Beyond ethical climate - 1 Understanding the Ethical Context of Organizations: The Role of Ethical Climate, Collective Moral Emotion, and Collective Ethical Efficacy on Ethical Behavior in Organizations Anke Arnaud Embry-Riddle Aeronautical University Marshall Schminke University of Central Florida mschminke@bus.ucf.edu DRAFT MANUSCRIPT: PLEASE DO NOT CITE WITHOUT PERMISSION KEYWORDS: Ethics, ethical climate, affective tone, moral emotion, ethical efficacy, ethical behavior Beyond ethical climate - 2 Understanding the Ethical Context of Organizations: The Role of Ethical Climate, Collective Moral Emotion, and Collective Ethical Efficacy on Ethical Behavior in Organizations ABSTRACT Traditional approaches to understanding the ethical context of organizations often focus on ethical work climate, which reflects the collective moral reasoning of organization members. However, such approaches overlook other components of the ethical environment that may influence how ethical judgments translate to ethical behavior. This study extends our understanding of the ethical context of organizations by considering how three distinct aspects of that context—collective moral reasoning (ethical climate), collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy—interact to influence ethical behavior. Results from 117 work units support our hypotheses. Implications and suggestions for future research are discussed. Beyond ethical climate - 3 Events involving broad-based ethical scandals at organizations such as AIG, Goldman Sachs, Countrywide Financial, Fannie Mae, Lehman Brothers, and Siemens AG continue to garner significant attention. Such ethical transgressions are not limited to the corporate world but extend to religious, military, political, and not-for-profit settings as well. In nearly all of these cases, post-mortem accounts point to dysfunctional ethical environments as providing the foundation for illegal and unethical activity (Arbogast 2008, Lewis 2009, Malik 2003, Miller and France 2002, Swartz and Watkins 2003). Collectively, these cases underscore the important role of the ethical context of organizations, in understanding how and why unethical behavior occurs. Research points to ethical work climate as an important component of an organization‘s ethical context, influencing ethical decision making and behavior (Treviño et al. 1998, Victor and Cullen 1988, Martin and Cullen 2006). However, we know little about the process by which its influence unfolds, and the conditions under which this influence is enhanced or mitigated. In this paper we seek to understand more about when—and how—ethical climate will translate to ethical behavior. Ethical work climates reflect the collective moral reasoning of organization members (Victor and Cullen 1988). Thus, strong ethical climates provide employees a foundation for thinking about moral issues. We argue that although organization members may reason effectively about the right thing to do, translating reason into action depends on the moderating effect of two additional contextual factors: collective moral emotion (in the form of collective empathy) and collective ethical efficacy. In short, we argue that the moral reasoning reflected in ethical climate is more likely to translate to ethical behavior if members 1) care about those impacted by their actions (empathy) and 2) believe in their ability to successfully follow through on their decision (efficacy). In all, we suggest ethical climate, collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy interact to create an environment more strongly related to ethical behavior. Our work contributes to the organizational ethics literature in several ways. We seek to improve our understanding of the process by which ethical climate translates to ethical behavior Beyond ethical climate - 4 by considering moderators of the relationship between ethical climate and ethical outcomes. We draw on the social intuitionist model of moral motivation (Leffel 2008) and the affect infusion model (Forgas 1995) in predicting that collective moral emotion provides an important moderator of this relationship. We build on behavioral plasticity theory (Brockner 1988) and social cognitive theory (Bandura 2001) to suggest that collective ethical efficacy also moderates the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior. In all, we present a more comprehensive approach to understanding the impact of ethical climate on ethical behavior by exploring the simultaneous impact of multiple contextual influences on ethical conduct. In particular, we explore the conditions under which the collective moral reasoning reflected in ethical climates will be more—and less—likely to translate to ethical behavior. The Ethical Context of Organizations Ethical Work Climates Organizational work climates reflect the shared perceptions employees hold regarding the policies, practices, and procedures that an organization rewards, supports and expects (Schneider and Reichers 1983). Based on the patterns of experiences and behaviors individuals encounter in their organizations (Schneider et al. 2000), work climates influence employee decisions about what constitutes appropriate behavior by reflecting ―the way things are done around here‖ (Reichers and Schneider 1990, p. 22). As such, work climates provide information that aids employees in making decisions regarding desired and appropriate behavior (Schneider 1990, Zohar and Luria 2005). As a sub-climate of the overall organizational work climate, ethical climate reflects the shared perceptions employees hold regarding the policies, practices, and procedures that an organization rewards, supports and expects with regard to ethics. As such, ethical climates provide valuable guidance to employees by reinforcing the normative systems that guide ethical decision making and behavior (Victor and Cullen 1988, Vidaver-Cohen 1988). Collective moral reasoning as the foundation for ethical climate. Ethical climate influences employee behavior through its impact on ethical decision making (Gaertner 1991, Beyond ethical climate - 5 Victor and Cullen 1988). Therefore, models of ethical climate are often grounded in the moral reasoning processes individuals utilize when making moral judgments and decisions (e.g., Babin et al. 2000, Cullen et al. 1993, Preble and Reichel 1988, Singhapakdi et al. 1996, Victor and Cullen 1988). The dominant approach to conceptualizing and measuring ethical climate is that of Victor and Cullen (1988, see also Cullen et al. 1993). Victor and Cullen‘s framework typifies the focus in the literature of ethical climate as reflective of collective moral reasoning. Specifically, Victor and Cullen suggest ethical climate influences decision making by reflecting prevailing norms regarding appropriate ethical reasoning in the organization. These norms reflect three classes of ethical theory—egoism, benevolence, and principle—which align with Kohlberg‘s (1984) threelevel framework for classifying types of moral reasoning: preconventional (self-focused reasoning), conventional (other-focused reasoning), and postconventional (reasoning based on universalistic principles). The focus of ethical climate research on the reasoning component of the ethical environment is appropriate and accurate, so far as it goes. Organizational norms regarding appropriate forms of moral reasoning should, and do, influence behavior. However, research suggests reason represents only part of the process; emotion plays a critical role as well (e.g., Damasio 1994, Etzioni 1988, Pizarro 2000). In the next section we explore the role of emotion as a part of the ethical context of organizations. In particular, we examine its relationship with reasoning, and its role in activating and focusing attention and cognitive resources on particular ethical issues and events. Moral Emotion Substantial research points to the relationship between reason and emotion during the judgment and decision making process (Damasio 1994). These dual roles of reason and emotion are recognized in the ethical decision making literature as well. Interest in the interaction between reason and emotion reflect a longstanding philosophical debate about the foundations of morality (Rozin et al. 1999), and reinforces historical tensions about the foundations of morality Beyond ethical climate - 6 reflected in rational (e.g., Kant 1789/1959) versus emotional (e.g., Hume 1740/1969) perspectives on ethics. Evidence demonstrates reason and emotion each play important roles in the emergence of moral judgments. For example, Greene et al. (2001) employed functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study differences in brain activity between subjects contemplating different types of moral dilemmas. The researchers presented subjects with the trolley dilemma, in which a runaway trolley car will kill five people in its path unless it is diverted. The five potential victims may be saved only if the participant is willing to throw a switch that will send the trolley onto an alternate track, where a single person who happens to be on the track will be killed instead. In a second version of the dilemma, known as the footbridge dilemma, the participant and a stranger are watching the runaway trolley from a footbridge directly above the tracks. In this case the five lives may be saved only if the participant is willing to push the stranger off the bridge and onto the tracks below, thus derailing the trolley and saving the five lives but in doing so, sacrificing the life of the stranger. Test coders evaluated the footbridge dilemma as a more personal—and thus presumably more emotional—decision. Greene et al. (2001) hypothesized that differences in how personal the moral dilemmas are perceived to be would engage distinct brain centers, reflecting different degrees of emotion and reason associated with making judgments regarding the two dilemmas. Results confirmed their predictions. In two experiments, the more personal dilemma activated brain areas associated with emotional processing, whereas the less personal dilemma activated areas associated with reasoning. Substantial additional neuroscience research supports the dual role of reason and emotion in making moral judgments (e.g., Greene et al. 2004, Koenigs et al. 2007, Moll and de OliveiraSouza 2007) and research from outside the realm of neuroscience supports this view as well. Scholars from psychology (Krettenauer and Eichler 2006, Moore et al. 2008, Valdesolo and DeSteno 2006, Vélez García and Ostrosky-Solís 2006), organizational studies (Gaudine and Thorne 2001, Sekerka and Bagozzi 2007), business ethics (Connelly et al. 2004), and moral Beyond ethical climate - 7 education (Morton et al. 2006, Walker et al. 1995) have all offered theoretical and empirical support that reason and emotion each represent important facets of the moral judgment process. In all, evidence indicates reason and emotion each play important roles in the formation of moral judgments which, in turn, relate to moral behavior. Ethical climate research provides a sound foundation for recognizing the importance of collective moral reasoning as part of the ethical context of organizations (e.g., Victor and Cullen 1988). However, research has been silent on the role of moral emotion. Integrating moral emotion into our thinking about the ethical context of organizations requires that we address three issues. First, can moral emotion be properly construed as a collective construct? Second, does a particular moral emotion represent the most appropriate starting point for modeling the relationship between collective moral reasoning and collective moral emotion? Finally, what role does moral emotion play in the process by which moral reasoning relates to moral behavior? We address each of these issues in turn. Moral Emotion as a Collective Construct The first issue relevant to considering moral emotion as part of the ethical context of organizations is whether emotion may be construed as a collective construct. That is, do collective perceptions of shared emotions exist in work units? Research reveals emotion or affect may manifest itself as a group-level construct, and shared emotions represent a stable characteristic of groups (e.g., Barsade et al. 2000, George 1990, George and James 1993). George (1990) provided an attraction-selection-attrition (ASA) (Schneider 1987) rationale for the process by which collective emotion, which she termed affective tone, emerges in a work unit. George (1990) defines affective tone as ―consistent or homogeneous affective reactions within a group‖ (p. 108), and proposes these collective emotions influence subsequent behavior in the work unit. She argues that individuals with similar affective concerns will be attracted to, selected by, and retained in a particular work environment. This ASA process, combined with group socialization processes that aid newcomers in learning organizational and group norms, Beyond ethical climate - 8 values, goals, and standards (Fisher 1986), will result in an identifiable affective tone in the group. Results support George‘s predictions that shared perceptions of emotion exist within groups, and these shared perceptions are predictive of outcomes like prosocial behavior and absenteeism. She concludes ―it is meaningful to speak of an affective tone of the work group‖ (p. 110). Other research also recognizes the existence of collective emotions. From early sociological work by Durkheim (1915/1965) to more recent work by de Waal (1996) and Keltner and Haidt (1999), scholars have pointed to collective emotion as a meaningful and useful construct. Research shows that work group members come to develop shared emotions in the course of executing their tasks (Barsade and Gibson 1998, Sandelands and St. Clair 1993, Smith and Crandell 1984), and that collective emotions influence organizational outcomes. For example, studies on emotional labor of service workers reveal the emergence of specific affective climates that promote group and organizational objectives (Hochschild 1983, Sutton 1991). In other research, collective emotion has been shown to influence outcomes such as communal identity (Heise and O‘Brien 1993) and group boundary identification (Frijda and Mesquita 1994). In all, researchers have developed sound theoretical and empirical bases for the existence of collective emotions in work units. Empathy as a Collective Moral Emotion The second issue relevant to our integration of moral emotion into the ethical context of organizations is the question of which moral emotion represents the best starting point for our model. A variety of emotions have been conceptualized as moral emotions, but those most commonly identified include empathy, guilt, shame, and gratitude. Empathy is defined as an emotional reaction to another person‘s situation characterized by feelings of compassion, tenderness, and sympathy (Batson, 1991). Guilt is "an individual's unpleasant emotional state associated with possible objections to his or her actions, inaction, circumstances, or intentions. Guilt is an aroused form of emotional distress that is distinct from fear and anger and based on the possibility that one may be in the wrong or that others may have Beyond ethical climate - 9 such a perception" (Baumeister, Stilwell and Heatherton 1994, p. 245). Shame, which is related to but distinct from guilt, is defined as ―a dejection-based, passive, or helpless emotion aroused by self-related aversive events‖ (Ferguson and Stegge 1998, p. 20). Whereas guilt concerns reactions to a particular event, an ashamed person ―focuses more on devaluing or condemning the entire self, experiences the self as fundamentally flawed, feels self-conscious about the visibility of one‘s actions, fears scorn, and thus avoids or hides from others‖ (Ferguson and Stegge 1998, p. 20). Finally, gratitude is defined as an affective reaction to receiving help from other people (McCollough et al. 2001). Although any of these emotions might influence the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior, we suggest empathy represents the best candidate for exploring impact of collective moral emotion for several reasons: It is an other-focused emotion, which is relevant to our interest in ethical behavior. It is associated with a broad range of prosocial behaviors rather than a specific behavioral response. And it is viewed as a foundational moral emotion, one upon which other moral emotions rest. Empathy has been construed as a moral emotion by a broad range of scholars interested in the relationship between emotion and moral decision making. It has been identified by developmental psychologists (Eisenberg 1986, Hoffman 1987), social psychologists (Batson 1991), and philosophers (Blum 1994) as a necessary component of the cognitive functioning required in making moral judgments. Pizarro (2000) argues that empathy represents ―the clearest candidate for being a truly moral emotion‖ (p. 359). It has been singled out as the most prototypic and the most widely discussed of the moral emotions, occurring regularly and reliably in situations involving moral issues (Hoffman 2000). Empathy is conceptualized as consisting of both a cognitive and an affective dimension (Mencl and May 2009, Pizarro 2000). The cognitive dimension involves an individual‘s ability to take the perspective of a target. The affective dimension reflects the emotional or feeling dimension of empathy, specifically compassion and sympathy toward a target (Batson 1990, Davis 1983). Research suggests the affective dimension is most strongly associated with moral Beyond ethical climate - 10 behavior (Moore 1990, Pizarro and Salovey 2002) and it is this affective dimension of empathy on which we focus. Empathy aligns with the definition of moral emotions as those that are ―linked to the interests or welfare either of society as a whole or at least of persons other than the judge or agent‖ (Haidt 2003, p. 276). Mencl and May (2009), for example, identify empathy as ―the moral emotion concerning the welfare of others‖ (p. 208) and Tangney et al. (2007) identify it as an other-oriented emotion. Further, our ultimate interest in this work is to understand the impact of ethical context on ethical behavior. The other-oriented focus of empathy corresponds directly with the definition of ethical behavior, which is doing what is ―…‗right‘ and ‗proper‘ and ‗just‘ in the decisions and actions that affect other people (Hosmer 1996, p. iii).‖ Guilt, shame, and gratitude, on the other hand, represent less attractive choices than empathy for the focal moral emotion in our model. Each of these emotions is only initiated by specific actions on the part of others (Ferguson and Stegge 1998; McCullough et al. 2001). Each activates relatively limited ranges of ethical action (McCullough et al. 2001). Further, guilt and gratitude may be conceptualized as deriving from empathy, in that they rest on empathy for their conceptual foundations (Baumeister et al. 1994, Harris 2003, Hoffman 1982, Lazarus and Lazarus 1994). Empathy, on the other hand, is not dependent on an actor‘s specific behavior (committing an unethical act to feel guilt or shame) or being the recipient of a benevolent act on the part of another. Rather than serving as a reaction to one‘s own ethical violation or an ethical act on the part of another person, it prompts ethical action and has been linked to a broad range of prosocial responses. In all, we believe collective empathy provides the best option for representing collective moral emotion as an antecedent to collective moral reasoning reflected in ethical climate. The Relationship between Ethical Climate and Collective Moral Emotion The third issue relevant to understanding the role of moral emotion is its relationship with ethical climate, and the collective moral reasoning it represents. An ethical work climate reflects its members‘ collective moral norms such as behaving responsibly toward peers, the Beyond ethical climate - 11 organization, and society in general. However, these general moral principles must be activated before they can exert an influence on behavior. A strong sense of collective empathy—and the reactions it engenders toward others such as compassion and sympathy—may serve as this activating agent. Moral emotion serves this role by acting as a centralizing agent, activating and focusing attention and cognitive resources on a particular issue or persons (Pizarro 2000). This suggests that moral emotion interacts with moral reasoning. Two theoretical perspectives describe the process by which moral emotion interacts with moral reasoning to produce ethical behavior. First, Leffel‘s (2008) social intuitionist model of moral motivation suggests moral emotion places individuals in a state of readiness for action, for following through on their moral principles (such as those present in ethical climate). Second, Forgas‘ (1995) affect infusion model describes the cognitive mechanisms by which emotion interacts with reason, and by doing so, strengthens the relationship between moral reasoning and ethical behavior. Leffel‘s (2008) social intuitionist model of moral motivation derives from Haidt‘s (2001) social intuitionist model (SIM) of moral judgment. Haidt argues that research on moral judgment has been dominated by rationalist models, which assume moral judgments are primarily the product of deliberate moral reasoning. He argues that in many cases, however, individuals respond to ethical situations not with careful moral reasoning but rather via quick, automatic responses based on moral intuition. Leffel extends Haidt‘s SIM model of moral judgment to a model of moral motivation, suggesting that these moral intuitions motivate moral behavior through their impact on moral emotion, and moral emotion strengthens (moderates) the influence of moral reasoning. Leffel (2008) argues that moral emotions prompt action on the part of actors. More specifically, moral emotions place individuals into motivational and cognitive states that increase the tendency toward prosocial reactions. Empathy in particular activates these action tendencies toward ethical behavior. The tendency toward action that moral emotion generates is not random. Rather, the Beyond ethical climate - 12 motivational state that results from moral emotion prompts actions that are congruent with extant moral principles (Batson et al 1997 in L; Leffel 2008). The principles that are enacted are those most regularly accessible (Narvaez and Lapsley 2005; in L), such as those present in an ethical climate. In all, the SIM model of moral motivation suggests that moral emotions serve to energize moral action by motivating individuals to respond to ethical events in the environment. Moral emotion places actors in a state of readiness for action and the moral reasoning present in an ethical climate will then propel that action. A second theoretical perspective, the affect infusion model (AIM) (Forgas 1995, Forgas and George 2001) also speaks to the mechanisms by which moral emotion influences the relationship between moral reasoning and ethical behavior. Forgas (1995) defines affect infusion as ―the process whereby affectively loaded information exerts an influence on and becomes incorporated into the judgment process, entering into the judge‘s deliberations and eventually coloring the judgment outcomes‖ (p. 39).2 The impact of affect may be felt in two ways, by influencing what people think and by influencing how they think (Bless 2000, Fiedler 2000, Fiedler and Forgas 1988, Forgas and George 2001). Because we are interested in how emotion influences the process by which moral reasoning relates to ethical behavior, we are interested in the latter, the process by which emotion influences how people think. Affect influences how people think via priming processes that determine how they encode and retrieve information and how that information is used in forming judgments. Four mechanisms are at work: selective attention, selective encoding, selective retrieval, and interpretation of information and situations. Selective attention refers to the process by which affect influences what information an actor focuses on. Individuals often face large quantities of information but tend to focus on information congruent with their affective state. Thus, an empathic state focuses attention on information concerning the welfare of others. Further, individuals spend more time and effort encoding these affect-congruent (in this case, empathy- Beyond ethical climate - 13 related) information, and this selective encoding makes these details more accessible in memory later on. Likewise, affect-congruent information is more easily recalled from memory and this selective recall results in more affect-congruent information available for use when applying moral reasoning principles during the judgment process. Finally, complex settings require individuals to interpret ambiguous information. Affect-congruent information may prime the associations and interpretations made by individuals in a complex setting. That is, emotion serves to strengthen the relationship between moral reasoning and action by shaping what information is encoded and retrieved from memory, and how that information is used in reasoning through moral situations. In all, when individuals are engaged in moral reasoning, the presence of moral emotion (collective empathy) enhances the attention, encoding, recall, and application of empathy-related information, thus strengthening the link between moral reasoning and ethical behavior. Both the social intuitionist model of moral motivation (Leffel 2008) and the affect infusion model (Forgas 1995) describe the processes by which the presence of moral emotion that is congruent with moral reasoning will strengthen the relationship between moral reasoning and behavior. The ethical climate of one‘s organization may support other-oriented reasoning. But in the presence of congruent affect (the other-oriented emotion of empathy), will place individuals in a state of readiness for action, will lead them to attend more closely to otheroriented information, encode that information more carefully, recall it more easily, and be more likely to use it in making a moral judgment. Through these processes, collective empathy will strengthen the relationship between an ethical climate and ethical action. In the previous sections we established that moral emotions may appropriately be conceptualized at the collective level, that empathy represents the best choice for representing the collective moral reasoning of a work unit, and that collective moral emotion moderates the relationship between moral reasoning and moral action. Thus, we hypothesize: H1: Collective moral emotion (empathy) moderates the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior such that the relationship between climate and behavior will be Beyond ethical climate - 14 stronger when empathy is high. Collective Ethical Efficacy as a Moderator of the Relationship between Ethical Climate and Ethical Behavior We now turn our attention to collective ethical efficacy and its role in influencing the relationship between the ethical context of an organization and ethical behavior. Collective efficacy reflects a work unit‘s shared belief in its collective ability to organize and execute successfully the actions required to achieve desired outcomes (Bandura 1997). We argue collective ethical efficacy interacts with moral reasoning in influencing ethical behavior. That is, the collective moral reasoning reflected in an ethical work climate may not be sufficient to elicit ethical behavior. Individuals must also believe they have the ability to execute successfully their desired course of ethical action. Members of a work unit may know the right thing to do. But if they feel confident in their collective ability to bring about the desired outcome, they are more likely to follow through in doing it. Two theoretical perspectives describe the process by which collective efficacy interacts with moral reasoning to produce ethical behavior. Behavioral plasticity theory (Brockner 1988) suggests collective efficacy buffers groups from negative environmental influences that threaten to disrupt the link between knowing the right thing to do and actually doing it. Social cognitive theory (Bandura 2001) describes several specific cognitive processes by which that buffering occurs. Behavioral plasticity theory (Brockner 1988) addresses the issue of why some actors are more plastic—more negatively influenced by detrimental conditions in their work environment—than others. Brockner suggested that self-esteem represents a major cause of observed variation in plasticity, and research has generally supported this assertion. Ganster and Schaubroeck (1991 in Pierce et al 93) found high self-esteem firefighters to be less negatively impacted by the presence of role conflict in their jobs, and Mossholder, Bedian, and Armenakis (1982; in Pierce) found high self-esteem nurses to be less negatively impacted by role stressors in their jobs. Likewise, Pierce, Gardner, Dunham, and Cummings (1993) found high self-esteem Beyond ethical climate - 15 electrical utility employees to be less negatively impacted by role ambiguity, conflict, and overload. Subsequently, Eden and colleagues (Eden and Aviram 1993, Eden and Kinnar 1991, Eden and Zuk 1995) argued that tests of plasticity theory could be extended from self-esteem to self-efficacy. That is, they theorized that like self-efficacy beliefs, like self-esteem, are capable of buffering individuals from contextual influences on the job. Their results indicate selfefficacy does limit plasticity. Jex and Gudanowski (1992) extended plasticity work further by considering the role of collective efficacy as a buffer against negative environmental factors. They found that collective efficacy buffered non-faculty university employees from the harmful impacts of a variety of negative environmental factors including role ambiguity, overloaded work hours, and situational constraints such as lack of supplies, work interruptions, and faulty work instructions. Similarly, we anticipate that collective ethical efficacy will buffer organization members from the influence of negative environmental factors that may derail the relationship between moral reasoning and ethical action. Social cognitive theory (Bandura 2001) addresses the processes by which that buffering may occur. According to social cognitive theory, collective efficacy beliefs impact performance by activating group and individual processes that reflect strategic, operational, and motivational aspects. Strategically, collective efficacy influences decisions about what is important to the group, what challenges to undertake, the type of future the group seeks to achieve, and the plans and strategies the group develops to reach those achievements (Bandura 1997, 2001). With respect to ethics in particular, higher levels of collective ethical efficacy will serve to focus members‘ attention on ethical actions as strategically important, providing a buffer from distractions that may lead to a focus on non-ethical issues. Operationally, collective efficacy affects how the group manages its limited resources. Collective efficacy may influence group decisions about where to apply resources that are required to perform well. Stronger efficacy beliefs also foster more efficient analytic thinking, Beyond ethical climate - 16 which enhances the problem solving process. Groups with strong collective efficacy beliefs also tend to remain task-focused, rather than being distracted by other intrusive thinking that may lead to stress, an emphasis on personal deficiencies, and a diversion of attention away from assessing how best to accomplish goals (Wood and Bandura 1989). With respect to ethics, higher levels of collective ethical efficacy will therefore lead the group to allocate sufficient resources to address ethical issues and to stay focused on those issues with sound problemsolving practices. Motivationally, collective efficacy affects how much effort members put into the group activity. As a result, it influences the group‘s staying power when collective efforts fail to produce quick results or when the group encounters forcible opposition, thus affecting the group‘s vulnerability to discouragement. Efficacy beliefs may also influence whether members think optimistically or pessimistically, whether they are vulnerable to stress and depression, and whether they think in self-enhancing or self-hindering ways (Bandura 1997, 2001). Further, collective efficacy beliefs affect goal-setting processes, in that higher levels of efficacy motivate higher goals and stronger commitment to reaching those goals (Wood and Bandura 1989). With respect to ethics in particular, higher levels of collective efficacy will induce members to be more committed to ethical outcomes and to follow through with ethical decisions and engage in ethical behaviors, even when doing so becomes difficult. Collective efficacy beliefs therefore buffer groups from a variety of contextual factors that have the potential to disrupt the connection between knowing what to do (moral reasoning) and actually doing it. Collective ethical efficacy does so by activating a variety of strategic, operational, and motivational processes that influence whether ethics are deemed important, how resources are allocated to pursuing ethical outcomes, and the motivation and effort exerted toward accomplishing those outcomes. As a result, under conditions of high collective ethical efficacy, the relationship between moral reasoning and ethical outcomes will be enhanced. Therefore, we hypothesize: H2: Collective ethical efficacy moderates the relationship between ethical climate and Beyond ethical climate - 17 ethical behavior such that the relationship between climate and behavior will be stronger when collective ethical efficacy is high. To this point we have suggested both collective moral emotion and collective ethical efficacy moderate the relationship between collective moral reasoning (ethical climate) and ethical behavior. In each case, we suggested that collective moral reasoning alone may not be sufficient to prompt ethical behavior. Rather, higher levels of collective empathy and collective efficacy beliefs each serve to strengthen the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior. Our theorizing for Hypotheses 1 and 2 raises the possibility of a three-way interaction as well. Specifically, we expect the strongest relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior to occur when both collective moral emotion and collective ethical efficacy are high. As outlined above, a strong sense of collective empathy may serve to activate the moral reasoning present in an ethical climate. That is, members of a work unit may know the right thing to do and be motivated to follow through if they feel a strong emotional connection toward the target (empathy). However, they will be most likely to do when they possess strong beliefs in their ability to do so. Therefore we hypothesize: H3: A three-way interaction between ethical climate, collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy exists, such that the interactive effect between ethical climate and collective moral emotion (Hypothesis 1) is stronger when collective ethical efficacy is high. Method Sample We contacted potential participants with the assistance of a team of alumni and students of a large university, utilizing a snowball sampling technique (Tepper 1995). The team identified organizations willing to participate in a study of organizational work climate. Members of the team served as contact persons with participating departments in each organization. We received agreement to participate from 117 departments across 103 organizations, Beyond ethical climate - 18 which included a variety of product- and service-oriented firms, public and private organizations, and for-profit as well as not-for-profit organizations. Employees in each department worked in the same physical work environment and interacted on a consistent basis. The team member distributed surveys to the supervisor and five employees in each department. The 648 individuals who responded to the 702 surveys distributed represented a response rate of 92.3 percent. Respondents‘ mean age was 30.4 years and 54 percent of the sample was male. Respondents averaged 4.2 years of tenure with their organization and 3.1 years of tenure with their department. A contact person within each department received a packet containing surveys to be completed by the five department members and the department supervisor. This contact person also received specific, written instructions regarding the completion of the surveys and procedures for returning them to the researchers. Participants had the option of returning the survey via regular mail or through the contact person. Procedures Surveys completed by employees included measures of demographic characteristics, ethical climate (collective moral reasoning), collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy. Surveys completed by supervisors included these measures plus a measure of ethical behavior in the work unit (Treviño and Weaver 2001), although only their responses to the ethical behavior measures are used in the analyses below. Measures Ethical climate. To measure the type of collective moral reasoning reflected in each work unit‘s ethical climate, we began with the Schminke et al. (2005) 16-item version of the Victor and Cullen (1988) ethical climate scale. Victor and Cullen‘s theory of ethical climate rests on Kohlberg‘s (1984) framework for classifying types of moral reasoning. Kohlberg describes three levels of moral reasoning: preconventional (self-oriented), conventional (otheroriented) and postconventional (universalistic). Research shows few individuals ever reach the postconventional level of moral reasoning (Rest and Narvaez 1994, Treviño et al. 2006, Weber Beyond ethical climate - 19 1990) and thus, shared climate perceptions are unlikely to develop at this level. Rather, shared perceptions are likely to develop at the self-focused (preconventional) and other-focused (conventional) levels at which most individuals engage in moral reasoning. We therefore conceptualize the moral reasoning component of ethical climate as including these two dimensions: self-focused and other-focused reasoning. Pilot testing the 16 items on a separate sample revealed two factors that reflected the anticipated self-focused and other-focused dimensions of collective moral reasoning.3 We selected the five highest-loading items on each factor as measures of the self-focused (α = .89; sample item: People around here are mostly out for themselves) and other-focused (α = .82; sample item: People in my department are actively concerned about their peers‘ interests) dimensions of collective moral reasoning. (All items appear in the appendix.) CFA on the current sample indicated this two-factor model provided a good fit to the data (χ2 = 1371.77, df = 390; RMSEA=.06, CFI = .97, NNFI = .96) and a better fit than an alternative one-factor model in which the self-focused and other-focused items were combined into a single factor (χ2 = 2599.80, df = 399; RMSEA=.11, CFI = .94, NNFI = .93; χ2 change = 1228.03, df = 9, p<.01). Collective moral emotion. Collective moral emotion was measured with a seven-item scale (α = .80) based on the empathic concern dimension of Davis‘s (1980) Interpersonal Reactivity Index. We modified Davis‘s items to reflect a department-level focus. (Sample item: People in my department sympathize with someone who is having difficulties in their job. See appendix for all items.) Employees indicated on a five-point scale the degree to which each item accurately describes their department (1 = Describes my department very well, 5 = Does not describe my department at all). Higher scores reflect higher levels of collective empathy. Collective ethical efficacy. Collective ethical efficacy was measured with a three-item scale (α = .87) adapted from Schwartz (1973). (Sample item: Generally people in my department feel in control over the outcomes when making decisions that concern ethical issues. See appendix for all items.) Employees indicated on a five-point scale the degree to which each item accurately describes their department (1 = Describes my department very well, 5 = Does not Beyond ethical climate - 20 describe my department at all). Ethical behavior. Ethical behavior was measured with Treviño and Weaver‘s (2001) tenitem (α = .91) ethical behavior scale. Supervisors indicated on a five-point scale how often they observed the various unethical behaviors in their work unit over the past year (1 = Never, 5 = Very frequently). (Unethical behaviors include: Unauthorized personal use of company materials or services; padding an expense account; taking longer than necessary to do a job; misuse of on-the-job time; concealing errors; falsifying time/quality/quantity reports; calling in sick just to take a day off; lying to supervisors; stealing from the company; and dragging out work in order to get overtime.) Responses were coded such that higher values reflected more ethical behavior. Control variables. Ethical decision making has been shown to be related to age (Kohlberg 1984), so we controlled for average age of department members. In addition, because shared perceptions of contextual factors are absorbed over time (Schneider and Reichers 1983) we also controlled for average tenure within the work unit. Aggregation Analysis Prior to aggregating individual responses into department-level variables, we assessed whether sufficient agreement exists among department members to justify aggregation of the measures of ethical climate and collective ethical efficacy to the unit level (James 1982, Kozlowski and Klein 2000). We did so by calculating average deviation (ADM) scores (Burke and Dunlap 2002) for each work unit. ADM scores have at least two advantages over alternative means of assessing agreement such as rwg scores. Unlike rwg assessments, they do not require that the researcher generate, a priori, the form of the null response distribution and accurately allocate percentages to each response. And because they yield estimates of agreement in the same units as the original scale, they provide a more direct conceptualization and assessment of agreement (Burke et al. 1999). The ADM index reflects the degree of interrater agreement by measuring the dispersion of responses around the mean member response. Lower scores indicate greater agreement, and Beyond ethical climate - 21 Burke and Dunlap identify .80 as the cutoff value for five-item scales. That is, values below .80 indicate sufficient agreement exists to render aggregation appropriate. We calculated average deviation (ADM) scores (Burke and Dunlap 2002) for each department, across each of the four collective variables that were assessed by employees (the two dimensions of ethical climate, collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy). The overall mean ADM for the four variables, across all departments, was .58. The mean ADM values for the four aggregated variables (ethical climate: self-focused collective moral reasoning, ethical climate: other-focused collective moral reasoning, collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy) were .53, .59, .62, and .60 respectively. All of these values are below the cutoff suggested by Burke and Dunlap (2002). In all, 93% of all departments had average ADM scores across the five variables below the .80 cutoff. We therefore retained all departments in the analyses (Varela et al. 2008). Results Correlations, means, and standard deviations for all variables are presented in Table 1. Scale reliabilities (coefficient alpha) appear on the diagonal. Measurement Model We conducted a series of CFAs on the items comprising the collective perception measures (ethical climate, collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy). Table 2 shows the results of these analyses. We first tested a four-factor model that corresponds with our conceptualization of the four collective variables as distinct from one another. This four factor model provides a good fit to the data (χ2 = 189.93, df = 224; RMSEA=.01, CFI = .99, NNFI = .99). Additional CFAs indicate the four factor model is a better fit than a three factor model that collapses the two collective moral reasoning dimensions of ethical climate into a single dimension (χ2 difference = 499.79, df = 3, p<.01), or a one factor model that collapses all items into a single contextual dimension (χ2 difference = 587.84, df = 6, p<.01). The final one-factor model presented in the Measurement Model section serves as the test of the Harman one-factor CFA solution (Podsakoff et al. 2003). This single method-driven Beyond ethical climate - 22 factor does not fit the data well, providing evidence that common method bias does not represent a serious concern. Hypothesis Testing We test our hypotheses via moderated regression using OLS. Results of the regression analyses are presented in Table 3. Hypothesis 1. We predicted that collective moral emotion (empathy) moderates the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior such that the relationship between climate and behavior is stronger when empathy is high. Because we have two measures of ethical climate (other-focused reasoning and self-focused reasoning) our model includes two interaction terms that provide tests of this hypothesis. Results of this lower-order interaction test are qualified by the result of the higher-order interaction proposed in Hypothesis 3, but the first (other-focused reasoning x collective moral emotion) reveals a significant interaction effect in the predicted direction, thus supporting Hypothesis 1. The second (self-focused reasoning x collective moral emotion) was not significant. These results provide partial support for Hypothesis 1; collective empathy strengthens the relationship between other-focused reasoning and ethical behavior, but does not moderate the relationship between self-focused reasoning and ethical behaviour. Hypothesis 2. We predicted that collective ethical efficacy moderates the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior such that the relationship between climate and behavior is stronger when efficacy is high. As in Hypothesis 1, we have two measures of ethical climate that result in two interaction terms providing tests of this hypothesis. Again, results of this lower-order interaction test are qualified by the result of the higher-order interaction proposed in Hypothesis 3, but the first (other-focused reasoning x collective ethical efficacy) was not significant. The second (self-focused reasoning x collective ethical efficacy) reveals a significant interaction in the predicted direction. These results provide partial support for Hypothesis 2. They suggest collective ethical efficacy strengthens the relationship between selffocused reasoning and ethical behavior, but does not moderate the relationship between other- Beyond ethical climate - 23 focused reasoning and ethical behaviour. Hypothesis 3. Finally, we predicted a three-way interaction in which the moderating effect of collective moral emotion on the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior is enhanced by the presence of collective ethical efficacy. Results in Table 3 show strong support for this hypothesis. Coefficients for both three-way interactions (other-focused reasoning x moral emotion x ethical efficacy, and self-focused reasoning x moral emotion x ethical efficacy) are significant and in the predicted direction. In both cases, higher levels of collective efficacy strengthened the moderating effect of collective moral emotion. Figure 1 illustrates the three-way interaction for other-focused ethical climate. The interaction effect in the bottom panel (high efficacy) is more pronounced than the interaction effect in the top panel (low efficacy). Similarly, Figure 2 illustrates the three-way interaction for self-focused ethical climate. The interaction effect in the bottom panel (high efficacy) is again more pronounced than the interaction effect in the top panel (low efficacy). In all, our results indicate ethical climate, collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy interact in influencing ethical behavior. In particular, the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior is strengthened by the presence of collective empathy and collective ethical efficacy and is at its strongest when both collective empathy and collective ethical efficacy are high. Discussion Recent scandals provide evidence of the influence organization environments may have in sanctioning or promoting unethical behavior. Existing research offers useful insights about the impact ethical work climates exert on ethical behavior. However, our study suggests an ethical climate—and the shared perceptions regarding appropriate moral reasoning it represents—may not, in and of itself, be enough to ensure ethical behavior. Organization members may know the right thing to do. But unless they also care about the targets of their behavior (empathy) and believe they have the capacity to carry out their desired actions (efficacy), ethical behavior is unlikely to occur. By exploring the impact of all three contextual Beyond ethical climate - 24 factors simultaneously—and the manner in which they interact with one another—our study provides a more comprehensive look at the impact the ethical context of organizations has on ethical behavior. Implications for Research The research presented here extends work on ethical climate in several ways. It suggests ethical climate alone may be insufficient to ensure ethical outcomes. It explores the conditions under which ethical climate might be expected to relate more strongly to ethical outcomes. And it introduces collective moral emotion and collective ethical efficacy into the conversation as moderators of the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior. Each has implications for research. The relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior. Our results highlight the importance of adopting a more comprehensive view of how ethical climate impacts ethical outcomes in organizational settings. Research shows ethical climate to be a consistent predictor of employee attitudes (Deshpande, 1996; Martin & Cullen, 2006). However, its ability to predict ethical behavior has been less robust. Meta-analytic results indicate evidence of a link between ethical climate and ethical behavior has been modest, with a mean correlation less than .16 (Martin & Cullen, 2006). This suggests the link between ethical climate and behavior might be a conditional one, dependent on the presence of other factors. Our model, which proposes the presence of collective moral emotion and collective moral efficacy will enhance the relationship, explained half-again more variance in ethical behavior (R2 = .26) than typical in previous work. Our results therefore suggest that considering ethical climate in isolation paints an incomplete picture. In practice, understanding ethical climate and its influence requires a broader conceptualization of the organizational ethical context, and how those contextual factors relate to and shape the impact of ethical climate. Our results provide evidence that ethical climates are related to the ethical behavior of organization members. However, they show the effect is dependent on both collective moral emotion and collective ethical efficacy. This leads us to speculate about other factors that might Beyond ethical climate - 25 influence the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior. We anticipate constructs like moral disengagement (Bandura 1999) may moderate the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior as well. Moral disengagement has been shown to play an important part in process by which individual moral judgment are formed. Further, it has previously been conceptualized as a collective-level variable via a process Ashforth and Anand (2003) term normalization. Thus, it represents a potentially potent additional contextual factor. Similarly, moral identity (Aquino and Reed 2002) has also been shown to play an important role in the moral judgment process. Because collective identity has been the focus of research as well, it represents another potential candidate for consideration as an ethics-related contextual factor that might help to understand more fully the impact of ethical climate. Beyond these ethics-specific constructs, organization climate researchers have begun to explore other factors that moderate the impact of organizational climates on outcomes. Leader characteristics like justice orientation (Liao and Rupp 2005), structural characteristics like formalization (Zohar and Luria 2005), and organizational characteristics like culture (Yang et al. 2007) have all been found to affect the strength of the relationship between work climates and organizational outcomes and might influence the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior as well. One particularly interesting potential moderator is climate strength, the degree of agreement between unit members with respect to their climate perceptions (Lindell and Brandt 2000). Climate strength reflects the degree of variation in individual perceptions around the average climate score of the work unit. Research indicates climate strength plays an important moderating role in the relationship between a variety of work climate types and outcomes. For example, climate strength has been shown to moderate the relationship between innovation climate and job attitudes (González-Romá et al. 2002), between procedural justice climate and team performance and absenteeism (Colquitt et al. 2002), and between service climate and customer satisfaction experiences (Schneider et al. 2002). However, although climate strength is Beyond ethical climate - 26 one of the most-studied moderators of the relationship between work climate and outcomes (Kuenzi and Schminke 2009), the theoretical process by which it influences these relationships is still unclear, and this is an issue worthy of attention in ethics research. Further, future work might benefit from considering not only the impact of climate strength, but the strength of other contextual features of the ethical environment (e.g., collective moral emotion and collective ethical efficacy) as well. However, doing so will require a more general theory of the process by which strong agreement about collective assessments like these will strengthen the relationship between contextual factors and outcomes. The role of emotion. Our results also point to the importance of emotion in the process by which ethical climate relates ethical behavior. Work climates influence behavior through their effect on the decision making processes of employees (Schneider 1990, Zohar and Luria 2005), and ethical climate research has embraced moral reasoning as the foundation for ethical climate and its impact on employee ethical decision making (e.g., Victor and Cullen 1988). However, research demonstrates the importance of emotion in the decision making process as well, and our results point to the important role collective emotion plays in this process. Our study demonstrates collective moral emotion—in the form of empathy—exists at the work unit level. It also suggests emotion activates the moral reasoning principles reflected in ethical climate (especially when paired with collective ethical efficacy), thereby increasing the likelihood that ethical behavior will emerge. Further, it shows the moderating effect of emotion is, in turn, contingent on the presence of collective ethical efficacy. In other words, emotion matters more when individuals also believe in their ability to execute successfully the actions required to achieve desired ethical outcomes. These results are consistent with work showing empathy facilitates the ethicality of decision making at the individual level (Mencl and May 2009) and research demonstrating that empathy enhances recognition of an ethical situation by raising moral awareness and improving people‘s evaluation of the possible adverse effects their actions and decisions may have on others (Vetlesen 1994). As such, our results lay the groundwork for establishing moral emotion as a Beyond ethical climate - 27 key component of the ethical context of organizations. Doing so raises important questions about the role that other collective emotions might play in the ethical decision making and behavior process. For example, scholars have explored the impact of positive emotions like excitement, enthusiasm, and pride on a variety of organizational outcomes. Positive emotions have been found to have an impact on positive organizational outcomes like organizational citizenship behaviors (Joireman et al. 2006), teamwork, and prosocial behavior (e.g., Barsade and Gibson 2007). Such results suggest positive emotions, as well as moral emotions, may play a role in shaping ethical decision making and behavior as well and thus, represent sound candidates for exploring other emotional influences on the relationship between moral reasoning and behavior. Our work focuses on the positive role played by collective moral emotion in activating the moral reasoning reflected in ethical climate. However, research demonstrates emotion may not always constitute a positive influence on ethical decision making and behavior. For example, Ratner and Herbst (2005) found that an unfavorable outcome of a good decision (defined as one identified as successful in the past or expected to perform better in the future) led individuals to shift away from that decision due to negative emotional responses to the outcome. Research has also shown that in some situations empathy in particular has the potential to bias decision making in a negative way. For example, study by Batson et al. (1995) demonstrated the potential for empathy to overpower fairness rules, thus leading to unfair outcomes. At the collective level, results like these suggest collective emotion, although influential, may not always represent a positive force in an organization. Further research is necessary to understand the conditions under which collective emotions in general, and empathy in particular, may serve to facilitate or hinder moral judgment and in turn, ethical action. Our study does not explore the antecedents of any of the contextual factors represented in our model. However, a fuller understanding of the role of collective moral emotion would include an examination of its antecedents, and the conditions under which it is most likely to emerge. For example, Greene et al. (2001) found people respond more emotionally when Beyond ethical climate - 28 engaging in decision-making that involves more personal scenarios. Likewise, Pizarro (2000) explains that emotions become more important when people are faced with uncertainty or ambiguity. More work is needed to understand whether factors like these may influence the strength of emotional responses in a situation and therefore, their potential for impacting the influence of climate on outcomes. The role of efficacy. The concept of ethical efficacy is new to the ethics literature. Mitchell and Palmer (2010) outlined a conceptual case for its relevance in organizational studies, and Mitchell, Palmer, and Schminke (2008) provided preliminary empirical support for the proposition that ethical efficacy strengthens individuals‘ self-regulatory mechanisms, which may mitigate the impact of supervisory unethical behavior on employees. Beyond this limited focus on individual-level effects, the ethics literature is silent with respect to efficacy, as either an individual or collective construct. Our results point to collective ethical efficacy as playing a three-tiered role in the ethical context of organizations. First, consistent with Hypothesis 2, efficacy strengthened the relationship between one dimension of ethical climate and ethical behavior. Following through on knowing the ethical course of action depends at least partially on believing in one‘s ability to be successful in doing so. Second, consistent with Hypothesis 3, collective ethical efficacy strengthened the positive moderating effect of collective moral emotion on the relationship between ethical climate and ethical behavior. Results suggest, as hypothesized, that moral emotion does act as a centralizing agent, activating and focusing attention and cognitive resources on a particular ethical issue or event. However, translating even this combination of moral reasoning (ethical climate) and a congruent collective moral emotion into ethical behavior is contingent on individuals‘ belief in their ability to follow through successfully on their intended course of action. Third, although not hypothesized, collective ethical efficacy also exerted a significant positive direct effect on ethical behavior. This is consistent with one aspect of social cognitive theory (Bandura 2001), which argues that collective efficacy beliefs represent a powerful direct Beyond ethical climate - 29 determinant of performance. Research offers some support for this direct relationship. A metaanalysis of 42 studies revealed average effect size of the relationship between collective efficacy and group performance of .41 (Gully et al. 2002). Similarly, a study by Campion et al. (1993) found that of 19 work group characteristics, a group‘s belief in its ability to be effective was the strongest predictor of group performance. Our results indicate ethical efficacy may exert a similar direct effect on ethical performance, which suggests further thinking about the complete role collective ethical efficacy may play in influencing ethical behavior. Implications for Practice Our research has practical implications as well. These involve a broad recognition of the multidimensional nature of ethical context, identifying an explicit role for emotion as part of the ethical environment, and exploring the role of assessment in managing the ethical context. Ethical context is multidimensional. Recognizing that ethical context consists of more than one component (i.e. ethical climate), and that the components exert unique effects on one another and on organizational outcomes, presents additional challenges for practitioners. However, it also offers the opportunity to diagnose the organization‘s ethical context in a more fine-grained—and ultimately, productive—way. Consider an organization concerned about an unacceptable level of unethical behavior among its members. If a deficient ethical context is suspected as a cause, any attempt to diagnose the problem using a measure that taps only the moral reasoning foundation of climate may fail on at least two counts. First, such a measure may be incapable of detecting deficiencies in terms of collective moral emotion or collective moral efficacy in the organization. Second, any effects that are detected may be inappropriately attributed to deficient collective moral reasoning. Diagnosing weaknesses in ethical context requires the ability to assess each component independently. Doing so paves the way for creating interventions aimed at improving specific problem areas, critical in making the most of limited resources. Ethical context involves emotion. In recent years, interest in emotion has proliferated among organization scholars. Emotion has taken a center-stage position in research on a variety Beyond ethical climate - 30 of organizational issues including leadership (Bono et al. 2007), justice (Chebat and Slusarczyk 2005), decision making (Maitlis and Ozcelik 2004), well-being (Updegraff et al. 2004), teams (Kelley and Barsade 2001), and negotiation (van Dijk et al. 2008). Thriving research streams in these areas have placed emotion at the center of practical discussions of how to manage organizations and the people in them. However, emotion has largely escaped the notice of researchers and practitioners interested in the ethical context of organizations. Our research points to collective moral emotion as an important component of ethical context and thus, it suggests those tasked with managing the ethical environment of organizations attend explicitly to its presence. Fortunately, research on the role emotion plays in individual moral judgments is plentiful. Therefore, practitioners wishing to consider the role of emotion at the collective level have a strong empirical and theoretical base upon which to build. Assessing the ethical context. Finally, our approach to conceptualizing the ethical context of organizations resulted in a multidimensional instrument for assessing ethical context. This instrument offers several potential benefits for practitioners and researchers alike, compared to previous instruments such as that of Victor and Cullen (1988). First, it is multidimensional, thereby tapping a broader range of the conceptual space that defines ethical context. Second, as a 20-item scale that assesses four dimensions of ethical context (ethical climate based on selffocused moral reasoning, ethical climate based on other-focused moral reasoning, collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy), it is more parsimonious than either the 26-item Victor and Cullen (1988) or the 36-item Cullen et al. (1993) instruments that assess ethical climate only. Finally, it is more predictive of ethical behavior than these alternative scales. The absolute value of correlations between our ethical context measures and ethical behavior are .33, .19, .20, and .18 for ethical climate based on self-focused moral reasoning, ethical climate based on other-focused moral reasoning, collective moral emotion, and collective ethical efficacy, respectively. All of these are stronger relationships than the .16 average correlation between ethical climate and behavior for studies utilizing the Victor and Cullen scales (Martin and Cullen 2006). Beyond ethical climate - 31 Limitations Our study reflects several limitations. First, all data were collected by survey. Although we utilized two separate sources for our predictor and outcome variables (employees and supervisors, respectively), and many key variables resulted from aggregating individual scores, all were collected via a single method. Results of a Harman one-factor test did not suggest common method bias represented a significant problem in our data. Second, our data were collected in a cross-sectional fashion. Therefore, our analyses do not provide strong test of the causal relationships implicit in our model. Rather, they can only provide a test of statistical moderation that is supported by the theoretical foundations of the model. A final limitation is that results were based on self-reports. However, studies of emotion and cognitive processes depend heavily on self-reports, and evidence generally supports their validity (Spector 1992), especially in work involving ethics-related issues. For example, metaanalysis of integrity measures suggests that self-report measures may even be beneficial, in that they tend to improve criterion-related validity in this type of research (Ones et al. 1993). In this study, we assured participants anonymity and did not ask them to disclose unethical conduct they may have personally committed, but rather to share their perceptions of the environment in which they work. However, we still need to be alert to the fact that self-reports may be vulnerable to self-enhancement biases. Conclusion The central goal of this research was to provide a more comprehensive view of how ethical climate influences ethical behavior than currently exists in the literature. Our model does this, and represents a more comprehensive conceptualization of ethical context of organizations, the relationships between its components, and its impact on ethical behavior than appears in previous work. As such, we suggest it provides a more sound theoretical footing for increasing our understanding of the process by which ethical climates shape ethical behavior. In all, our results provide new insights about the structure of ethical climate and the contextual factors that Beyond ethical climate - 32 enhance or mitigate its effects. However, the results also point to a number of important questions for researchers interested in understanding the structure and impact of the ethical context of organizations. Further research is needed to clarify these additional issues. Beyond ethical climate - 33 Footnotes 1 Forgas (1995) uses affect as a generic label that includes both emotions and moods. He defines moods as ―low-intensity, diffuse, and relatively enduring affective states without a salient antecedent cause and therefore little cognitive content (e.g. feeling good or bad)‖ and emotions as affective states that ―are more intense, short-lived, and usually have a definite cause and clear cognitive content‖ (p. 41). Although Forgas notes the principles underlying the AIM apply to both types of affective states, our focus is on emotion, in particular moral emotion. Emotions like empathy, guilt, shame, and gratitude are commonly conceptualized as having a moral foundation (McCullough et al. 2001), whereas moods are not.] 2 The AIM suggests affect infusion will be most pronounced in situations that call for higher levels of open, constructive processing such as that reflected in heuristic or substantive processing. Forgas (1995, 2002) argues that complex organizational settings are likely to induce substantive processing, paving the way for affect priming as the process by which affect will infuse judgments. We believe issues of organizational ethics are often likely to induce substantial processing. However, our study is not capable of detecting whether ethical issues trigger substantive versus heuristic processing, nor whether affect-as-information or affect priming processes are at work. Thus, we do not offer a specific prediction regarding the type of infusion process in operation. 3 Two hundred sixty-four surveys were distributed among 173 MBA students from a large state university who were employed full time, and 101 entrepreneurs and employees of start-up, high tech companies, both in the Southeastern United States. The 174 individuals who responded to the surveys represented a response rate of 66%. Fifty-eight percent of the sample was male and had a mean age of 29 years (SD = 7.60). Respondents averaged 3.2 years of tenure with their organizations (SD = 3.6) and 2.0 years of tenure with their departments (SD = 1.7). Factor analysis (principal components with oblique rotation) yielded two factors with eigenvalues greater than one. The first factor explained 46.0% of the variance and contained five items reflecting self-focused moral reasoning (alpha = .91). Factor loadings ranged from .65 to Beyond ethical climate - 34 .95, with no cross-loadings greater than .21. The second factor explained 11.8% of the variance and contained seven items reflecting other-focused moral reasoning (alpha = .89). Factor loadings ranged from .63 to .71, with no cross-loadings greater than .25. Beyond ethical climate - 35 References Aquino, K., A. Reed. 2002. The self-importance of moral identity. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 83 1423-1440. Arbogast, S.V. 2008. Resisting Corporate Corruption: Lessons in Practical Ethics from the Enron Wreckage. M&M Scriviner Press, Salem, MA. Ashforth, B.E., V. Anand. 2003. The normalization of corruption in organizations. Res. Organ. Behavior 25 1-52. Babin, B.J., J.S. Boles, D.P. Robin. 2000. Representing the perceived ethical work climate among marketing employees. J. Acad. Marketing Sci. 28 345-58. Bandura, A. 1986. Social Foundations of Thought and Action. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Bandura, A. 1991. Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action. W.M. Kurtines, J.L. Gewirtz, eds. Handbook of Moral Behavior and Development: Theory, Research and Applications, Vol. 1. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 71-129. Bandura, A. 1993. Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning. Ed. Psych. 28 117-148. Bandura, A. 1997. Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. Freeman, NY. Bandura, A. 1999. Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality Soc. Psych. Rev. 3 193-209. Bandura, A. 2000. Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current Directions Psych. Sci. 9 75–78. Bandura, A. 2001. Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Rev. Psych. 52 1–26. Bandura, A., D. Cervone. 1986. Differential engagement of self-reactive influences in cognitive motivation. Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes 38 92-113. Barsade, S.G., D.E. Gibson. 1998. Group emotion: A view from top and bottom. M.A. Neale, E. A. Mannix, eds. Research on Managing Groups and Teams, Vol. 1. JAT Press, Stamford, CT, 81-102. Beyond ethical climate - 36 Barsade, S.G., D E. Gibson. 2007. Why does affect matter in organizations? Acad. Management Perspectives 21 36-59. Barsade, S.G., A.J. Ward, J.D.F. Turner, J.A. Sonnenfeld. 2000. To your heart's content: A model of affective diversity in top management teams. Admin. Sci. Quart. 45 802-836. Batson, C.D. 1990. How social an animal? The human capacity for caring. Amer. Psych. 45 336– 346. Batson, C.D. 1991. The Altruism Question: Toward a Social-Psychological Answer. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. Batson, C.D., T.R. Klein, L. Highberger, L. Shaw. 1995. Immorality from empathy-induced altruism: When compassion and justice conflict. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 68 1042-1054. Baumeister, R.F., A.M. Stillwell, T.F. Heatherton. 1994. Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psych. Bull. 115 243-267. Bless, H. 2000. Moods and general knowledge structures: Happy moods and their impact on information processing. J.P. Forgas, ed. Feeling and Thinking: The Role of Affect in Social Cognition. Cambridge University Press, New York, 201-222. Blum, L.A. 1994. Moral Perception and Particularity. Cambridge University Press, New York. Bono, J.E., H.J. Foldes, G. Vinson, J.P. Muros. 2007. Workplace emotions: The role of supervision and leadership. J. Appl. Psych. 92 1357-1367. Burke, M.J., W.P. Dunlap. 2002. Estimating interrater agreement with the Average Deviation (AD) index: A user‘s guide. Organ. Res. Methods 5 159-172. Burke, M.J., L.M. Finkelstein, M.S. Dusig. 1999. On Average Deviation indices for estimating interrater agreement. Organ. Res. Methods 2 49-68. Campion, M.A., G.J. Medsker, A.C. Higgs. 1993. Relations between work group characteristics and effectiveness. Implications for designing effective work groups. Personnel Psych. 46 823-850. Campion M.A., G.J. Medsker, A.C. Higgs. 1993. Relations between work group characteristics and effectiveness: Implications for designing effective work groups. Personnel Psych. 46 Beyond ethical climate - 37 823-850. Cervone, D., N. Jiwani, R. Wood. 1991. Goal setting and the differential influence of selfregulatory processes on complex decision-making performance. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 61 257-266. Chebat, J., W. Slusarczyk. 2005. How emotions mediate the effects of perceived justice on loyalty in service recovery situations: An empirical study. J. Bus. Res. 58 664– 673. Chen, G., P.D. Bliese. 2002. The role of different levels of leadership in predicting self- and collective efficacy: Evidence for discontinuity. J. Appl. Psych. 87 549–556. Clore, G.L., N. Schwarz, M. Conway. 1994. Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. R.S. Wyer, T.H. Srull, eds. Handbook of Social Cognition, 2nd ed. Vol.1, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 323-419. Colquitt, J., R. Noe, C. Jackson. 2002. Justice in teams: Antecedents and consequences of procedural justice climate. Personnel Psych. 55 83-109. Connelly, S., W. Helton-Fauth, M.D. Mumford. 2004. A managerial in-basket study of the impact of trait emotions on ethical choice. J. Bus. Ethics 51 245- 267. Cullen, J.B., B. Victor, J.W. Bronson. 1993. The Ethical Climate Questionnaire: An assessment of its development and validity. Psych. Rep. 73 667-674. Damasio, A.1994. Descartes‘ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. Penguin Book, New York. Davis, M.H. 1980. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog Selected Documents Psych. 10 85. Davis, M.H. 1983. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 44 113-126. De Georges, R.T. 1999. Business Ethics, 5th ed. Prentice Hall, New Jersey. De Rivera, 1984. The structure of emotional relationships. P. Shaver, ed. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 5. Emotions, Relationships, and Health. Sage, Beverly Hills, CA, 116-145. Beyond ethical climate - 38 de Waal, F.B.M. 1996. Good Natured. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Durkheim, E. 1915/1965. The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life. J.W. Swain, Trans. The Free Press, New York. Original work published 1915. Eisenberg, N. 1986. Altruistic Emotion, Cognition, and Behavior. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J. Eisenberg, N. 2000. Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Ann. Rev. Psych. 51 665-698. Etzioni, A.1988. The Moral Dimension: Toward a New Economics. Free Press, New York. Ferguson T.J., H. Stegge. 1998. Measuring guilt in children: A rose by any other name still has thorns. J. Bybee, ed. Guilt and Children, Academic, San Diego, 19-74. Ferrell, O.C., J. Fraedrich. 1997, Business Ethics: Ethical Decisions Making and Cases. Houghton Miffin, Boston, MA. Fiedler, K. 1991. On the task, the measures and the mood in research on affect and social cognition. J.P. Forgas, ed. Emotion and Social Judgments. Pergamon Press, Elmsford, NY, 83-104. Fiedler, K. 2000. Towards an integrative account of affect and cognition phenomena. J.P. Forgas, ed. Feeling and Thinking: The Role of Affect in Social Cognition. Cambridge University Press, New York, 223-252. Fiedler, K., J.P. Forgas. Eds. 1988. Affect, Cognition, and Social Behavior: New Evidence and Integrative Attempts. Hogrefe, Toronto, 44-62. Fisher, C.D. 1986. Organizational socialization: An integrative review. K.M. Rowland, G.R. Ferris, eds. Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management, Vol. 4. JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, 101–145. Forgas, J.P. 1995. Mood and judgment: The Affect Infusion Model (AIM). Psych. Bull. 117 3966. Forgas, J.P. 2002. Towards understanding the role of affect in social thinking and behavior. Psych. Inquiry 13 90-102. Forgas, J.P., J.M. George. 2001. Affective influences on judgments and behavior in organizations: An information processing perspective. Organ. Behav. Human Decision Beyond ethical climate - 39 Processes 86 3-34. Frijda, N.H., B. Mesquita. 1994. The social roles and function of emotions. S. Kitayama, H. Marcus, eds. Emotion and Culture: Empirical Studies of Mutual Influence. American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., 51-87. Gaertner, K. 1991. The effect of ethical climate on managers' decisions. R.M. Coughlin, ed. Morality, Rationality and Efficiency: New Perspectives on Socio-economics. M.E. Sharpe, Armonk, NY, 211- 226. Gaudine, A., L. Thorne. 2001. Emotion and ethical decision-making in organizations. J. Bus. Ethics 31 175-188. George, J.M. 1990. Personality, affect, and behavior in groups. J. Appl. Psych. 75 107-116. George, J.M., I.R. James. 1993. Personality, affect, and behavior in groups revisited: Comment on aggregation, levels of analysis, and a recent application of within and between analysis. J. Appl. Psych. 78 798-804. Gibson, C.R. 1999. Do they do what they believe they can? Group efficacy and group effectiveness across tasks and cultures. Acad. Management J. 42 138-152. Gibson, C.B. 2001. Me and us: Differential relationships among goal-setting training, efficacy and effectiveness at the individual and team level. J. Organ. Behav. 22 789 – 808. Gibson, C.R., P.C. Early. 2007. Collective cognition in action: Accumulation, interaction, examination, and accommodation in the development and operation of group efficacy beliefs in the workplace. Acad. Management Rev. 32 438-458. González-Romá, V., J. Peiró, N. Tordera. 2002. An examination of the antecedents and moderator influences of climate strength. J. Appl. Psych. 85 956-970. Greenberg, M.D. 2009. Perspectives of Chief Ethics and Compliance Officers on the Detection and Prevention of Corporate Misdeeds: What the Policy Community Should Know. Rand, Santa Monica, CA. Greene, J.D., L.E. Nystrom, A.D. Engell, J.M. Darley, J.D. Cohen. 2004. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron 44 389–400. Beyond ethical climate - 40 Greene, J.D., R.B. Sommerville, L.E. Nystrom, J.M. Darley, J.D. Cohen. 2001. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293 2105-2109. Gully, S.M., K.A. Incalcaterra, A. Joshi, J.M. Beaubien. 2002. A meta-analysis of team efficacy, potency, and performance: Interdependence and level of analysis as moderators of observed relationships. J. Appl. Psych. 87 819-832. Guzzo, R.A., P.R. Yost, R.J. Campbell, G.P. Shea. 1993. Potency in groups: Articulating a construct. British J. Soc. Psych. 32 87-106. Haidt J. 2003. Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. C.L. Keyes, J. Haidt, eds. Flourishing: Positive Psychology and the Life Well-Lived. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, 275–89. Harris, N. 2003. Reassessing the dimensionality of the moral emotions. British J. Psych. 94 457473. Heider. F. 1958. The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. Wiley, New York. Heise, D.R., J. O‘Brien. 1993. Emotion expression in groups. M. Lewis, J.M. Haviland, eds. Handbook of Emotions. Guilford Press, New York, NJ, 489-498. Hochschild, A.R. 1983. The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human Feeling. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA. Hoffman, M.L. 1982. Development of prosocial motivation: Empathy and guilt. N. Eisenberg, ed. The Development of Prosocial Behavior. Academic Press, San Diego, CA. Hoffman, M.L. 1987. The contribution of empathy to justice and moral judgment. N. Eisenberg, J. Strayer, eds. Empathy and its Development. Cambridge University Press, New York, 47-80. Hoffman, M.L. 2000. Empathy and Moral Development: Implications for Caring and Justice. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Hoffman, M.L. 2002. How automatic and representational is empathy, and why. Behav. Brain Sci. 25 38-40. Hosmer, L.T. 1996. The Ethics of Management. Irwin, Chicago. Beyond ethical climate - 41 Hume, D. 1740/1969. A Treatise of Human Nature. Penguin, London. Original work published 1739 & 1740. James, L.R. 1982. Aggregation bias in estimates of perceptual agreement. J. Appl. Psych. 67 219-229. James, L.R., J.M. Brett. 1984. Mediators, moderators, and tests for mediation. J. Appl. Psych. 69 307-322. James, L.R., S.A. Mulaik, J.M. Brett. 2006. A tale of two methods. Organ. Res. Methods 9 233244. Joireman, J., D. Kamdar, D. Daniels, B. Duell. 2006. Good citizens to the end? It depends: Empathy and concern with future consequences moderate the impact of a short-term time horizon on organizational citizenship behaviors. J. Appl. Psych. 91 1307-1320. Jones, T.M. 1991. Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Acad. Management Rev. 18 366-395. Jung, D.I., J.J. Sosik. 2003. Group potency and collective efficacy. Group Organ. Management 28 366-391. Kant, I. 1789/1959. Foundations of the metaphysics of morals L. W. Beck, Trans. Bobbs-Merrill, Indianapolis, IN. Original work published 1789. Kelly, G.A. 1955. The Psychology of Personal Constructs. Norton, New York. Kelley, J.R., S.G. Barsade. 2001. Mood and emotions in small groups and work teams. Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes 86 99–130. Keltner, D., J. Haidt. 1999. Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition & Emotion 13 505-521. Koenigs, M., L. Young, R. Adolphs, D. Tranel, F. Cushman, M. Hauser, A. Damasio. 2007. Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgments. Nature 446 908– 911. Kohlberg, L. 1984. The Psychology of Moral Development. Harper & Row, San Francisco, CA. Kozlowski, S.W.J., K.J. Klein. 2000. A multilevel approach to theory and research in Beyond ethical climate - 42 organizations. K.J. Klein, S.W.J. Kozlowski, eds. Multilevel Theory, Research, and Methods in Organizations. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA, 3-90. Kuenzi, M., M. Schminke. 2009. Assembling fragments into a lens: A review, critique, and proposed research agenda for the organizational work climate literature. J. Management 35 634-717 Krettenauer, T., D. Eichler. 2006. Adolescents' self-attributed moral emotions following a moral transgression: Relations with delinquency, confidence in moral judgment and age. British J. Developmental Psych. 24 489-506. Lazarus, R. S. 1991. Emotion and Adaptation. Oxford University Press, New York. Lazarus, R.S., B.N. Lazarus. 1994. Passion and Reason: Making Sense of Our Emotions. Oxford University Press, New York. Lee, C., J-L. Farh. 2004, Joint effects of group efficacy and gender diversity on group cohesion and performance. Appl. Psych.: An Internat. Rev. 53 136-154. Lewis, H.B. 1971. Shame and Guilt in Neurosis. International University Press, Madison, CT. Lewis, M. 1992. Shame: The Exposed Self. Free Press, New York. Lewis, M. 2009. The man who crashed the world. Vanity Fair 588(August) 98-103, 136-139. Liao, H., D. Rupp. 2005. The impact of justice climate and justice orientation on work outcomes: A cross-level multifoci framework. J. Appl. Psych. 90 242-256. Lindell, M, C. Brandt. 2000. Climate quality and climate consensus as mediators of the relationship between organizational antecedents and outcomes. J. Appl. Psych. 85 331348. Lindsley, D.H., D.J. Brass, J.B. Thomas. 1995. Efficacy-performance spirals: A multilevel perspective. Acad. Management Rev. 29 645 – 678. Maitlis, S., H. Ozcelik. 2004. Toxic decision processes: A study of emotion and organizational decision making. Organ. Sci. 15 375-393. Malik, O. 2003. Broadbandits. John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ. Martin, K. D., J.B. Cullen. 2006. Continuities and extensions of ethical climate theory: A meta- Beyond ethical climate - 43 analytic review. J. Bus. Ethics 69 175-194. Mayer, R.C., M.B. Gavin. 2005. Trust in management and performance: Who minds the shop while the employees watch the boss? Acad. Management J. 48 874-888. McCullough, M.E., S.D. Kilpatrick, R.A. Emmons, D.B. Larson. 2001. Is gratitude a moral affect? Psych. Bull. 127 249-266. Mencl, J., D.R. May. 2009. The effects of proximity and empathy on ethical decision-making: An exploratory investigation. J. Bus. Ethics 85 201–226. Miller, L., D. France. 2002. March 4th. Sins of the fathers. Newsweek 139(9) 42-46, 48-49, 5152. Mitchell, M. S., N. F. Palmer. 2010. The managerial relevance of ethical efficacy. M. Schminke, ed. Managerial Ethics: Managing the Psychology of Morality. Routledge, New York, 89-108. Mitchell, M. S., N. F. Palmer, M. Schminke. 2008. Understanding the influence of ethical efficacy. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Anaheim, CA. Moll, J., R. de Oliveira-Souza. 2007. Moral judgments, emotions and the utilitarian brain. Trends Cognitive Sci. 11 319-321. Moore, B.S. 1990. The origins and development of empathy. Motivation Emotion 14 75–80. Moore, A.B., B.A. Clark, M.J. Kane. 2008. Who shalt not kill? Individual differences in working memory capacity, executive control, and moral judgment. Psych. Sci. 19 549-557. Morton, K.R., J.S. Worthley, J.K. Testerman, M.L. Mahoney. 2006. Defining features of moral sensitivity and moral motivation: Pathways to moral reasoning in medical students. J. Moral Ed. 35 387-406. Ones, D.S., C. Viswesvaran, F.L. Schmidt. 1993. Meta-analysis of integrity test validities: Findings and implications for personnel selection and theories of job performance [Monograph]. J. Appl. Psych. 78 679-703. Piers, G., M. Singer. 1971. Shame and Guilt: A Psychoanalytic and Cultural Study. Norton, NY. Beyond ethical climate - 44 (Original work published 1953) Pizarro, D. 2000. Nothing more than feelings? The role of emotions in moral judgment. J. Theory Soc. Behaviour 30 355-376. Pizarro, D.A., P. Salovey. 2002. Being and becoming a good person: The role of emotional intelligence in moral development and behavior. J. Aronson, ed. Improving Academic Achievement. Academic Press, San Diego, TX, 247–266. Podsakoff P.M., S.B. MacKenzie, J.Y. Lee, N.P. Podsakoff. 2003. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psych. 88 879–903. Preble, J.F., A. Reichel. 1988. Attitudes towards business ethics of future managers in the U.S. and Israel. J. Bus. Ethics 7 941-949. Preston, S.D., F.B.M. de Waal. 2002. Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav. Brain Sci. 25 1-72. Prussia, G.E., A.J. Kinicki. 1996. A motivational investigation of group effectiveness using social-cognitive theory. J. Appl. Psych. 81 187–198. Ratner, R.K., K.C. Herbst. 2005. When good decisions have bad outcomes: The impact of affect on switching behavior. Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes 96 23-37. Reichers, A.E, B. Schneider. 1990. Climate and culture: an evolution of constructs. Schneider, B. ed. Organizational Climate and Culture. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA, 5-39. Rest, J. R., D. Narvaez. 1994. Moral development in the professions: Psychology and applied ethics. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. Riggs, M.L., P.A. Knight. 1994. The impact of perceived group success-failure on motivational beliefs and attitudes: A causal model. J. Appl. Psych. 79 755-766. Rozin, P., L. Lowery, S. Imada, J. Haidt. 1999. The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). J. Personality Soc. Psych. 76 574-586. Sandelands, L.E., L.L. St. Clair. 1993. Toward an empirical concept of group. J. Theory Soc. Beyond ethical climate - 45 Behavior 23 423-458. Schminke, M., M.L. Ambrose, D.O. Neubaum. 2005. The effect of leader moral development on ethical climate and employee attitudes. Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes 97 135-151. Schneider, B. 1987. The people make the place. Personnel. Psych. 40 437-453. Schneider, B. 1990. Organizational Climate and Culture. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA. Schneider, B., D. Bowen, M. Ehrhart, K. Holcombe. 2000. The climate for service: Evolution of a construct. N.M. Ashkanasy, C.P. Wilderom, M.F. Peterson, eds. Handbook of Organizational Culture and Climate. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, 21-36. Schneider, B., M.G. Ehrhart, D.M. Mayer, J.L. Saltz, K. Niles-Jolly. 2005. Understanding organization-customer links in service settings. Acad. Management J. 48 1017- 1032. Schneider, B., A. Reichers. 1983. On the etiology of climates. Personnel Psych. 36 19-41. Schneider, B., A. Salvaggio, E. Subirats. 2002. Climate strength: A new direction for climate research. J. Appl. Psych. 87 220-230. Schwartz, S.H. 1973. Normative explanations of helping behavior: A critique, proposal and empirical test. J. Experiment. Soc. Psych. 9 349-364. Schwarz, N., G.L. Clore. 1988. How do I feel about it? The informative function of affective states. K. Fiedler, J.P. Forgas, eds. Affect, Cognition, and Social Behavior, Hogrefe, Goettinger, Germany, 44-62. Sekerka, L.E., R.P. Bagozzi. 2007. Moral courage in the workplace: moving to and from the desire and decision to act. Bus. Ethics: A European Rev. 16 132-149. Singhapakdi, A., S.J. Vitell, K.C. Rallapalli, K.L. Kraft. 1996. The perceived role of ethics and social responsibility, a scale development. J. Bus. Ethics 15 1131-1140. Smith, K.K., S.D. Crandell. 1984. Exploring collective emotion. Amer. Behavioral Sci. 27 813828. Spector, P.E. 1992. Summated Rating Scale Construction: An Introduction, Vol. 82. Sage, Newbury Park, CA. Beyond ethical climate - 46 Stajkovic, A.D., D. Lee, A.J. Nyberg. 2009. Collective efficacy, group potency, and group performance: Meta-analyses of their relationships, and test of a mediation model. J. Appl. Psych. 94 814-823. Sutton, R.I. 1991. Maintaining norms is about emotional expression: The case of bill collectors. Admin. Sci. Quart. 36 245-268. Tangney, J.P. 1998. How does guilt differ from shame? J. Bybee, ed. Guilt and Children. Academic, San Diego, 1–17. Tangney, J.P., J. Stuewig, D.J. Mashek. 2007. Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Rev. Psych. 58 345-372. Tepper, B. 1995. Upward maintenance tactics in supervisory mentoring and no mentoring relationships. Acad. Management J. 38 1191-1205. Treviño, L.K., K.D. Butterfield, D.L. McCabe. 1998. The ethical context in organizations: Influences on employee attitudes and behaviors. Bus. Ethics Quart. 8 447-476. Treviño, L.K., G.R. Weaver. 2001. Organizational justice and ethics program ―follow-through‖: Influences on employees‘ harmful and helpful behavior. Bus. Ethics Quart. 11 651-671. Treviño, L.K., G.R. Weaver, S.J. Reynolds. 2006. Behavioral ethics in organizations: A review. J. Management 32 951-990. Updegraff, J.A., S.L. Gable, S.E Taylor. 2004. What makes experiences satisfying? The interaction of approach–avoidance motivations and emotions in well-being. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 86 496–450. Valdesolo, P., D. DeSteno. 2006. Manipulations of emotional context shape moral judgment. Psych. Sci. 17 476-477. Van Dijk, E., G.A. van Kleef, W. Steinel, I. van Beest. 2008. A social functional approach to emotions in bargaining: When communicating anger pays and when it backfires. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 94 600–614. Van Zomeren, M., R. Spears, C.W. Leach, A.H. Fischer. 2004. Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group Beyond ethical climate - 47 efficacy. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 87 649-664. Varela, O.E., M.J. Burke, R.S. Landis. 2008. A model of the emergence and dysfunctional effects of emotional conflict in groups. Group Dynam.: Theory, Res. Practice 12 112126. Vélez García, A., F. Ostrosky-Solís. 2006. From morality to moral emotions. Internat. J. Psych. 41 348-354. Vetlesen, A.J. 1994. Perception, Empathy, and Judgment: An Inquiry Into the Preconditions of Moral Performance. Pennsylvania State University Press, University Park, PA. Victor, B., J.B. Cullen. 1988. The organizational bases of ethical work climates. Admin. Sci. Quart. 33 101-125. Vidaver-Cohen, D. 1998. Moral climate in business firms: A conceptual framework for analysis and change. J. Bus. Ethics 17 1211-1226. Walker, L. J., R.C. Pitts, K.H. Hennig, M.K. Matsuba. 1995. Reasoning about morality and reallife moral problems. M. Killen, D. Hart, eds. Morality in Everyday Life: Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Berkeley, CA, 371-407. Weber, J. 1990. Managers‘ moral reasoning: Assessing their responses to three moral dilemmas. Human Relations 43 687-702. Wood, R., A. Bandura. 1989. Social cognitive theory of organizational management. Acad. Management Rev. 14 361-384. Yang, J., K.W. Mossholder, T.K. Peng. 2007. Procedural justice climate and group power distance: An examination of cross-level interaction effects. J. Appl. Psych. 92 681-692. Zaccaro, S.J., V. Blair, C. Peterson, M. Zazanis. 1995. Collective efficacy. J.E. Maddux, ed. Selfefficacy, Adaptation, and Adjustment: Theory, Research, and Application. Plenum, New York, 305-328. Zohar, D., G. Luria. 2005. A multilevel model of safety climate: Cross-level relationships between organization and group-level climates. J. Appl. Psych. 4 616-628. Beyond ethical climate - 48 Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelationsa Mean s.d. 1 Ethical climate: Self-focused reasoning 3.12 0.98 (.89) Ethical climate: Other-focused reasoning 2.69 0.87 -.26** (.82) Collective moral emotion 2.56 0.76 -.46** .53** (.80) Collective ethical efficacy 2.52 0.89 -.38** .55** .58** (.87) Ethical behavior 2.25 0.78 -.33** .19* .20** .18** a 2 Scale reliabilities (Chronbach‘s α) in parentheses on the diagonal * = p < .05; ** = p < .01 3 4 5 (.91) Beyond ethical climate - 49 Table 2 Fit Indices for Measurement Model Model χ 90% C.I. d 2 f RMSEA for RMSEA CFI NNFI Four factor 189.83 224 .01 .01-.02 .99 .99 Three factor 499.79 227 .11 .09-.12 .95 .95 One factor 777.67 230 .16 .14-.17 .89 .88 Beyond ethical climate - 50 Table 3 Results of regression analysis for ethical behavior Model Constant b Std. β Error 3.10** (.90) .00 (.00) .03 Mean age -.02* (.01) -.22* Ethical climate: Other-focused reasoning -.07 (.17) -.04 -.34** (.13) -.27** Collective moral emotion -.03 (.18) -.02 Collective ethical efficacy .44* (.19) .25* 1.32** (.48) .29** Self-focused reasoning x collective moral emotion -.14 (.26) -.05 Other-focused reasoning x collective ethical efficacy .36 (.36) .11 Self-focused reasoning x collective ethical efficacy .49* (.25) .21* Collective moral emotion x collective ethical efficacy 1.66** (.38) .43** Other-focused reasoning x moral emotion x ethical efficacy 1.15* (.67) .21* Self-focused reasoning x moral emotion x ethical efficacy 1.59** (.46) .39** Mean department tenure Ethical climate: Self-focused reasoning Other-focused reasoning x collective moral emotion R2 = .26 * = p < .05; ** = p < .01; one-tailed b = unstandardized coefficients; β = standardized coefficients Beyond ethical climate - 51 Figure 1 Three-way interaction between other-focused ethical climate, collective emotion, and collective ethical efficacy 5 4 3 2 1 0 5 4 3 2 1 0 5 5 4 4 3 3 2 2 1 Beyond ethical climate - 52 Figure 2 Three-way interaction between self-focused ethical climate, collective emotion, and collective ethical efficacy 5 4 3 2 1 0 5 4 3 2 1 0 55 44 33 22 Beyond ethical climate - 53 Appendix: Scale Items Ethical climate: Self-focused collective moral reasoning 1. In my department people‘s primary concern is their personal benefit. 2. People in my department think of their own welfare first when faced with a difficult decision. 3. People in my department are very concerned about what is best for them personally. 4. People around here protect their own interest above other considerations. 5. People around here are mostly out for themselves. Ethical climate: Other-focused collective moral reasoning 6. What is best for everyone in the department is the major consideration. 7. In my department it is expected that you will always do what is right for society. 8. People around here have a strong sense of responsibility to society and humanity. 9. People in my department are actively concerned about their peers‘ interests. 10. The most important concern is the good of all the people in the department. Collective moral emotion 1. People in my department sympathize with someone who is having difficulties in their job. 2. For the most part, when people around here see that someone is treated unfairly, they feel pity for that person. 3. When people in my department see someone being treated unfairly, they sometimes don‘t feel much pity for them. (R) 4. In my department people feel sorry for someone who is having problems. 5. Sometimes people in my department do not feel very sorry for others who are having problems. (R) 6. Others misfortunes do not usually disturb people in my department a great deal. (R) 7. People around here feel bad for someone who is being taken advantage of. Collective ethical efficacy 1. When necessary, people in my department take charge and do what is morally right. 2. Generally people in my department feel in control over the outcomes when making decisions that concern ethical issues. 3. People around here are confident that they can do the right thing when faced with moral dilemmas. 53