sister, but I don’t…Because I hate her and her husband so much, I don’t take them as my own even though they did nothing to me, but it’s their blood, you know. I isolated myself from those four people”. In terms of her current attachment patterns, the participant has formed close bonds with the male care worker in charge of her cottage at the children’s home due to his availability and proximity to her. “This lady’s husband, you know, I take him as a father figure, and Sean (housefather) also, cause I’m here most of the time than what I am there”. 86 The participant also has a host family that she visits on weekends. She is close to her host parents and their children, although she did not get along with their 18-year old son in the beginning. Her views seem unrealistic, as she appears to idolise her host parents’ relationship and their family context. This family represents her ideal view of what a family is suppose to be like and contrasts directly with her own family. “They like foster parents, but they not. We actually have hosts and volunteers and things, you know, and they my hosts. Other people would say ‘I’m going to my hosts this weekend’. I would not say that. I feel closer to them, so I’ll say ‘I’m going to my mom for the weekend’. They have a son who is 18 and a daughter of 12”. “Well, with their son I used to fight a lot. That was last year, but this year we getting along very well as if we like becoming brother and sister for real. We are getting that bond. And my sister, it’s fine. We close together. We do everything together. I can tell her anything. She won’t blurt it out to my mother”. The participant also seemed to have unrealistic feelings towards her biological father whom she has never met. This might explain her attachment towards her housefather and her host father due in part to their positive relationship with her, which is in stark contrast to her relationship with her stepfather. “… I’m a daddy’s little girl, although I don’t know my father I love him more”. Although the participant appeared to be securely attached to her host family, this is not the case of her attachment experiences in the children’s home. She has not attached to anyone in the home apart from her housefather. She feels that they are not listened to in the home and thus isolates herself. This is similar to the experience she had with her mother. “I actually find it very difficult. I actually hate this place. I can’t stand it. Sometimes I feel like it’s not a children’s home, it’s like an adults home. They listen more to house parents than they do to children. I actually feel like I’m being marginalized”. 6.2.2 The Kinetic Family Drawing Test (KFD) According to Kaplan and Main’s classification system, Participant One’s Kinetic Family Drawing (KFD) indicated an emphasis on invulnerability, as opposed to an emphasis on vulnerability, where the child appears alone, isolated from the group and looking worried 87 and fearful. Her drawing depicted an image of unrealistic happiness, as all the figures in the drawing had the same large and brightly coloured smile. There was a lack of individuation in the drawing, as all the figures in the drawing were drawn the same size, shape and w ith the same clothing, with no indication of gender. According to Kaplan and Main’s classification system, Participant One displays insecure -avoidant attachment. 6.2.3 The Draw-a-Person Test (DAP) Participant One’s drawing indicated that she feels insecure and rigid and this rigidity is present in her interpersonal relationships. She has a poor self-concept and feels inferior. She feels inadequate and tends to be aggressive and acts out to compensate for these feelings. 6.2.4 The Sentence Completion Test It was evident from this test that she has strong resentment feelings towards her mother and feels her mother is unreliable. Her experiences with her mother have also led her to believe that her mother’s actions are typical of those of all mothers. “Most mothers dump their kids in gutters”. “My mother always disapproves of my actions”. “I wish that my mother weren’t my mother”. Perhaps, as a result of feeling rejected by her mother, she tends to idealise her biological father and wishes she had a relationship with him, possibly in the hope of undoing the damage she experienced in her family. Her general view about fathers is more positive and contrast strongly with her view about mothers. She thus believes that fathers love their children much more than mothers do. “Most fathers love their kids”. “My greatest wish is to be with my dad before I die”. “I wish that my father could be here with me”. Most of the responses on the test indicated that Participant One is angry and irritable and can easily loose control if her demands are not met. Because her needs were never catered for and 88 her demands were never met in her family, she regards acting out as the only means of getting attention and getting those needs met. “Taking orders makes me very angry”. “Mos t girls irritate me”. “If I can’t get what I want, I go mad”. “I lose my temper very easily”. “I could hate a person who gets on my nerves”. The overall themes present in Participant One’s responses to the Sentence Completion Test are those of rejection and feeling despondent about her situation. “I often wish I could leave the world”. “I feel like giving up when I lose my selfesteem”. “I feel like running away when people push me aside and make my life difficult for me”. 6.2.5 The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) The responses Participant One gave in the test indicated that she feels unsupported and feels that she has to manage on her own. However, when she feels unable to cope independently she gives up on the task at hand. She abandons the task because it is not realising her expectations. She feels ignored and isolated by her parents and perceives other people as easily losing interest in her. She feels unsupported and exploited by her mother. She perceives her siblings as more important than she is in the family, and that they are chosen over her. She feels pushed by her mother into a life that she has no control over. She feels abandoned and burdened. 6.2.6 Integration of data When the results from the different data sources are integrated, one gets a se nse of not only clear patterns and relationships among attachment oriented themes across different data sources, but also nuanced insights into the attachment experiences of the participant. The fact that the participant had negative attachment experiences is pervasive throughout the different data sources, but is particularly accentuated in the clinical interview, the Sentence Completion Test, and the TAT. She has experienced a profound sense of rejection, 89 abandonment, isolation, exploitation and betrayal within her family. This has caused her to feel intense anger, hatred, aggression and resentment primarily towards her mother whom she feels did not provide her with the security and love that she needed and who chose to look the other way while the participant suffered abuse at the hands of her stepfather. She perceives her mother as unreliable and feels burdened by the fact that she cannot rely on her. This was proved repeatedly when their mother would treat her siblings favourably. She feels that she has been victimised because of her father and that this is another reason for the favouritism her mother has shown towards her siblings. Her mother was never able to take care of her as she was supposed to. The participant was therefore left vulnerable and bur dened with responsibilities that someone her age should not have been concerned with. All these negative experiences with her mother have profoundly shaped her views regarding mothers and fathers in general as is evident from the Sentence Completion Test. This further explains why her anger and resentment seems to be more directed towards her mother than towards her stepfather who had abused her. It also explains her propensity towards forming bonds with males as demonstrated by her bonds with her host father and male care worker in the children’s home as well as her need to be close to her biological father. Her negative family experiences provides some possible insight into the results of the KFD, which indicates a yearning for a better family in which her need for love, acceptance, nurturance and protection are met. After getting clear instructions on the KFD test, the participant drew her host family instead of her biological family. Her host family is everything that her biological family is not and represents for her the ideal family that she strives to be a member of. It is a family where she has unconditional love, acceptance and care. It is a family where she is allowed to be the child that she is, unburdened by adult 90 responsibilities and safe from the fear and vulnerability of being exploited and abused. She thus drew her host parents and their two children. She however failed to include herself in the drawing. Thus despite her strong desire to be part of her host family, it also appears that she does not want to contaminate the ideal family portrait by including herself. Her history of neglect and experiences of sexual abuse in her own family makes it difficult, if not impossible, for her to think of herself as worthy to be part of this ideal family – the host family. The KFD drawing also depicts an image of unrealistic happiness, as proposed by Kaplan and Main’s classification system. It is unrealistic because of the extreme manifestation of happiness in the drawing. This could be linked to the abuse she suffered in her own family as well as to her perceptions of what the ideal family looks like. The clinical interview also support this, wherein she indicates that her needs were secondary to others needs within her family. This is supported by t he results of the DAP test that indicates that she has a poor self-concept and feels inferior to others. She appears to feel angry with her own family for corrupting her and making her so unlovable, unworthy and unwanted and making it nearly impossible for her to be thought of as lovable and wanted by others. It is though her family has deprived her of experiencing a normal, loving family life and is still now trying to deprive her of experiencing this normality in her host family. A major issue within her family other than the abuse is the fact that her being different from her siblings were always pointed out by instances in which they would get preferential treatment and she would be compared to them. There appeared to be issues of sibling rivalry present in the family. This sibling rivalry was initially carried over to her host family whereby it was difficult then to get along with their 18-year-old son in particular. However, after the adjustment phase within the host family and the host parents treating all the children, participant included, equally, the sibling rivalry dissipated. All this is indicated in the clinical interview. 91 In the DAP test the participant drew a picture of a man. This indicates that she is struggling with her sensuality, given her past experience of sexual abuse. As a defence, she is denying her femininity, which could attract unwanted sexual attention. It appears that she wishes that she were physically stronger in order to protect herself. She is also trying to protect herself by repressing her femininity. This also seems to emerge in the KFD test where there is a lack of individuation of the figures in terms of gender. The family is portrayed in non-sexual terms, as if to safeguard them from contamination and to preserve their idealised status. Further evidence of defensiveness is provided by results from the clinical interview in which the participant states her unwillingness to form bonds with anyone in the children’s home and prefers to isolate herself. There is thus a contradiction between her need for belonging and closeness on the one hand, and her determination to protect herself from future abuse on the other. Evidence consistent with the above is clear from other data sources as well. For example, the participant failed to answer two questions on the Sentence Completion Test, which also indicates that she wants to protect herself from past experiences of abuse. Most of the answers on the test indicate that she blames herself for the abuse and that she has a very negative view of the dynamics of relationships to the exclusion of positive relationship components. The only positive relational dynamic present in her answers is the idealised nonexistent relationship between herself and her absent, nonabusive biological father, whom she knows nothing about, but who could be someone who is lovable and in turn makes her feel loved. The answers the participant gave on the TAT indicate that she longs for a long lost love and she fears that her feelings will never be reciprocated. This is supported by her answers on the Sentence Completion Test where she longs to be with her biological father she has never known, who could have been a positive attachment figure in her life, unlike 92 what she has experienced in her family thus far. There are strong feelings of isolation present in the TAT. She is afraid that there is no sustained interest in her as a person, not by her parents or any body else. She views her interactions with others in a very negative way and she is normally the one that loose out. She does not trust herself with any decision-making, as she fears she might make the wrong decision. She desires to be rescued by someone and feel loved and accepted as she is. Table 6.1 Attachment-related themes generated for Participant One DATA SOURCE Clinical Interview Kinetic Family Drawing Test THEMES Idealisation, Abandonment, Anger, Hatred, Isolation, Rejection, Betrayal Invulnerability, Unrealistic happiness, Lack of individuation, Insecure-avoidant attachment Draw–a-Person Test Insecurity, Rigidity, Inadequacy, Poor selfconcept, Aggression Sentence Completion Test Unreliability, Resentment, Rejection, Anger, Irritability Thematic Apperception Test Lack of support, Isolation, Exploitation, Abandonment, Burdened 6.2.7 Summary of the results for Participant One There seems to be consistency between the participant’s past attachment patterns as revealed by her background information and her current attachment patterns as revealed by the research data. It also appears as if her past has impacted on her current attachment 93 patterns, which can be described as insecure in terms of the research results. The situation in the children’s home seems to reinforce these patterns. She tends to be wary and not to commit herself fully in relationships with other people, even though it is her greatest need to be loved and accepted by others. This seems to be supported by her attachment towards her housefather and her host family. She tends to keep her distance in relationships in the fear that she might be hurt and betrayed again. 6.3 Participant Two 6.3.1 The clinical interview When describing his current relationship with his parents, Participant Two implied that his relationship with his mother is positive. It also appeared as if his rela tionship with his father is improving although it was not good in the past. His past family life used to be characterised by domestic violence and abuse primarily as a result of his father’s alcoholism. “Well, mostly we used to go to our mother, visit our mother, so they used to fight a lot. We didn’t really clutch with my father, but now since last year we started seeing him... When I was young it was quite hectic with my father. He used to pay for school fees. He took us through school, but then he hit us, and stuff like that, accused us of stealing. Those days he used to be an alcoholic, he used to drink a lot”. Participant Two appeared shy and introverted and because of this he seemed inclined to limit his relationships with peers and adults. He has few close friends outside the children’s home, but has not formed any attachments with the adults or his peers in the children’s home. His attitude towards relationships seems avoidant and disinterested because of his shy demeanour and his past experiences within his family. “I have one best friend at school. We do everything together at school…I don’t have a mother or father figure at the home. I also don’t have host parents”. 94 He is close to his younger siblings who are also in the children’s home. He regards them as his responsibility. He feels he has to look out for them and provide for them and he believes that being in the children’s home would facilitate this ideal. “I think the children’s home is helpful. It’s helpful cause, I mean, I get to do my schoolwork and do sports at school and be able to go to university…I want to be able to provide for my family”. 6.3.2 The Kinetic Family Drawing Test (KFD) Participant Two’s drawing had all the elements indicative of a secure attachment. The family is portrayed as essential happy, comfortable and realistic and the figures are interacting with each other. This is in stark contrast to the way the family is portrayed in the clinical interview. This might however have a lot to do with the fact that he is artistically inclined and may have drawn a picture of an idealised family, as many family portraits are normally portrayed. 6.3.3 The Draw-a-Person Test (DAP) Participant Two’s drawing indicated that he has aggressive tendencies as a result of feeling inadequa te and anxious. However, he has a need to control these aggressive tendencies. He has a poor self-concept and feels inferior, especially physically. He has high achievement aspirations. He is emotionally and socially poorly adjusted. 6.3.4 The Sentence Completion Test From his responses to the test Participant Two appeared to feel like he is not ‘good enough’, that he is a failure. These feelings stem from the failure of his family life and the fear that this failure might spill into other spheres of his life, particularly his academic life and have some disastrous outcome, which might influence his future. He uses the academic success of his classmates as a goal to strive towards. 95 “My greatest fear is failing”. “My classmates are good academics”. His fear of failure causes him to pressurise himself to do his utmost. He regards academic success as the key to ultimately living a good life. He sees pushing himself as the only way to overcome his odds and move on in life. “I am afraid of ending up, being a nobody”. “I’d like most to be a student with a degree and a sportsman”. “My greatest wish is to make it in life”. “I often wish I could finish school”. There was an indication that Participant Two feels disappointed and unsupported by his father and wishe s things were different. “My father always used to drink”. “I wish that my father would change his ways”. He appeared to experience his mother as nurturing, loving and caring and feels that mothers are generally good to their children. However, he worries about her security needs and also expresses indirect security concerns about his siblings and himself. “Most mothers care for their babies or children”. “My mother always takes care of me”. “I wish that my mother would find a permanent job”. 6.3.5 The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) Participant Two’s responses to the test were very guarded. His responses were descriptive in nature and were very short, even after clear instructions were given and supplementary questions were asked to get an elaboration on the answers given. These responses indicated that he feels abandoned by his family and wishes to be re-united with them. He feels he has to manage on his own, but wishes there was someone available that could be supportive of him. 6.3.6 Integration of data Although Participant Two came across as extremely guarded the integration of the results of his tests reveals some interesting attachment related dynamics. There appears to be 96 a strong focus on the attachment relationship between the participant and his father and to a lesser degree his mother, and all the data sources point towards this. His father was not his initial primary caregiver but later on when he was left in his father’s care did he become his primary caregiver. It appears that the negative relational dynamic between himself and his father has had the greatest impact on him. The participant’s KFD appears to be of a close and happy family, comfortably interacting with one another. However, his father is not included in the drawing. Present in the drawing is his mother, his siblings and himself. It appears that the participant thinks of his ideal family as consisting only of the members included in the drawing, as there is no history of any serious conflict between them and they appear to be supportive of each other. He regards his father as the only source of conflict in the family. His father is excluded and distanced from the family possibly because of the trouble he has caused the family in the past and his negative interactions with each family member, that is, causing the family’s break-up by physically abusing his wife and children and not showing interest in the family by engaging in affairs outside the home. He appears to perceive his father as disruptive to the family as a result of his excessive drinking bouts. His father’s exclusion from the drawing can therefore be said to stem from his inability to fulfil his responsibilities towards the family. He is thus regarded as unworthy of being part of the family. Insights similar to that deduced from the KFD is also reflected in the DAP test of the participant. The DAP test drawing is of a professional, well dressed, older man. It appears that he wishes he were older and thus more responsible than what he currently is. This figure is also in stark contrast to his perceptions of his father as useless and unworthy and could represent the person that he believes his father is suppose to be or that he wants him to be. He wants to be able to provide for his own and his family’s security needs, unlike his parents and his father in particular, who were not able to provide for his material and emotional needs 97 when he was growing up. He however feels inadequate because his parents never provided an example on which to model his behaviour on. He also feels inferior and anxious for not being in a position and not knowing how to provide for his family, as his father cannot be counted on to fulfil this function. Because he is the eldest male child of the family, he expects of himself to fulfil the role of the provider in the family since his father is absent in this capacity. He lacks confidence in himself and his abilities. He wishes he were a stronger person, physically as well as emotionally in order to overcome his past adversities and provide for a brighter future for himself as well as his mother and siblings. Other tests also seem to point to the fact that expectations placed upon him by his situation and by himself outweigh his potential to live up to those expectations. In the analysis of the Sentence Completion Test it appears that he is expected to give more than he is able to give at the moment. This pressure comes more from him than from others with the exception of his mother, whom he perceives as overwhelmed and weak and who he has to support, financially as well as emotionally. He appears to be preoccupied with success, as he regards this as the only way to overcome his previous disadvantages and secure an optimistic future. This is evident in the DAP test wherein it appears that he feels inadequate and feels the need to compensate for this and hopefully prevail over this perceived inadequacy. He also expresses his negative feelings towards his father’s past conduct. This is also deduced from both the KFD and DAP tests. He however hopes that his father has changed his ways and is able to fulfil his role as caregiver and provider, so that he can return to being a child and not concern himself with the financial and emotional well being of the family. His responses in the TAT, though extremely short, indicate his fear of coping independently. From his responses to the Sentence Completion Test it is evident that he is a follower rather than a leader and is easily influenced by others. He has strong feelings of 98 helplessness since he is forced to mature bef ore his time. He wishes he had support from significant others in his life and that everything was not left up to him. Though the KFD appears to be of a secure family, it is inconsistent with the results of the other data sources. The drawing is the ideal family that he wishes he had. He wishes that his family could be re united in order to be that family portrayed in his drawing. The fact that he is artistically inclined creates the illusion in his drawings of happiness and contentment and masks what is really lacking. His responses to the storytelling-based tests however give a clearer picture, though not a lot, of what he is currently experiencing emotionally. His shy demeanour contributes to this as well and that makes him more inaccessible. Table 6.2 Attachment-related themes generated for Participant Two DATA SOURCE THEMES Clinical Interview Guardedness, Shyness, Avoidance, Disinterest Kinetic Family Drawing Test Happiness, Realistic portrayal of family, Secure Attachment Draw–a-Person Test Aggression, Inadequacy, Inferiority, Poor social and emotional adjustment, Poor selfconcept Sentence Completion Test Poor self -concept, Pressure, Disappointment, Lack of support Thematic Apperception Test Guardedness, Abandonment 99 6.3.7 Summary of the results for Participant Two In general the participant was extremely guarded both in the clinical interview and in his responses to the projective tests. It appears that he puts up a happy, secure facade in order for his parents, especially for his mother, to cope better with the situation. However, he fears so many things that are out of his control. He appears to feel responsible for the happiness and security of his mother and his younger siblings. He feels burdened by this, as he is not able to do anything about it. He fears being a failure and essentially ending up like his (alcoholic) father and not being able to provide for his mother’s security needs. He is placing a lot of pressure on himself to better his and his family’s circumstances. He struggles to relate to his father as a result of attachment abuse he experienced at his hands. He blames himself, and in part, his father for not succeeding in keeping the family together. 6.4 Participant Three 6.4.1 The clinical interview Talking about her parents seemed to be an emotional issue for Participant Three. She was close to her parents, but they had passed away when she was very young. She suffered neglect at the hands of her maternal uncle who was responsible for her after her parents passed away. “My mom and my father passed away, so my uncles were looking after me. They were not looking after me so well so I landed up here”. “I was very close to them (parents). I can’t remember when they died. I was still young. I was about five”. She is reluctant to form relationships with any adult figures in the children’s home, as she is still holding on to the memory of her departed parents. “I don’t have such a person here that I see as a mother or father figure”. 100 Although she regards her host mother as a mother figure, she tends to distance herself in the relationship. She is rigid in intimate relationships due to fear that she might be left alone again. “I do make friends easily with people but some people I feel that they right for me and some people are not right for me”. She has only one close friend (a younger girl) in the children’s home and appears not to be interested in forming other relationships. She currently displays an insecure-avoidant attachment pattern. “I don’t usually go out. I’m usually in my room, or if I really want to go out, I go to Mamazi, my friend in the home”. 6.4.2 The Kinetic Family Drawing Test (KFD) Participant Three’s drawing had all the elements of insecure-avoidant attachment as previously discussed in the analysis of Participant One’s drawing. The Kinetic Family Drawing (KFD) indicated an emphasis on invulnerability, as opposed to an emphasis on vulnerability, where the child appears alone, isolated from the group and looking worried and fearful. Her drawing depicted an image of unrealistic happiness, as all the figures in the drawing had the same large and brightly coloured smile. There was an individuation of figures in the drawing; however, two figures (mother and self) were drawn exactly alike. 6.4.3 The Draw-a-Person Test (DAP) The drawing of Participant Three indicated that she feels insecure and there is a strong element of rigidity and self -consciousness in her interpersonal relations. She, however, has a desire for interpersonal contact and affection. She feels empty and displays withdrawal tendencies. There is an indication that she feels inferior and immature, and as a result displays aggressive tendencies. 101 6.4.4 The Sentence Completion Test The themes evident in Participant Three’s responses to the test indicated that she mourns the loss of her parents and the loss of her family. “I wish that my father was alive”. “I wish that my mother was alive”. “My greatest wish is to have a family”. It appeared that she wishes to escape her circumstances: “I often wish I could fly”. Participant Three appeared to desperately want normality and normality for her includes having a loving family and being happy within this family. “If I were a king I would make everything okay”. “I’d like most to be happy”. “My greatest wish is to have a family”. There was an indication that she worries about being a failure and the shaming effect that failure has. “I am ashamed that I failed Maths”. “I feel like giving up when I fail”. 6.4.5 The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) The responses Participant Three gave to the test were very short. These responses indicated that she feels unhappy but is striving to change the situation. She feels abandoned by her family and perceives people as unreliable. She feels angry and worries about her future. 6.4.6 Integration of data As was the case with Participant Two, Participant Three was extremely guarded. The integration of the results for Participant Three reveals heightened attachment needs and dynamics and this is pervasive throughout the different data sources. These are as a result of early loss of the primary caregivers and the forced adaptations to multiple carers and 102 environments. In the KFD the participant drew a picture of her father, mother and herself. She has an older brother whom she has not had contact with for some time, that is excluded from the drawing. It appears that she feels no connection to him due in part to the lack of contact between them. Because from his history (which is not included in the study) it is evident that the brother has created problems everywhere he went and has in turn created problems for her when they were together. The figures in the drawing appear happy but there is no interaction between the people in the drawing. This could be as a result of the fact that she has experienced no real affection and contact from a caregiver growing up and thus does not have an idea of how to act in loving, warm relationships. It could also be related to the minimal amount of time she spent with her parents before their death. The re is a sense of rigidity in the drawn figures. There is a lack of flexibility and interaction. Her parents died when she was five years old and although she was close to them, it appears that she does not have clear recollections of them (evidence from the clinical interview supports this), as she has not spent enough time with them. It could further mean that the family in the KFD is the perfect family that is portrayed in her drawing, a loving mother and father with an only child (herself). Another interesting aspect is the lack of individuation between the two female figures in the drawing. This indicates that she desires to be exactly like her mother in all respects or that she regards herself as a miniature model of her mother. The DAP drawing is of a happy, yet stiff postured female figure with outstretched arms. This mirrors the rigidity expressed in the clinical interview as well as the KFD test. This indicates that the participant is rigid in interpersonal relations due to her inexperience with loving caregivers and her past history of sexual abuse and neglect. The outstretched arms, however, could be taken as an indication that she strongly desires interpersonal contact and affection. The KFD also shows a lack of interaction between the family members even though the family is portrayed as happy and united. The happiness portrayed in the drawing 103 appears thus to be a mask of the fear and desperation she feels. Because she has never had a real family life experience, her needs are heightened in this respect and she does not have role relationship representations of how family members interact with each other in a close loving way. This could also explain why there is an absence of interaction in her KFD despite the fact that the drawing portrays a happy family. She desires to be nurtured and loved and to have the family she never had growing up. She desires simply for the family to be in close proximity to each other so that they could have contact with each other. There is an element of guardedness in her drawings as there is a lack of detailing usual for a person her age. It’s though she is trying not to reveal too much then having to deal with the negative feelings related to the exposure. There is an absence of any female characteristics in the KFD and DAP test drawings other than the long hair and simplistic dresses indicative of figures representing females. It appears that the participant only included the necessary female characteristics to indicate the difference between the figures and is afraid of showing more. This is in large part due to her past experience of sexual abuse and neglect. The only way to protect herself is not to express too much. This frees her from the contaminating experience of the sexual abuse. Her drawings depict an air of innocence. The drawings appear to be those of a younger person. From the responses on the Sentence Completion Test it is evident that she still mourns and is struggling with the loss of her parents. It appears as if she is preoccupied with this loss. Her greatest wish is to be a part of a loving family, preferably her biological family, whom she has not had enough time with. It’s though she is trying to recreate the loving family she once had. She appears to feel misplaced and desires interpersonal conta ct with a loving caregiver. She is very guarded and does not tend to allow herself to express too much emotion, as if to ward off any negative thoughts and feelings of her neglectful past or to invite unnecessary attention. This is evident in her responses to the TAT. She appears to be 104 unhappy with her life and wishes it were better. She has fantasies that she will be re-united with her family someday. She appears to feel empty inside and cry a lot. She desires to be rescued by someone. A theme that is pervasive throughout this participant’s data sources is her preoccupation with being a part of a loving, caring family and being taken care of. Evident from her history is the fact that she has never had a stable home environment and never had dependable people on whom she could count. Her existence is characteristic of a sole, lonely journey, which is too much for a person her age to handle. Table 6.3 Attachment-related themes generated for Participant Three DATA SOURCE THEMES Avoidance, Distance, Rigidity, Guardedness Clinical Interview Kinetic Family Drawing Test Invulnerability, Unrealistic happiness, Partial individuation, Insecure-avoidant attachment Aggression, Insecurity, Rigidity, Withdrawal Draw–a-Person Test Desire for normality, Escape, Fear of failure Sentence Completion Test Thematic Apperception Test 6.4.7 Unhappiness, Abandonment, Unreliability, Guardedness Summary of the results for Participant Three She struggles to recover from and deal with the loss of her parents. She still mourns their passing. She feels abandoned and worried about her future because essentially she has to 105 provide for herself, her own future, as she has no contact with any family members. She struggles to get close to other people due to past experiences of abuse and neglect. However, she yearns to be loved, accepted and taken care of. She has led a difficult life based on her background history. She lost her parents at a very young age, was separated from her sibling, and was exposed to multiple caregivers since after the death of her parents. Her upbringing was characterised by instability, neglect and (sexual) abuse and having to adjust continually to different environments. Her parents’ death is a sensitive issue for her and not much information could be ascertained with the clinical interview and most of the tests. From the little information that could be gathered and the analysis of the tests, it appears that she is insecurely attached due to her past experiences. 106 CHAPTER 7 DISCUSSION 7.1 Introduction In this section the results of the study is discussed. This will be done by contextualizing the results within the theoretical literature discussed in chapters 2, 3 and 4. The results yielded a wide range of themes related to the attachment patterns of children in children’s homes. For the purpose of this chapter, common themes will be grouped together and discussed under a broad category. 7.2 Attachment needs The results of the study clearly highlighted the attachment needs of adolescents. The needs of the participants are consistent with those expounded in the literature on attachment. Attachment needs in infancy, as illustrated in chapter 3 of the study, involve the desire for proximity to the attachment figure and the sense of security derived from the attachment figure’s presence. In adolescence attachment needs differ slightly. The adolescent needs an emotional secure base from which to explore a range of emotional states and to become autonomous and independent from family without losing familial attachment (Fonagy, 2001). It is possible to assess and describe an infant’s attachments by observing, describing, and making inferences from the infant’s behaviour, as illustrated by Ainsworth’s “Strange Situation”, because the infant does not ha ve the use of cognitions or language yet and rely on behaviour to show emotions. After infancy, however, you cannot describe an attachment adequately without describing the individual’s representational models of relationships. Ainsworth (1989) proposed that hormonal, neurophysiological, and cognitive changes associated with adolescence, not just socioemotional experience, may underlie normative 107 shifts in attachment processes. Representational models include feelings, beliefs, expectations, behavioural strategies, and rules for directing attention, interpreting information, and organising memory (Colin, 1996). The attachment theory account of the repetition of early patterns of behaviour in later life is primarily cognitive, i.e. internal representational models (Fonagy, 2001). The child whose internal working model of the caregiver is focused on rejection is expected to evolve a complementary working model of the self as unlovable, unworthy, and flawed (Fonagy, 2001). The caregiver’s responses convey how others regard the self, particularly in times of need, distress and raised affect (Senior, 2002). Accordingly, all children need a stable home and the love and care of both parents if they are to achieve optimal emotional development (Chesser, 1912-). Every child needs to experience real affection, appreciation and recognition from other people, especially his primary caregiver (usually parents). Without this he receives no assurance that he is good, worthy, loveable, and wanted (Bowley, 1947). Thus the importance that participants attach to the family when it comes to satisfying these needs cannot be overemphasised. Despite feeling abandoned, rejected, isolated, burdened and exploited by their caregivers and families, the participants still wish that they could be re-united with their families. They wish like any secure child that the primary caregiver could fulfil a nurturing and supportive role by being an emotional secure base. This relates to the foundation of adolescent attachment, where the parent-adolescent relationship serves to provide an emotional secure base from which the adolescent can explore the wide range of emotional states arising when learning to live as a relatively autonomous adult (Allen & Land, 1999). As adolescents experiment with life, most continue to derive support and protection from their childhood attachment figures. Theory suggests that those who are unable to derive support from early attachment figures will often persist in seeking it, nonetheless (Colin, 1996). This need for familial attachment is also highlighted by Participant One’s strong 108 identification and idealisation of her absent, nonabusive father whom she has never met. Participant Two hopes that his family will re-unite while Participant Three fantasises that she will in some way be reunited with her deceased parents. The desperation to have these needs met is further highlighted in the results of Participant One. The participant seems to be preoccupied with her host family that is everything that her family was not. According to Howe et al (1999), caregiving that has been neglectful, unpredictable and unresponsive often leads individuals to maximise their attachment behaviour and develop a preoccupation with relationships and their availability. This preoccupation with relationships and their availability stems from children needing the care and love that was absent from their lives. The results from Participants Two and Three also highlight this. An interesting aspect in the case of Participant One is that she has formed close bonds with male figures, that is, her housefather in the children’s home and her host father. This is contrary to Bowlby’s (1951) assertion that children are likely to attach to a female caregiver because women are essentially perceived as more available, caring, nurturing, emotional and soothing than men. A possible explanation for this could be that the participant has had a tumultuous relationship with her mother and has as a result came to the conclusion that mothers in general do not love their children as much as fathers do. It is possible that she regards her mother as more responsible for her plight because her mother not only did nothing about the abuse, but her mother also took her husband’s side when the participant informed her about the abuse. It is also noteworthy that the attachment needs of the participants are not only emotional in nature, but also materially orientated. Major concerns for the participants are the extent to which their material needs are met. Thus they had preoccupations with academic failure and financial security as a means of being able to provide for themselves and their families. This preoccupation is manifested in the need to succeed academically, the desire that family life will improve and they would be reunited, and the hope that a parent would 109 find a job. As to be expected, these preoccupations are a major source of stress for the participants. They have to concern themselves with issues that were supposed to be the responsibility of parents. They do not have the skills and resources to assume these responsibilities. They either take on these responsibilities because they want to or because it is forced upon them by circumstances. For some this represents a lost childhood because there is no time to engage in activities or have concerns typical of children their age. This might also be why they are reluctant to form relationships with children their own age. Either way, it thus appears as if the adolescent who has been abused and institutionalised is caught between the need to assume adult responsibilities and especially the desire to be a normal child. It appears as if adolescent boys tend to embrace this responsibility because society socialises them that way and adolescent girls tend to assume this responsibility reluctantly because they do not have a choice. 7.3 Past attachment experiences As indicated in the participants’ profiles in section 5.3.3, all three participants experienced abuse, whether physical or sexual, perpetrated by their attachment figures and caregivers. Participant One’s history shows that she had an insensitive mother and a sexually abusive stepfather. Participant Two’s history indicated that he had an absent and physically abusive father and an overwhelmed mother who later became unresponsive after she was diagnosed with depression. The family broke up after 12 years. Participant Three’s history shows that her parents died when she was 5 years old. She then experienced neglectful caregiving from her maternal uncle that led to her being sexually abused by a stranger. She had multiple caregivers and environment changes since after her parents died. She experienced various permanent separations. Evidence of the abuse is clear from the results. All three participants were insecurely attached as a result of their negative early experiences 110 with their attachment figures. According to Howe et al. (1999), caregiving that is and has been neglectful, physically abusive, sexually abusive, rejecting, inconsistent or repeatedly interrupted (multiple serial carers) will greatly increase the risk of children developing an insecure attachment pattern, which in turn increases the risk of later developmental disturbances. The insecure attachment means that the participants are not free to evaluate attachment-related experiences in a balanced, accurate manner. The participants also find it difficult to make sense of their emotions. This is consistent with Howe et al’s (1999) assertion that secure attachment gives the child the cognitive strengths to make sens e of and understand emotions. All of the participants also experienced separation from their attachment figures and families, either early or later in their lives. Separation occurred as a result of forced separation by the state, abandonment and death of the caregivers. According to Fonagy (2001), separation involves two stressors: unprotected exposure and the sense of being cut off from the critical source of protection. In addition to experiencing these two separation related stressors, the participants also experienced separation from familiar surroundings, i.e. the home environment. Of the three participants, only Participant Two had positive experiences with his mother. Participant One’s experiences were negative, while the same was the case for Participant Three who was placed in the care of a relative when she was 5 years old. Bowley (1947) emphasises that it is important for the child’s emotional development that he should have this assurance to offset his inner fears of badness, unworthiness, unlovableness, and his sense of rejection. Environments can enhance growth or create risks for children. The context that fosters optimal growth is generally the family (Hook, 2002d). The family introduces children to the physical world through the opportunities it provides for play and exploration of objects and ultimately to form a secure identity in adolescence (Bowlby, 1988; Hook, 2002d). It also creates bonds between people that are unique. The attachments children form 111 with parents and siblings usually last a lifetime, and they serve as models for other significant relationships (Gomez, 1997). A common theme present in the data indicates that the participants feel unsafe and unprotected by their families. They also feel excluded from and rejected by their families. As a result they feel that they have to support themselves and cope independently. Actual closeness of the attachment figure is frequently the means by which the child is able to feel secure. Thus, the past influences or biases expectations but does not determine these. Both older children and adults continue to monitor the accessibility and responsiveness of the attachment figure (Fonagy, 2001). Another pervasive theme present in the participants’ responses to the tests is that of abandonment and rejection. According to Main and Goldman (in Morgan, 1999), rejection refers to an active hostile or cold response to the child. Abandonment in turn refers to the desertion and forsaking of a person. The participants feel abandoned by their families. To an extent, they appear to blame themselves for the abandonment as they feel that they are not ‘good enough’ for their families. They have been in the care of their abusing caregivers for a long period before being removed from them, and by using their cognitive capacities, they tend to look at their own contribution to where they are now (the children’s home) and their part in their formed relationships and attachments. Apart from the rejection and abandonment, the participants also suffered severe maternal deprivation. As is discussed in chapter 4, maternal deprivation entails the removal of the child from his caregivers (families) by social services due to abuse and/ or neglect perpetrated in the home and placement in protective care. This abandonment, reje ction and deprivation makes the participants feel unwanted. According to Safonova and Leparsky (1998), this also deprives the child from sensory and emotional stimuli necessary for the child to develop the skills and resources needed to make realistic sens e of his own life and his surroundings. 112 7.4 The children’s home The purpose of the children’s home is essentially to offset the impact that the past abuses, abandonment, neglect and deprivation has on the child and to provide an alternative environment where the child’s needs will be met. What is also true though is the fact that the quality of early and continuing close relationships affects children’s emotional development, interpersonal style and social behaviour (Senior, 2002). This is evident from the participants’ experience in the children’s home. The participants’ attachment histories seem to be reinforced in the children’s home. Even though the participants’ institutionalisation came about (after infancy, and primarily) in early adolescence, and even though they would not be considered maternally deprived in the sense that Bowlby (1952) originally used the term, they essentially spent a significant time of their development in an institutional environment. This however failed to offset the negative experiences the study participants had in their families. It is possible that this is a result of the extreme nature of the abuse and the effect that it had on the participants. This effect can probably be linked to the fact that the abuse happened at an age in which the individual’s beliefs regarding relationships are consolidated. It thus follows that the participants displayed insecure attachment patterns in the children’s home. This insecure attachment is manifested in their reluctance or inability to form close relationships with peers and to attach to significant adults within the children’s home. Although the home afforded them order and consistency, there was little of the affective nurturance essential for healthy emotional development. According to Batchelor (1998), children’s love and growth require long nurturance from people who make enduring commitments to them, i.e. attachment figures, be it family or care workers at the children’s home. Absence of this nurturance in the children’s home can be ascribed to the fact that the staff-child ratio is extremely low and there is not enough interaction between care worker and child to facilitate a secure attachment relationship. This gives rise to feelings of insecurity, 113 isolation, guardedness, rigidity, shyness and a general disinterest in relationships. The contradiction here is that the participants have relationship needs, yet in order to protect themselves, they are forced to behave in ways that do not facilitate the building of healthy relationships. 7.5 Children’s self-concepts as a function of their attachment relationships The literature highlights the concomitant relationship between the child’s self-concept and his attachment relationships. Fonagy (2001) asserts that the experience of being cared for relates directly to self -esteem. A basic assumption of attachment theory is that critical variations in the quality of one’s early experiences with caregiving figures shape the formation of mental representations or internal working models of close relationships (Senior, 2002). Mental representations are useful representations to the extent that they capture important causal/ relational properties of the environment and the self well enough to allow an individual to operate successfully in that environment (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999). Internal working models comprise of: (a) self model – containing perceptions of one’s own worth and lovability, and (b) other model – containing expectations regarding the essential goodness, trustworthiness, and dependability of important others in one’s social world (Senior, 2002). By adulthood, the assessment of internal representational models is typically evidenced in how individuals organise, process, and communicate about attachment information, rather than in the behavioural patterns observed in infancy (Black, Jaeger, McCartney, & Crittenden, 2000; Colin, 1996). Kobak and Cole (in Black et al., 2000) argue, however, that with the onset of formal operations, the cognitive competence underlying adult expressions of security is available during adolescence. Specifically, adolescents possess the skills to describe abstract aspects of themselves and their relationships, to contrast these with 114 alternative models and to distance themselves enough from immediate experience to generalize appropriately about these experiences. Moreover, Kobak and Cole argue that changes in adolescents’ abilities to reason allow them to operate on (or monitor) these abstract, differentiated models in more sophisticated ways (in Black et al., 2000). Furthermore, adolescents can consider several aspects of models simultaneously as well as evaluate the validity of abstract propositions regarding the self and attachment figures. These skills enable adolescents to detect and correct inconsistencies in their models of themselves and others (Black et al., 2000). Results of the study, especially that of the projective drawings tests, indicate that the participants have a compromised self-model and other model largely due to their negative life experiences. This compromise self- and other model further serves the purpose of regulating their present attachment behaviours. Participants indicate anger directed towards the self for not being able to do anything about their situation. There are also elements of helplessness and hopelessness in this regard. Themes emerging suggest that most children feel insecure, inadequate and anxious and as a result need to control their environment. Most children generally have a strong need for attention, support and acceptance, but the participants illustrate elevations of these specific needs. From a developmental perspective, the issue of the self is especially relevant for the participants given the fact that their prime task according to Erikson is to secure a sense of self, to form a secure identity (Hook, 2002b). The socioemotional context status of adolescence may also play a role in how these adolescents choose to describe their relationships with parents. According to Erikson (in Black et al., 2000), adolescents are confronted with the task of constructing an identity. It thus follows that certain conditions are necessary for the formation of a secure identity. According to Bloss (in Black et al., 2000), a critical condition is the differentiation of the self from parents. This differentiation can only 115 be achieved if the child has in the past experienced positive attachment dynamics in their relationships with their caregivers. Erikson (in Fonagy, 2001) asserts that the quality of the maternal relationship determines the sense of trust that mothers create in their children which then persists throughout life. For the study participants this condition was clearly not met. Their insecure attachment coupled with the rejection, abuse and neglect they have experienced at their hands of their caregivers have compromised their ability to form a positive sense of self. 7.6 Ability to adjust / adapt to circumstances The study participants appear to be poorly adjusted to their environment, the children’s home. Adaptation is Piaget’s term for the ability to adjust to the demands of the environment, a process during which schemes are elaborated, changed, and developed (Cockcroft, 2002). According to Crittenden (2000), adaptability is best described as a process in which not fully accurate dispositional representations are consistently reorganised into less inaccurate dispositional representations that permit flexible and adaptive use of all behavioural strategies without distorting the mental processing of information. That is, security refers to a specific strategy suitable to protective relationships in relatively safe contexts, whereas adaptability refers to a process of selecting strategies that promote survival under all kinds of conditions. Bowlby argued that because attachment relationships are internalised or represented, these early experiences and subsequent expectations get taken forward to serve later behavioural and emotional adaptation, even in totally new contexts and with different people (Kobak, 1999). This is a new experience for all of the participants. None of the participants have ever been in any form of residential care or protective care before their admission to the children’s home. Consistent with Bowlby’s assertion, 116 participants appear to have internalised their early experience to the extent that it has shaped their beliefs about relationships. The theory suggests a number of reasons why there could be a lack of adaptation. It is for example argued that a lack of attention to parent-child ties harms both the child’s capacity for adaptation to the institution and his ability to benefit from the opportunities available. This is because of the trauma of separation upon entrance to the institution and the repeated need for separation at each visit coupled with ignorance of the need to shape a new parentchild relationship (Eisikovits & Beker, 1986). According to Wicks-Nelson and Israel (1997), children are either at risk or can be resilient in situations where they are placed away from their parents (caregivers) and family. Risk factors, or risks, are variables that increase the chance of behaviour deviations or difficulties. In the presence of risk, some individuals succumb (are vulnerable) whereas others maintain healthy functioning, that is, are resilient (Wicks -Nelson & Israel, 1997). According to Smith and Prior (1995), resilience implies protection from risk factors, or the ability to bounce back in the face of life’s adversities. Some risk factors can be viewed as stemming from individual disposition to respond maladaptively to life experiences. The participants have clearly not adapted to their situation. This however does not mean that the participants have not derised and employed strategies to cope with their situation. In this context the term coping is used to refer to strategies, although not positive, that participants use to survive in their current circumstances. Although this appears to be similar to adaptation/ adjustment, the difference is the negative connotation inherent in coping. Although participants found it difficult to adapt, they did manage to cope. The studies of Murphy and Moriarty (in Batchelor, 1998) on the coping capacities of children, asserted that children’s coping processes included cognitive functions (e.g. being able to mould and manipulate their environment assertively), as well as the use of a range of defensive 117 strategies. The strategies that study participants used ranged from not forming relationships in the children’s home and their reluctance to form relationships with peers and adult outside the children’s home as well as the use of fantasising that family will be reunited in cases where this is impossible. Resiliency is often conceptualised as arising from the child’s competence and adaptability (Wicks -Nelson & Israel, 1997). According to Garmezy (1975), competence is indicated by self -esteem, self -discipline, belief that events can be controlled, regulation of impulsive behaviour, and the ability to think abstractly and flexibly. Results from the present study sugge st evidence consistent with the above -mentioned views. When the context is stable, it benefits children to learn the strategies suited to it as soon as possible. On the other hand, when the context is variable, children need to learn to use more than one strategy flexibly. In the circumstances where the context is first stable in one way and then becomes stable in a completely different way, the early impact of the context could have deleterious effects on children’s ability to adapt (Crittenden & Claussen, 2000). Research indicates that formally institutionalised children may adapt to an environment of many nonintimate adults but find it more difficult to trust that permanent adults will remain accepting and available (Crittenden & Claussen, 2000). There is a risk that by remaining too vigilant, too open to strangers, and too demanding of attention, formally institutionalised children in a permanent placement will both fail to explore the full range of possible self-development and disappoint attachment figures, who, becoming discouraged, may become less accepting of the children (Crittenden & Claussen, 2000). 118 CHAPTER 8 CONCLUSION 8.1 Introduction The focus of this study is to explore and develop a better understanding of the nature of attachment patterns of institutionalised adolescents by looking at how attachment abuse, maternal deprivation and institutionalisation can be detrimental to forming close relationships. The findings of the study are presented and discussed in chapters 6 and 7 respectively. In this chapter, the study is concluded. This will be done by reflecting on the themes and examining the extent to which the aim of the study was achieved. The study itself will also be critically reflected on and possible suggestions for improvement will be made. 8.2 Reflecting on the themes emerging from the study The themes generated from the study, though overlapping significantly, appear to be nonetheless consistent with the theoretical literature. They thus indicate that older children who have been separated from their caregivers and placed in a children’s home because they were abused, neglected or maternally deprived, tend to be insecurely attached. The participants’ did not receive adequate caregiving from primary caregivers. They were also abused physically and/ or sexually and experienced rejection and abandonment. The literature highlights the importance of adequate caregiving that is necessary for the child to develop a healthy sense of self and the implications if this is absent. The effects of the neglect, abuse and maternal deprivation that the children suffered seem to be compounded by the circumstances in which they were placed. In institutions or children’s homes there is an increased risk that children will continue with insecure attachment patterns, as the staff-child ratio is extremely low and there is not enough interaction between care worker and child to 119 facilitate a secure attachment relationship. All these factors therefore seem to conspire in order to ensure that insecure attachment persists throughout the child’s development. 8.3 Were the aims of the study met? It was extremely difficult to attribute institutionalisation as having a consequence on attachment patterns per se, as the participants were adolescents (16 year olds) and were only placed in institutional care during this developmental stage due to attachment abuse. Institutionalisation is however the consequence of removal from abusive homes and is thus secondary to the attachment abuse perpetrated. Background histories of the participants documenting their care from caregivers and the abuse perpetrated by caregivers (attachment abuse) contributed largely to a picture of the attachment patterns formed in childhood. However, institutionalisation can impact on attachment patterns for later development of close relationships. The study succeeded in its aim to describe the nature of attachment patterns in institutionalised children. The data sources, especially the projective tests, yielded rich data that provided significant insight into the attachment patterns of the participants. This enabled the researcher to develop a better understanding of the participants’ attachment patterns. The major limitation of the study is that it does not explain why the attachment patterns are insecure. However, the central aim was to develop an improved understanding rather than to explain. 120 8.4 Reflecting on the study Because doing research is rarely problem free and because it is almost inevitable that problems are encountered during the research process, it is necessary to reflect on the process in order to identify ways in which the research might have been conducted more successfully. 8.4.1 Sampling The sampling of the study served as a limitation, because the pool from which the participants were chosen was too small. The generalizability power of the study is therefore extremely limited. It needs to be added however that the purpose of the study was to increase understanding of the phenomena under investigation and not to generalize the results of the study to wider populations. Accessing participants also proved challenging. The participants were only available during the late afternoons (from 16h00 to 18h00) and were tired towards the completion of the test battery. Due to the sensit ive nature of the study, some participants were guarded (which is usually the case in adolescence) and others were not able to express themselves clearly, as they were too emotional at times. Most information had to be taken from their case files, which were in most instances incomplete. Parents or caregivers failed to give complete information during the intake interview at the children’s home, as some were absent due to death, imprisonment, and disappearances. The developmental stage of the participants chosen (i.e. adolescents) served as an obstacle, as adolescence in and of itself is a complex stage. Adolescence is characterised by profound biological, psychological, and social developmental stages. During adolescence the adolescent is faced with the ta sk of forming a secure identity, which is made up of and influenced by childhood experiences with caregivers and significant others. For adolescents, identity is both a matter of determining who one is and a matter of deciding who one will be. Traumatised and abused children, as is the case with the participants used in the study, 121 struggle in determining who they are because of the amount of trauma they experienced and not knowing how to process these experiences. They inevitably struggle in deciding who they will be due to the hatred they have of themselves and others they view as potential abusers and due to the child not having appropriate role relationship representations that they can constructively use in forming a secure identity. 8.4.2 Data collection and analysis Because of the complexity of the developmental stage on which the study focussed, it was deemed necessary to include data sources that will provide sufficient information in order to achieve the aims of the study. Notwithstanding this, collecting data was not without difficulties. There were a number of problems with the transcription of the clinical interviews, which were tape-recorded. Apart from the disruptions, some background noises were caught on tape. This made transcription of some of the actual interview material difficult. The researcher therefore had to rely on her notes and her recollections of the interview situation. Tape recorders also do not capture non-verbal language. In both these instances it has to be conceded that valuable data might have been lost. In terms of data analysis, content analytical theme generation relies on interpretation. The researcher’s interpretation was guided by her preconceived belief and norm system. Any interpretation excluded other interpretations. Thus in the analytical process, certain material which was regarded as irrelevant was discarded. It therefore has to be stressed that no objectivity claims are made. The results merely represent one way of understanding the phenomena that was investigated. 122 8.5 To conclude… Securing a solid base identity made up of various roles, talents, skills, and preferences is the key challenge that individuals face generally, but is a far greater challenge for adolescents who have had a difficult upbringing, as is the case with all the participants used in the study. The caregiver’s responses convey how others regard the self, particularly in times of need, distress and raised affect. Thus, the quality of the relationship with the caregiver has a profound bearing on the child’s understanding of self. Children that have been institutionalised as a result of abuse grapple with the question “Who am I?” The inconsistent, unpredictable love and interest of caregivers means that the self is experienced as inherently not very loveable, unworthy and of no great interest. Self-esteem is low as a result. The lack of synchronicity and attunement with caregivers means that children find it difficult to get others to recognise, understand and respond to their needs. These children are faced with a greater task of forming their identity, as they were seldom exposed to close attachment relationships during early childhood, and this is likely to have implications for future adult relationships. 123 REFERENCES Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709-716. Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillside, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Allen, J.P., & Land, D. (1999). Attachment in adolescence. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 319-335). New York: Guilford Press. Appel, K.E. (1931). Drawings by children as aids in personality studies. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1, 129-144. Ashmore, R.D., & Jussim, L. (Eds.). (1997). Self and identity. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Babbie, E.R., & Mouton, J. (2001). The practice of social research. Cape Town: Oxford University Press. Batchelor, J. (1998). Adaptation to prolonged separation and loss in institutionalised children: Influences on the psychological capacities of adults 124 “orphaned” throughout childhood. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Smith College School for Social Work, Northampton, Massachusetts. Berk, L.E. (1989). Child development. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Berk, L.E. (1996). Infants and children: Prenatal through middle childhood (2nd ed.). Boston: Allen and Bacon. Berliner, L., & Elliott, D.M. (1996). Se xual abuse of children. In J. Briere, L. Berliner, J.A. Bulkley, C. Jenny, & T. Reid, (Eds.), The APSAC handbook on child maltreatment (pp. 51-71). London: Sage Publications, Inc. Black, K.A., Jaeger, E., McCartney, K., & Crittenden, P.M. (2000). Attachment models, peer interaction behavior, and feelings about the self: Indications of maladjustment in dismissing/ preoccupied (Ds/E) adolescents. In P.M. Crittenden & A.H. Claussen, (Eds.), The organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture, and context (pp. 300-324 ). New York: Cambridge University Press. Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. World Health Organization, Geneva. Bowlby, J. (1952). Maternal care and mental health. Bulletin of World Health Organization, 3 , 355-534. 125 Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Attachment (Vol.1). London: Hogarth Press. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Separation, anxiety and anger (Vol. 2). London: Hogarth Press. Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York: Basic Books. Bowley, A.H. (1947). The psychology of the unwanted child. Edinburgh: E. & S. Livingstone Ltd. Brassard, M.R., Hart, S.N., & Hardy, D. (1991). Psychological and emotional abuse of children. In R.T. Ammerman & M. Harsen (Eds.), Case studies in family violence (pp. 255-270). New York: Plenum. Bretherton, I. (1991). The roots and growing points of attachment theory. In C.M. Parkes et al. (Eds.), Attachment across the life cycle (pp. 9-32). London: Routledge. Bretherton, I. (1995). The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth. In S. Goldberg et al. (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, developmental and clinical perspectives (pp. 45-84). London: The Analytical Press. Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K.A. (1999). Internal working models in attachment relationships: A construct revisited. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of 126 attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 89-114). New York: Guilford Press. Carlsson, E., & Sroufe, L.A. (1995). Contribution of attachment theory to developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology. Vol. 1: Theory and Methods (pp. 581-617). New York: Wiley. Cassidy, J. (1999). The nature of the child’s ties. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 3-20). New York: Guilford Press. Chandler, L.A., & Johnson, V.J. (1991). Using projective techniques with children: A guide to clinical assessment. Springfield, IL: C.C. Thomas. Chesser, E. (1912- ). Unwanted child. London: Rich and Cowan. Cicchetti, D. (1989). How research on child maltreatment has informed the study of child development: Perspectives from developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 377-431). New York: Cambridge University Press. Cockcroft, K. (2002). Theories of cognitive development: Piaget, Vygotsky, and Information-processing theory. In D. Hook, J. Watts, & K. Cockcroft, (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 175-199). Cape Town: UCT Press. 127 Colin, V.L. (1996). Human attachment. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Committee on the Rights of the Child. (1999). Consideration of reports submitted by states parties under article 44 of the convention on the rights of the child. Initial reports of States parties due in 1997: South Africa. 22/05/99. CRC/C/51/Add.2. (State Party Report). Crittenden, P.M. (1985). Social networks, quality of child rearing, and child development. Child Development, 56, 1299-1313. Crittenden, P.M. (2000). A dynamic-maturational exploration of the meaning of security and adaptation: Empirical, cultural, and theoretical considerations. In P.M. Crittenden & A.H. Claussen, (Eds.), The organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture, and context (pp. 358-383). New York: Cambridge University Press. Crittenden, P., & Ainsworth, M. (1989). Child maltreatment and attachment theory. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 432-463). New York: Cambridge University Press. Crittenden, P.M., & Claussen, A.H. (2000). Adaptation to varied environments. In P.M. Crittenden & A.H. Claussen, (Eds.), The organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture, and context (pp. 234-248). New York: Cambridge University Press. 128 David, H.P. (1992). Born unwanted: Long-term developmental effects of denied abortion. Journal of Social Issues, 48 (3), 163-181. Davis, A. (1981). The residential solution: State alternatives to family care. London: Tavistock Publications. Eagle, G. (1998). Male crime victims: The social and pe rsonal construction of meaning in response to traumatogenic events. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa. Eisikovits, Z., & Beker, J. (Eds.). (1986). Residential group care in community context: Insights from the Israeli experience. New York: The Haworth Press. Erickson, M.F., & Egeland, B. (1996). Child neglect. In J. Briere, L. Berliner, J.A. Bulkley, C. Jenny, & T. Reid, (Eds.), The APSAC handbook on child maltreatment (pp. 420). London: Sage Publications, Inc.. Feeney, J.A., Hohaus, L., Noller, P., & Alexander, R.P. (2001). Becoming parents: Exploring the bonds between mothers, fathers, and their infants. London: Cambridge University Press. Finnegan, R.A., Hodges, E.V.E., & Perry, D.G. (1996). Preoccupied and avoidant coping during middle childhood. Child Development, 67, 1318-1328. 129 Fonagy, P. (1999). Psychoanalytic theory from the viewpoint of attachment theory and research. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 595-624). New York: Guilford Press. Fonagy, P. (2001). Attachment theory and psychoanalysis. New York: Other Press. Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Moran, G.S., Steele, H., & Higgitt, A. (1992). The integration of psychoanalytic theory and work on attachment: The issue of intergenerational psychic processes. In D. Stern & M. Ammaniti (Eds.), Attaccamento e psiconalis (pp. 19-30). Bari, Italy: Laterza. Freud, S. (1977). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. In A. Richards & A. Dickson (Eds.), The Penguin Freud Library. Vol. 7 on sexuality: Three essays on the theory of sexuality and other works. London: Penguin. Freud, S. (1991). The ego and the id. In A. Richards & A. Dickson (Eds.), The Penguin Freud Library. Vol. 11 on metapsychology. London: Penguin. Fury, G., Carlson, E.A., & Sroufe, A. (1997). Children’s representations of attachment relationships in family drawings. Child Development, 68 (6), 1154-1164. Garmezy, N. (1975). The experimental study of children vulnerable to psychopathology. In A. Davids (Ed.), Child personality and psychopathology (Vol. 2). New York: Wiley-Interscience. 130 Geiger, B. (1996). Fathers as primary caregivers. Westport: Greenwood Press. Gergeley, G., & Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback model of parental affectmirroring. International Journal of Psycho -Analysis, 77, 1181-1212. Gitter, A.H. (1999). Adolescents’ and mothers’ models of attachment as predictors of developing autonomy and relatedness in observed family interactions. Unpublished distinguished majors thesis. University of Virginia, Virginia. Goldberg, S. (2000). Attachment and development. London: Arnold, a member of the Hodder Headline Group. Gomez, L. (1997). An introduction to object relations. London: Free Association. Groth-Marnat, G. (1997). Handbook of psychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Gullestad, S.E. (2001). Attachment theory and psychoanalysis: Controversial issues. Scandinavian Psychoanalytical Review, 24, 3-16. Hammersly, M. (1996). The relationship between qualitative and quantitative research: Paradigm loyalty versus methodological eclecticism. In J.T.E. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of qualitative research methods for psychology and the social sciences (pp. 159-174). Leicester: British Psychological Society. 131 Hammond, S. (1997). Using psychometric tests. In G.M. Breakwell, S. Hammond, & C. Fife -Schaw, (Eds.), Research methods in psychology (pp. 194-212). London: Sage Publications. Harris, D.B. (1963). Children’s drawing s as measures of intellectual maturity. New York: Harcourt, Brace, & World. Hart, S.N., Brassard, M.R., & Karlson, H.C. (1996). Psychological maltreatment. In J. Briere, L. Berliner, J.A. Bulkley, C. Jenny, & T. Reid, (Eds.), The APSAC handbook on child maltreatment (pp. 72-89). London: Sage Publications, Inc.. Hill, J.P., & Holmbeck, G.N. (1986). Attachment and autonomy during adolescence. Annals of Child Development, 3, 145-189. Hilzinger, D.G. (2002). “Not one of these little ones should perish”: Orphanages in the Roman Catholic Archdiocese of Philadelphia 1850-1950. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Graduate School of Arts and Science, Columbia University, New York. Holmes, J. (1995). John Bowlby; attachment theory and psychoanalysis. In S. Goldberg et al. (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, developmental and clinical perspectives (pp. 19-43). London: The Analytical Press. Hook, D. (2002a). Psychoanalytic, cognitive, and psychosocial developmental psychology: The ‘hows’ and ‘whys’. In D. Hook, J. Watts, & K. Cockcroft, (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 3-13). Cape Town: UCT Press. 132 Hook, D. (2002b). Erikson’s psychosocial stages of development. In D. Hook, J. Watts, & K. Cockcroft, (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 265-293). Cape Town: UCT Press. Hook, D. (2002c). Bronfenbrenner’s ecological theory of development. In D. Hook, J. Watts, & K. Cockcroft, (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 312-324). Cape Town: UCT Press. Hook, D. (2002d). Critical issues in developmental psychology. In D. Hook, J. Watts, & K. Cockcroft, (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 343-366). Cape Town: UCT Press. Hook, D., & Cockcroft, K. (2002). Basic concepts and principles in developmental psychology. In D. Hook, J. Watts, & K. Cockcroft, (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 14-28). Cape Town: UCT Press. Hook, D., & Watts, J. (2002). Freud’s psychoanalytic theory of development and personality. In D. Hook, J. Watts, & K. Cockcroft, (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 57-91). Cape Town: UCT Press. Howe, D., Bra ndon, M., Hinings, D., & Schofield, G. (1999). Attachment theory, child maltreatment and family support: A practice and assessment model. London: Macmillan Press Ltd. 133 Hulse, W.C. (1951). The emotionally disturbed child draws his family. Quarterly Journal of Child Behaviour, 3, 152-174. Hunter, W.M., Coulter, M.L., Runyan, D.K., & Everson, M.D. (1990). Determinants of placement for sexually abused children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 14, 407-418. Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books. Kaplan, H.I., & Sadock, B.J. (1998). Synopsis of psychiatry: Behavioral sciences / clinical psychiatry (8th ed.). New York: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). Children’s internal representations of attachment as seen in family drawings and in a separation anxiety interview. In M. Main (Chair), Attachment: A move to the level of representation. Symposium presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto. Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1986). Instructions for the classification of children’s family drawings in terms of representation of attachment. Berkeley: University of California. Kobak, R. (1999). The emotional dynamics of disruptions in attachment relationships: Implications for theory, research, and clinical intervention. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 21-43). New York: Guilford Press. 134 Kuzel, A.J. (1992). Sampling in qualitative inquiry. In B.F. Crabtree & W.L. Miller (Eds.), Research methods for primary care (pp. 33-44). London: Sage. Kvale, S. (1996). Interviews: An introduction to qualitative research interviewing. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxfor d University Press. Little, M. (1982). Family breakup (1st ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Louw, D.A. (1991). Human development. Cape Town: Kagiso Tertiary. Louw, D., Louw, A., & Schoeman, W. (1995). Developmental psychology. In D.A. Louw & J.A. Edwards (Eds.), Psychology: An introduction for students in Southern Africa. Johannesburg: Lexicon. Machover, K. (1949). Personality projection in the drawing of the human figure. Springfield, IL: Thomas. Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Lack of mourning in adulthood and its relationship to infant disorganisation: Some speculations regarding causal mechanisms. In M.T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E.M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 135 McCarthy, J.B. (Ed.). (2000). Adolescent development and psychopathology. New York: University Press of America, Inc. McCluskey, U., & Hooper, C. (Eds.). (2000). Psychodynamic perspectives on abuse: The cost of fear. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Morgan, J.Y. (1999). The application of attachment theory to a psychotherapy case. Unpublished masters dissertation, Rand Afrikaans University, Johannesburg, South Africa. Mortensen, K.V. (1991). Form and content in children’s human figure drawings: Development, sex differences and body experience. New York: New York University Press. Moshman, D. (1999). Adolescent psychological development: Rationality, morality, and identity. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Mouton, J., & Marais, H.C. (1996). Basic concepts in the methodology of the social sciences. Pretoria: HSRC Publishers. Mudaly, B.S. (1985). The life -world of youth in children’s homes. Unpublished masters dissertation, Rhodes University, Grahamstown, South Africa. Murphy, K.R., & Davidshofer, C.O. (2001). Psychological testing: Principles and applications (5th ed.). New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. 136 Murray, H.A. (1938). Explorations in personality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Mussen, P.H., Conger, J.J., Kagan, J., & Huston, A.C. (1990). A child’s world: Infancy through adolescence (6th ed.). New York: MacGraw -Hill. Neuman, L.W. (1997). Social research methods: Qualitative and quantitative approaches (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Nye, R.D. (1992). Three psychologies: Perspectives from Freud, Skinner, and Rogers (4th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/ Cole Publishing Company. Parker, K.C.H., Hanson, R.K., & Hunsley, J. (1988). MMPI, Rorschach, and WAIS: A meta-analytic comparison of reliability, stability, and validity. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 367-373. Pianta, R.C., & Longmaid, K. (1999). Attachment-based classifications of children’s family drawings: Psychometric properties and relations with children’s adjustment in kindergarten. Journal of Clinic al Child Psychology, 28 (2), 244-264. Rachlin, H. (1991). Introduction to modern behaviorism (3rd ed.). New York: Freeman. 137 Safonova, T., & Leparsky, E.A. (1998). The unwanted child. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22 (2), 155-157. Santrock, J.W. (1999). Life -span development. Boston: McGraw-Hill. Santrock, J.W. (2001). Child development (9th ed.). Boston: MacGraw-Hill. Senior, L. (2002). Attachment theory. In D. Hook, J. Watts, & K. Cockcroft, (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 247-264). Cape Town: UCT Press. Smith, J., & Prior, M. (1995). Temperament and stress resilience in school-age children: A within-families study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 168-179. Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999). The measurement of attachment security in infancy and childhood. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 287-318). New York: Guilford Press. Staddon, J. (1993). Behaviorism: Mind, mechanism and society. London: Duckworth. Steinberg, L. (1990). Interdependency in the family: Autonomy, conflict, and harmony in the parent-adolescent relationship. In S.S. Feldman & G.R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 255-276). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 138 Stosny, S. (1995). Treating attachment abuse: A compassionate approach. New York: Springer Publishing Company. Tizard, B., & Rees, J. (1975). The effects of early institutional rearing on the behaviour problems and affectional relationships of four -year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 16, 61-73. Todd, J.T., & Morris, E.K. (1995). Modern perspectives on John B. Skinner and contemporary behaviorism. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. van Ijzendoorn, M.H., & De Wolff, M.S. (1997). In search of the absent father – meta-analysis of infant-father attachment: A rejoinder to our discussants. Child Development, 68, 604-609. Van Staden, F., & Nieuwoudt, J. (2001). The contextualisation of self-attitudes: An investigation of adolescents in a South African children’s home. Pretoria: Department of Psychology, University of South Africa. Vass, Z. (1999). The inner formal structure of the H -T-P Drawings: An exploratory study. Unpublished paper, Eotvos Lorand University, Budapest, Hungary. Watts, J. (2002). Klein’s object relations theory of development and personality. In D. Hook, J. Watts, & K. Cockcroft, (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 92-106). Cape Town: UCT Press. 139 Weinfield, N.S., Sroufe, L.A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E.A. (1999). The nature of individual differences in infant-caregiver attachment. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 68-88). New York: Guilford Press. Westen, D. (1999). Psychology: Mind, brain, & culture (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Wicks-Nelson, R., & Israel, A.C. (1997). Behavior disorders of childhood (3rd ed.). New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Winnicott, D.W. (1971). Playing and reality. London: Tavistock. Wolff, W. (1942). Projective methods for personality analysis of expressive behaviour in preschool children. Character & Personality, 10, 309-330. Zeanah, C.H., Mammen, O.K., & Lieberman, A.F. (1993). Disorders of attachment. In C.H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health. New York: Guilford Press. 140