Cognitive age as a criterion explaining senior tourists’ motivations Ana M. González, Carmen Rodrı́guez, Mauro R. Miranda and Miguel Cervantes Ana M. González, Carmen Rodrı́guez, Mauro R. Miranda and Miguel Cervantes are all based at the University of Leon, Leon, Spain. Abstract Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to gain a broader knowledge of the concept of cognitive age and its use as a variable when segmenting the market of people aged over 55. Design/methodology/approach – A proposal for measuring this variable was developed, the dimensions used being: actions, interests, feelings, health, thinking and expectations. A personal survey of 400 individuals aged over 55 who had been involved in some tourist travel in the last year was undertaken. Findings – A principal component analysis resulted in the extraction of a single factor labeled ‘‘cognitive age’’. The outcome of the cluster analysis undertaken was the establishment of two segments, i.e. active livers and stable passives. The technique of multiple correspondence analysis reflected differences both in motivations for tourism and in the behavior as tourists of the two segments. Research limitations/implications – It should be pointed out that there were insufficient repetitions of the methodology to indicate reliably whether it is consistent and that difficulties were encountered in gathering information from the group investigated. Practical implications – The results obtained permit it to be stated that cognitive age enriches knowledge about senior tourists; hence, it seems to be of interest to take it into consideration when establishing marketing strategies directed towards this group. Originality/value – After its overview of the various methodologies relating to cognitive age, this investigation is one of the few applications of this criterion to segmentation of senior citizen tourists undertaking journeys of longer duration. Furthermore, it is the sole piece of research so far undertaken to investigate its usefulness in an individualized manner. Keywords Tourism, Older consumers, Motivation (psychology), Cognition, Consumer behaviour Paper type Research paper Introduction Misapprehensions about the real conditions in which they find themselves at this stage in their lives often distort the image of senior customers (Dychtwald and Flower, 1989). People frequently consider them to be dependent, weak, lonely and physically or mentally limited (see Appendix 1) (Dychtwald and Gable, 1990; Clary, 1991; Grande Esteban, 1993). However, most reach this age in full possession of all their capacities, and whenever they think or speak about old age, they are actually referring to a stage still distant in the future, a fact that is worth stressing. This state of affairs shows that the stereotype is more an exception than a rule (Vilchez, 1994), since a new generation of older folk has emerged that is composed of satisfied, self-sufficient individuals not suffering from any serious limitations (Cooper and Marshall, 1984; Martin et al., 1998). Received: March 2008 Revised: May 2008 Accepted: July 2008 PAGE 148 j Hence, modifications are taking place in the characteristics of senior citizens. These changes, together with the well-known aging of the population, form one of the principal demographic alterations that will affect the tourism industry, justifying interest in this sector of the market, as senior citizens now often lead a very active form of life. This activity means INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH j VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009, pp. 148-164, Q Emerald Group Publishing Limited, ISSN 1750-6182 DOI 10.1108/17506180910962159 they can, and do, engage in a range of leisure activities, among which they include travel, since they have the time and the money needed to enjoy them (Clary, 1991; Hagan and Uysal, 1993; Lanquar, 1995; Loverseed, 1993, 2000; Viant, 1993; Dunn, 1994; Van Harssel, 1994; Morgan and Levy, 1996; Chazaud, 1997; Mathur et al., 1998; Javalgi et al., 1999; Bigné et al., 2000; Dann, 2000, 2001; Wang et al., 2005). Nevertheless, even if people work with the concept of a sector made up of seniors, they should remember that seniors are not a homogeneous group in terms of demography, lifestyles and health, or psychological, social and behavioral patterns (Moschis, 1994, 2003). Indeed, they still constitute a market segment that few have investigated, understanding which is of more and more interest. This interest comes from an academic perspective, since there is virtually no theoretical framework in existence for research applying to this age group. Interest also comes from a business perspective, because this is a group of consumers that is steadily growing in terms of both size and purchasing power. Their heterogeneity is so extensive that there is not even any consensus as to how to describe them in the marketing literature. Various different ways of referring to this group are in use, the most frequent in the last ten years or so being the following: B ‘‘older market’’ (Sherman et al., 2001; Carrigan et al., 2004); B ‘‘mature market’’ (Shoemaker, 2000; Moschis et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2005); B ‘‘50-plus market’’ (Silvers, 1997); B ‘‘senior market’’ (Reece, 2004); and B ‘‘maturing market’’ (Whitford, 1998). The same vagueness affects the thresholds adopted as the boundary for old age, since authors fix its onset at anywhere from 50 up to 65 years of age. This vagueness highlights the relevance of discussions about taking chronological age as a variable when analyzing this market. The questioning of chronological age arises because numerous studies confirm that seniors identify themselves as being younger than that age, rejecting the characteristics typically related to their chronological age and manifesting behaviors influenced by an age differing from that which calculations using their birth certificates would give (Havighurst and Albrecht, 1953; Tuckman and Lorge, 1954; Blau, 1956, 1961; Kutner et al., 1956; Phillips, 1956, 1957, 1961; Tuckman and Lavell, 1957; Jyrkilä, 1960; Tuckman et al., 1961; Zola, 1962; Rosow, 1967, 1974; Guptill, 1969; Barak et al., 1981). These studies back up the claims various authors make that traditional socio-demographic criteria are not sufficiently explanatory in view of the growing personalization of individual consumption patterns in developed economies (Lambin, 1995; González Fernández and Bello Acebrón, 2002). Cognitive age as a criterion for tourist market segmentation Trends in measurement of cognitive age When it comes to analyzing and proposing strategies for more mature consumers, people increasingly commonly accept that self-perceived age should replace chronological age. This age is a component of the self-images people have and as a construct authors term it ‘‘cognitive age’’ (Barak et al., 1981; Barak and Stern, 1986; Dychtwald and Flower, 1989; Moschis et al., 1993). In the 1950s scholars made the first attempts to measure cognitive age by means of a very simple method – that of just asking the interviewees to what age category they thought they belonged. This technique is called a single-item scale (Blau, 1956), and the responses to it differ from one study to another, although the commonest categories are young, middle-aged and seniors. Its chief defects are the lack of knowledge of the concepts that people have of each age category, the influence of negative stereotypes of seniors present in society (which may bias responses; Rosow, 1974), and lastly the fact that it is a measurement handling a complex phenomenon in an overly simplistic way (Stephens, 1991). Table I shows the main pieces of research work that use this sort of scale. j j VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH PAGE 149 Table I Application of cognitive age scales: single item and semantic differential Author Year Scale Sector/age group Comments Guptill 1969 Semantic differential Sociology; 55 to 87 Notes the aspects most significant in measuring age identification Kastenbaum et al. 1972 Personal agea Gerontology; 20 to 69 Studies the relationship between chronological age and the concepts of personal, interpersonal and consensual age Baum and Boxley 1983 Single item Psychology; 74.5 (average age) Suggests that being involved in some personal activity is related to feeling younger Guiot 2001b Personal age (modified) Consumer behavior; 20 to 75 Analyzes the use of cognitive age when segmenting the mature women’s clothing market Note: aThis scale is the scale on which Barak and Schiffman (1980) base their scale. It is a scale derived from the age decade scale Later, another scale came into use, the semantic differential scale (Guptill, 1969; George et al., 1980; Mutran and George, 1982), originally consisting of 50 bipolar adjectives (Osgood and Suci, 1955). This scale has evolved into simpler versions, permitting evaluation of self-perceived age by means of a calculation of the distance between the opinion people have about themselves and their opinions with respect to the other two reference points (see Table I). Apart from the difficulty of administering this type of test, it has little empirical underpinning, a fact worthy of highlighting. George et al. (1980) further stress this aspect, recommending the development of models allowing a description of the causes and consequences of age identification. Finally, the most widely accepted scale emerged, termed the ‘‘age decade scale’’ (Barak et al., 1981), and using as its base the personal age concept of Kastenbaum et al. (1972). Its measurement looks at four dimensions, relating to: 1. the age people feel they are; 2. the age they think they look; 3. the age that reflects their pattern of actions; and 4. the age that their interests show. All these are dimensions evaluated in decades of age. Once the figures for each of the dimensions listed are available, calculation of the cognitive age simply involves taking the arithmetic mean of the set. This scale constitutes the basis for major contributions and innovations to the system for measuring cognitive age in recent work (Guiot, 1999, 2001a, b; Barak et al., 2001; Catterall and Maclaran, 2001; Gwinner and Stephens, 2001; Sherman et al., 2001; Szmigin and Carrigan, 2001a). Table II lists applications of this methodology. With respect to the research mentioned previously, an overview of three pieces of work specifically in the field of tourism and using the age decade scale is of particular relevance to the current investigation. These are the studies by Cleaver and Muller (1998), Mathur et al. (1998) and Szmigin and Carrigan (2000). The research by Cleaver and Muller (1998) verifies the theoretical connection between cognitive age and reasons for traveling as a new way of segmenting the tourism market. In this instance the age decade scale uses only two dimensions out of the original four, i.e. age felt and activities common to the age group (felt and activities), and measurement of the motives for vacation travel is by means of nine items adapted from Beard and Ragheb (1980, 1983). According to the results of this work, several reasons for traveling and personal values are associated with the two cognitive age variables considered. Tourism products j j PAGE 150 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 Table II Applications of the age decade scale Author Year Sector/age group Comments Barak and Schiffman 1980 Consumer behavior (hair care); 55 þ Barak and Gould 1985 Barak 1987 Nelson and Smith 1988 Barak and Rahtz 1990 Chua et al. 1990 Goldsmith and Heiens 1992 Wilkes 1992 Van Auken and Barry 1995 Johnson 1996 Villanueva Orbáiz 1997 Barak and Rahtz 1999 Szmigin and Carrigan 2000 Szmigin and Carrigan 2001 Gwinner and Stephens 2001 Catteral and Maclaran 2001 Barak et al. 2003 Goulding and Shankar 2004 Mathur and Moschis 2005 Ramos Soler 2005 Eastman and Iyer 2005 The variable ‘‘cognitive age’’ yields data that cannot be obtained exclusively from the use of chronological age Women as consumers of leisure; 30-69 Considers six concepts of age: chronological, cognitive, ideal, youthfulness, discrepancy and disparity Gerontology; 30-69 Confirms the reliability and validity of the variable cognitive age. Investigates the area researched by Kastenbaum et al. (1972) in greater depth Consumer behavior (advertising for the Combines the concepts of cognitive age and elderly); 55 and above cooperation, highlighting differences within the overall grouping of seniors Generic; 60 and above Examines two measures of age – cognitive age and youth – and their relationship with a set of characteristics Consumer behavior; 55 and above Examines the relationship between the predictors for successful aging and cognitive age Gerontology; 21-92 Validates two hypotheses: the greater the chronological age, the smaller the proportion of it represented by subjective age; and the greater the variability in perceptions of the age of people in their thirties Consumer behavior (fashion); 60-79 Measurement properties of cognitive age and its relationship with specific variables Generic; 55-90 Studies the validity of the construct cognitive age by means of three methods: semantic differential, Likert and proportionality (ratios) Consumer behavior; 18-87 Cognitive age can be a key variable in marketing travel, recreation, leisure and other related services Consumer behavior; 50 þ Cognitive age strengthens and improves analyses of the behavior patterns of seniors Generic; 60-94 Examines two specific dimensions: ‘‘perceived youthfulness’’ and ‘‘feeling old’’. Uses four concepts of age: chronological, cognitive, perceived youthfulness and feelings of being old Consumer behavior (holiday The study could not validate the hypothesis that destinations); 50-90 innovatory behavior by consumers is linked to their perception of their age Consumer behavior; 50-75 Study of the perception and use of time by seniors, together with how this influences their purchasing habits Consumer behavior; 55-91 Coherence of cognitive age as a criterion explaining consumer behavior Behavior of the elderly; 50 and above Argues that age and aging are socially structured, hence attitudes towards aging are not fixed but changing Behavior; 20-59 Compares satisfaction with internal age in four different cultures in Asia and Africa. This measurement is the difference between the cognitive age and the desired age of individuals Consumer behavior; 30-40 Examines the cultural movement of ‘‘rave’’ festivals and the influence of cognitive age on the migration of people from groups of higher ages towards such events Consumer behavior; 18-92 A replication and extension of the work by Wilkes (1992). Factors influencing self-perception and behavior Advertising; 65 and above Studies self-perceived age and life-style as criteria for segmentation and application to advertising Internet; 65-85 Seniors with the lowest cognitive ages are more likely to use the internet in comparison with those with higher cognitive ages; j j VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH PAGE 151 that offer adventure, excitement or challenges are more likely to be attractive to seniors who feel like, or wish to act like, younger folk. Another contribution in relation to the field of tourism is the work by Mathur et al. (1998). Their research incorporates two additional dimensions, ‘‘health’’ and ‘‘thinking’’, alongside the original four of the age decade scale. This study relates to the concept of new-age elderly of Schiffman and Sherman (1991), contrasting with the traditional elderly. The study attempts to identify the new-age segment as a market within the overall group comprising the old, to pinpoint the values that may distinguish the two groups of elderly and to spot the principal needs and lifestyles of older consumers. In its definition of cognitive age, six dimensions are used to elicit an age decade: ‘‘I feel’’, ‘‘I look’’, ‘‘My health is’’, ‘‘I do most things’’ and ‘‘I think’’, all followed by the phrase ‘‘as though I am in my . . . ’’, and ‘‘My interests are mostly those of a person in his/her . . . ’’. The results indicate that the cluster of new-age elderly have as characteristics their greater independence, their desire to be more in control of the situation and their greater satisfaction with life than is the case for traditional elderly. The findings also reveal that more advantageous way of achieving segmentation of the seniors market for travel is the use of variables related to these values. Investigations by Szmigin and Carrigan (2000) find it is not possible to validate the central hypothesis of the study, i.e. the idea that consumers with lower cognitive ages would be more likely to show innovatory behavior. They adopted the four dimensions of the scale as proposed by Barak (1987) and measured innovatory behavior by consumers by means of the Domain-Specific Innovativeness Scale of Goldsmith and Hofacker (1991), which shows itself to be valid and reliable in previous applications (Goldsmith and Flynn, 1992; Goldsmith et al., 1993). After comparison of the outcomes from the two scales, the results would indicate that there is no significant relationship between cognitive age and innovatory behavior on the part of consumers. Proposal of a model for measuring cognitive age In view of the small number of specific contributions to the tourism market by investigative projects using cognitive age, together with the need to widen understanding of the concept in question, still rather limited among those studying marketing, this research project aims at establishing a model for measurement and validating it in the sector concerned. From an overview of information referring to scales and their use, the age decade scale emerges as having most extensive fine-tuning over the course of time and hence is the scale with the most testing (see Table III). The model proposed here envisages modifications to the original version by Barak et al. (1981). It is somewhat similar to the proposals of Mathur et al. (1998) and of Clark et al. (1999), but even so has substantial adjustments relative to them. In the first place, the scale suggests incorporation of the dimension termed ‘‘expectations’’, justifying this inclusion by the fact that many of the ideas that other dimensions reflect (actions, interests, feeling, health and thoughts) depend on the expectations people have from life, hence influencing these perceptions. In this way the tool gains a more dynamic character, with some reflection of the perspectives on life each individual reveals. Secondly, elimination of the dimension termed ‘‘appearance’’ seems necessary. This omission is the outcome of a considerable body of literature, in which there are a number of references indicating its extreme closeness to chronological age, and because it reflects more of a social aspect, while cognitive age attempts to grasp an internal belief (Barak et al., 1981; Barak, 1987; Schiffman and Sherman, 1991; Goldsmith and Heiens, 1992; Wilkes, 1992; Villanueva Orbáiz, 1997; Barak, 1998; Clark et al., 1999; Barak et al., 2001; Guiot, 2001a, b). Thus, the scale of measurement for cognitive age proposed in this research has as its basis the use of six items reflecting each of the following dimensions: 1. actions; 2. interests; 3. feelings; 4. health; j j PAGE 152 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 Table III Modified applications of the age decade scale Author Year Sector/age group Comments Montepare and Lachman 1989 Gerontology; 14-83 Shows that the subjective age identified varies from adolescence through to old age, changing systematically. As chronological age increases, women show a greater discrepancy between real and subjective age Mathur et al. 1998 Consumer behavior (travel and leisure); 55 and above Investigates the values and lifestyles of the senior citizen sector and their likely potential for the travel and leisure market Cleaver and Muller 1998 Consumer behavior (psychological motives for traveling); 56-93 Associates self-perceptions of seniors with the reasons for a journey and personal values. Uses a two-dimensional scale Clark et al. 1999 Behavior of mature women; 55 and above Physical activity can be used as a method for improving and maintaining health and satisfaction with life. The cognitive age shown reflects this fact Sherman et al. 2001 Consumer behavior; 55 and above Comparison of the habits of two groups of seniors and the influence of sex. Verification of the validity and reliability of the NAVO scalea Wei 2005 Consumer behavior; 20 and above Investigates socio-demographic aspects and cognitive age in relation to innovatory behavior by consumers Notes: aNew-Age Value Orientation (NAVO) scale. This scale is a set of characteristics drawn up by Mathur et al. (1998) and Sherman et al. (2001) to distinguish traditional seniors from ‘‘new-age’’ seniors 5. thinking; and 6. expectations. These are items that endeavor to define the age group to which individuals believe they belong. In respect of the age threshold for consideration in this study of seniors, after a review of the published literature the decision was to cover people aged 55 years and above, in the light of the satisfactory results numerous authors obtain in this way (Barak et al., 1981; Javalgi et al., 1999; Moschis, 1992, 1996, 2003; Marı́n Vidal, 1993; Viant, 1993; Gibson, 1995; Ortega Martı́nez, 1989; Cai, 1997; Dunne and Turley, 1997; Mathur et al., 1998; Bai et al., 1999; Faranda and Schmidt, 1999; Shoemaker, 2000; Hsu and Lee, 2002; Reece, 2004; Wang et al., 2005). Research method Objectives, hypotheses and sample design This work goes further into the concept of cognitive age and its use in segmenting the market composed of people aged 55 years and above, as also to relate it to motivations and behavior in respect of tourism. There are also a set of more precise objectives closely linked with the principal aim: B analysis of the cognitive age of tourists; B grouping of individuals into segments as a function of their cognitive ages; B study of the relationship between the cognitive age of individuals and their motivations for tourism by segments; and B an investigation of tourist behavior on long-duration journeys of each segment. The hypotheses adopted for the research are the following: H1. Significant differences occur between the cognitive age and the chronological age of seniors. j j VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH PAGE 153 H2. Different cognitive ages highlight the existence of varying motivations for tourism among seniors. In light of the objectives noted above, the study performed a personal survey of individuals aged 55 years of age and above who had undertaken some tourist travel in the last year. This did not just sample individual households, but also leisure centers for seniors, because there is one in each of the zones into which the city analyzed is divided and also because they bring together high concentrations of the population under study. The technical details of the study are specified in Table IV. The study uses a questionnaire with four sections: 1. habits regarding tourism involving long-duration journeys, i.e. destination, type of accommodations used, type of transport, corporation organizing the journey and specific reasons for each of the journeys referred to; 2. motivations for tourism; 3. cognitive age; and 4. demographic and socio-economic characteristics. Hypothesis testing To attain the aims of the current piece of research, various multi-variable statistic techniques were used. Firstly, principal component analysis was the technique that rendered down the information from the six dimensions going to make up cognitive age, measured in accordance with the model proposed. The outcome is the extraction of a single factor explaining approximately 82 percent of the total variance, consequently labeled ‘‘cognitive age’’, as it clearly represents that variable very accurately. The figures for the statistics justifying the analysis described are noteworthy, being 0.93 for Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin and 2,503,640 for Bartlett’s test. In relation to analysis of the validity and reliability of the cognitive age scale being proposed, the value for Cronbach’s a reaches 0.95. This figure indicates an excellent level of internal consistency for the scale and corroborates the relevance of the new dimension included in the scale. Since one of the objectives of the investigation is to segment the market by using the cognitive age variable, cluster analysis in accordance with Ward’s method was used, the variable for differentiating individuals being the cognitive age factor emerging from the prior analysis. The results emerging clearly show a distinction between two groups within the sample. One of these reaches a total of 223 subjects, termed ‘‘active livers’’, presenting a large majority of individuals whose cognitive ages are lower. The other group, amounting to a total of 177 individuals, are those people termed ‘‘stable passives’’, their cognitive ages being higher, while this group also has a much larger percentage of subjects who feel as old as their real age. Table IV Technical details of study j Universe 45,851 people of both sexes, aged 55 years and above and living in Leon, a Spanish city Sample 400 people aged over 55 having undertaken some form of travel for tourist purposes during the previous 12 months Technique Personal survey in the form of a structured questionnaire Sampling procedure Multi-stage random sampling with proportional assignment by district, sex and age Sampling error 5 percent (p ¼ q ¼ 50) Confidence level 95.50 percent (K ¼ 2s) j PAGE 154 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 Once this natural grouping emerges, validation of it is necessary. The technique in use is discriminant analysis. An application of this method also serves to check the first hypothesis, that there are significant differences between the cognitive age and the chronological age of seniors. With the intention of pointing up the differences between the two variables, i.e. cognitive age and chronological age, and to facilitate checking that the first is superior to the second, calculation of various statistics for discriminant analysis are necessary. Both Wilks’s l (Table V) and Box’s M (Table VI) show higher values for cognitive age as opposed to chronological age, which gives positive support for the first hypothesis. With respect to the canonical discriminant functions, more than 80 percent of the function visibly explains the differences between the groups, but this figure does not show the same forecasting capacity in relation to chronological age. A look at the results of the classification shows that the percentage of individuals correctly classified reaches an optimum level of 100 percent in the case of the use of cognitive age for analyses (Tables VII and VIII). Comparison of these results with the distributions of chronological ages of individuals by segments makes demonstrates clearly that percentage of individuals correctly classified decreases substantially, since the cognitive ages differ significantly from the chronological (Table IX). Next, the ways in which different perceptions of age in relation to motivations for tourism manifest themselves were subjected to analysis. These ways are measured by means of a set of statements deriving from the research by Beard and Ragheb (1980) and Kabanoff (1982) (see Table IV). To this end, the technique of multiple correspondence analysis, with application to each group separately for the 12 active formats reflecting reasons for involving in tourism, is the choice. The intention is to enrich the description of these groups and emphasize their motivations in an independent way, since the factors emerging from the application of this analysis are different for each of the segments. This selection also permits checking of a second hypothesis, that different cognitive ages make it plain that there are Table V Tests of equality for group averages. cognitive age/chronological age Cognitive age Birth year Wilks’s l F gl1 Gl2 Significance 0.34 0.66 780.82 206.01 1 1 398 398 0.00 0.00 Table VI Results from Box’s M test: cognitive age/chronological age Box’s M Approximately gl1 gl2 Significance Cognitive age Chronological age 2.21 0.92 1 458508.50 0.34 0.92 2.21 1 458508.46 0.14 Note: Confirms the null hypothesis that the population covariance matrices are the same Table VII Self-values: cognitive age/chronological age Cognitive age Chronological age Function Eigenvalue Percentage of variance Accumulated percentage Canonical correlation 1 1 1.96a 0.52a 100.00 100.00 100.00 100.00 0.81 0.58 Note: aThis analysis uses the first canonical discriminant functions j j VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH PAGE 155 Table VIII Results of classificationa,b: cognitive age Predicted grouping 1 2 Ward method Original Count Percentage Cross-validationa Count Percentage Total 1 2 1 2 223 0 100.00 0.0 0 177 0.0 100.00 223 177 100.00 100.00 1 2 1 2 223 0 100.00 0.00 0 177 0.00 100.00 223 177 100.00 100.00 Notes: aCorrectly classifies 100.00 percent of the original grouped cases; bcorrectly classifies 100.00 percent of the grouped cases validated by cross-validation Table IX Results of classificationa,b: chronological age Predicted grouping 1 2 Ward method Original Count Percentage Cross-validationa Count Percentage Total 1 2 1 2 187 56 83.90 31.60 36 121 16.10 68.40 223 177 100.00 100.00 1 2 1 2 187 56 83.90 31.60 36 121 16.10 68.40 223 177 100.00 100.00 Notes: aCorrectly classifies 77.00 percent of the original grouped cases; b correctly classifies 77.00 percent of the grouped cases validated by cross-validation varying motivations for tourism among seniors. The results show that for the active livers segment, four motivational factors emerge that explain more than 60 percent of the variance: getting acquainted with new people, breaking with routine, traveling for health and the pleasure of becoming acquainted with new places or things. In contrast, the four motivational factors that are relevant for the stable passives segment, explaining nearly 70 percent of the variance, are: cultural novelties, personal development, shared relaxation and isolated challenge (see Appendix 3). Analysis of the factorial maps obtained would permit observation of the relationship between cognitive age and motivations for tourism. Interpretation of the different ways of responding to motivations in the various factorial maps for the two segments permits the observation that the group termed ‘‘active livers’’ presents as the principal characteristic motivating their journeys the possibility of facing challenges, combined with a certain degree of standoffishness. In fact, they show no interest in traveling in an organized group, or in getting acquainted with other people or cultures at their destinations. Furthermore, they consider travel to be necessary for maintaining their health and balance. For them travel may well be just one more activity to which they are accustomed, which interests them, and which they enjoy. Furthermore, with respect to the details for Segment 2, the stable passives, their posture of indifference to the various motivations proposed requires emphasis, an attitude which links to the group’s rather inactive profile. They may well travel just for the sake of traveling, showing some apathy with respect to specific motivations. This behavior is probably due to the taking of simple decisions not requiring much involvement on their part, as very often they limit themselves to leisure packages programmed by associations specializing in catering to seniors. The final part provides a description of the longer-duration journeys made by the two segments, showing that active livers travel considerably more often than stable passives. j j PAGE 156 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 People in the first group prefer cities, which may be on the coast or inland, while those from the second visit either cities or towns and villages, but always on the coast. Even destinations chosen within Spain are different, since active livers prefer the Northern (Cantabrian) coast, but stable passives the Eastern (Mediterranean) shoreline. As for foreign destinations, the first segment is the one choosing these primarily. In relation to other behavioral habits, the groups also show a number of differences. Active livers select higher price range accommodation, and normally travel in their own vehicles, either as couples or with their family, their chief reasons for travel being to do something interesting for the family group or to visit family or friends. However, stable passives mostly take middle to low price range accommodation and generally travel by bus accompanied by their spouses or in an organized tour group. The packages on offer from associations which specialize in tours for seniors in Spain, like the Social Security linked organization called ‘‘Imserso’’, the local-government-backed set-up called ‘‘Club de los 60’’, and others of a similar nature are the chief options they take into account when making choices about travel plans. Conclusions of the research The intended objective of this research is to gain a deeper knowledge of the concept of cognitive age and its use as a variable when segmenting the market of people aged 55 years and above. This objective is of interest, because among the academic community there is an increasing recognition of the need to understand the heterogeneity of senior consumers. In fact, a number of academics, upon consideration, indicate that research which introduces this variable is better for it. Thus, Sherman et al. (2001) conclude that cognitive age is more effective as an indicator of satisfaction with life than chronological age. Barak and Rahtz (1999) find that those people who have a lower cognitive than chronological age have higher self-esteem and greater confidence in their abilities. Furthermore, Chua et al. (1990) state that those who are cognitively younger show greater satisfaction with life, are more active, and more socially involved. Likewise, when it comes to the capacity to predict behavior, cognitive age proves to be superior as compared to the traditional variable, chronological age (Smith and Moschis, 1984; Sherman et al., 2001). This requires greater awareness of its nature, with an eye to defining marketing strategies more directed towards such people’s needs and wishes. In relation to this reality, segmentation of the seniors market clearly needs to develop along the same lines. This is the motive for proposing the use of a new criterion for segmentation, cognitive age, since there are research findings that indicate that the perceptions people have of their age influence their behavior patterns. In the case of segmentation focusing specifically on seniors, there is even more justification, because as an individual’s age increases an ever greater dissociation between real age and the individual’s own perception of personal age comes into play. After its overview of the various methodologies relating to cognitive age, this investigation is one of the few applications of such a criterion to segmentation of senior citizen tourists undertaking journeys of longer duration. Furthermore, it is the sole piece of research so far to investigate its usefulness in an individualized manner, not associating it with other variables, the intention of this approach being to justify its inclusion as standard in questionnaires, as has traditionally been done with chronological age. Research usually incorporates cognitive age together with other variables, such as values or lifestyles. Consequently, it does not discover whether a specific piece of behavior is derived from the individual’s cognitive age or from the other variables under consideration. In the present piece of work, analysis of cognitive age is isolated, making possible the demonstration that different cognitive ages show differing behavior patterns in respect of tourism. To this end, the work puts forward proposals for measuring this variable, taking as a starting point the original model for the age decade scale of Barak et al. (1981) and showing some similarity to the suggestions made by Mathur et al. (1998) and by Clark et al. (1999). However, this work proposes the omission of the dimension called ‘‘appearance’’. Support for such an omission comes from an extensive range of literature which offers various references to indicate the great proximity of this dimension to chronological age. A further j j VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH PAGE 157 adjustment is the inclusion of a new dimension, termed ‘‘expectations’’, as many of the conceptions reflected in the other dimensions (actions, interests, feelings, health and thoughts) would seem to depend on the expectations subjects have in life, so that this gives a more dynamic character to the tool. After the demonstration of the value of the measurement, the outcome of its application is the establishment of two segments, called ‘‘active livers’’ and ‘‘stable passives’’. Subjects in the first of these two groups characteristically have a perception of their age lower than its real value, while those in the second have a cognitive age equal to or greater than their chronological age. The level of agreement between chronological and cognitive age is very low. Both motivations for tourism and the behavior as tourists of the two segments reflect these differences. Thus, active livers seek some degree of isolation and like to meet challenges when traveling. They see travel as permitting them to maintain an adequate balance in their state of health and hence they consider it to be a habitual activity, which they enjoy. With respect to their habits, they show a preference for cities, whether on the coast or inland, outside or within Spain. Within Spain the Northern coast is one of their favorite destinations, with travel being in their own private vehicles. They tend to stay in higher price range accommodation, traveling either as couples or as a family, with the intention of visiting family or friends, but on other occasions with the aim of doing something that will interest the family group. Stable passives characteristically express indifference to the various motivations suggested in the course of this research. They present a somewhat inactive profile, probably due to taking simple decisions that do not imply much involvement on their part, since very often they do no more than participate in packaged leisure travel. They visit both cities and smaller places, as long as they are costal, mostly destinations on the Mediterranean shores of Spain, and prefer to travel by bus. They usually stay in middle or low price range accommodation, accompanied by their spouses or in an organized travel group. The packages offered by associations that specialize in dealing with the elderly, such as Imserso, Club de los 60 and other similar groups are the principal options they consider when planning travel. Finally, the results obtained permit the statement that cognitive age enriches knowledge about seniors as tourists, because Wilks’s l and Box’s M statistics show higher values for this variable and because more than 80 percent of the canonical discriminant function explains the differences between groups. This finding demonstrates that the variable is more discriminating than chronological age and also influences reasons for tourism, highlighting differentiated behavior patterns. Hence, taking it into consideration when establishing marketing strategies directed towards this group would seem to be of interest. To take it into account from a professional angle when establishing marketing strategies, such as market segmentation, as also when deciding on business actions like defining the tourist service that is to be offered or which sort of advertising message will be most impactful, would also appear to be useful. The fact that there may be some defects in the approach taken here should receive some emphasis. One major limitation relates to the lack of a full methodological framework, as there is a shortage of repetitions of the methodology that could indicate whether it is consistent. Another point for highlighting is the chronological age of the group investigated, since, as this rises, the difficulties in gathering information increase as a consequence of lack of concentration and problems of understanding, which may sometimes lead to bias in the data recorded. Moreover, as this work is the first investigation in which the scale proposed has been used, a great deal still remains to be done in terms of demonstrating and improving the process of applying it, which underscores the need to carry out future lines of research so as to gain a deeper awareness of these features. For these reasons, work on the following lines of investigation would appear to be appropriate: j B to check on the usefulness of cognitive age in association with other types of variable in the tourism market; B to compare the results obtained from its application to other target groups, such as the young or the middle-aged; and B to apply the study, in just the format used here, more extensively, comparing results from different countries relating to the behaviors and preferences of seniors as tourists. j PAGE 158 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 References Barak, B. (1987), ‘‘Cognitive age: a multidimensional approach to measuring age identity’’, International Journal of Aging and Human Development, Vol. 25 No. 2, pp. 109-25. Barak, B. (1998), ‘‘Inner-ages of middle-aged prime-lifers’’, International Journal of Aging and Human Development, Vol. 46 No. 3, pp. 89-228. Barak, B. and Rahtz, D.R. (1999), ‘‘Perceived youth: appraisal and characterization’’, International Journal of Aging and Human Development, Vol. 49 No. 3, pp. 231-57. Barak, B. and Stern, B. (1986), ‘‘Subjective age correlates: a research note’’, The Gerontologist, Vol. 26, pp. 571-8. Barak, B., Gould, S. and Schiffman, L.G. (1981), ‘‘Cognitive age: a non-chronological age variable’’, in Monroe, K.B. (Ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 8, Association for Consumer Research, Ann Arbor, MI, pp. 602-6. Barak, B., Mathur, A., Lee, K. and Zhang, Y. (2001), ‘‘Perceptions of age-identity: a cross-cultural inner-age exploration’’, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 18 No. 10, pp. 1003-29. Bai, B.X., Smith, W., Cai, L.A. and O’Leary, J.T. (1999), ‘‘Senior-sensitive segments: looking at travel behavior’’, in Chon, K.S. (Ed.), The Practice of Graduate Research in Hospitality and Tourism, Haworth Press, Binghamton, NY, pp. 75-89. Beard, J.G. and Ragheb, M.G. (1980), ‘‘Measuring leisure motivation’’, Journal of Leisure Research, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 20-33. Beard, J.G. and Ragheb, M.G. (1983), ‘‘Measuring leisure motivation’’, Journal of Leisure Research, Vol. 15 No. 3, pp. 219-28. Bigné, J.E., Font Aulet, X. and Andreu Simó, L. (2000), Análisis de los destinos turı́sticos. Marketing de destinos turı́sticos-análisis y estrategias de desarrollo, ESIC Editorial, Madrid. Blau, Z.S. (1956), ‘‘Changes in status and age identification’’, American Sociological Review, Vol. 21, April, pp. 198-203. Blau, Z.S. (1961), ‘‘Structural constraints on friendship in old age’’, American Sociological Review, Vol. 26, pp. 429-39. Cai, L.A. (1997), The Silver Market Is Not All Gold: An Analysis of Senior Travelers’ Expenditure Patterns, Chrie, New York, NY, pp. 191-4. Carrigan, M., Szmigin, I. and Wright, J. (2004), ‘‘Shopping for a better world? An interpretative study of the potential for ethical consumption within the older market’’, Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 21 No. 6, pp. 401-17. Catterall, M. and Maclaran, P. (2001), ‘‘Body talk: questioning the assumptions in cognitive age’’, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 18 No. 10, pp. 1117-33. Chazaud, P. (1997), ‘‘Loisirs et marketing des seniors’’, Marketing Touristique et Tranche d’Âge, Les Cahiers Espaces, Vol. 54, pp. 23-4. Chua, C., Cote, J.A. and Leong, S.M. (1990), ‘‘The antecedents of cognitive age’’, in Gorn, G., Goldberg, M. and Pollay, R. (Eds), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 17, Association for Consumer Research, Ann Arbor, MI, pp. 880-5. Clark, S.D., Long, M.M. and Schiffman, L.G. (1999), ‘‘The mind-body connection: the relationship among physical activity level, life satisfaction and cognitive age among mature females’’, Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, Vol. 14 No. 2, pp. 221-40. Clary, D. (1991), ‘‘Les nouveaux comportements touristiques du troisième âge en Europe, L’Amérique comme modèle?’’, in Forneau, F. and Marchena, M. (Eds), Aménagement et Développement du Tourisme en France et en Espagne, Séminaires franco-espagnols, pp. 257-79. Cleaver, M. and Muller, T.E. (1998), ‘‘Testing the theoretical link between cognitive age and travel motives: a new way to segment the Australasian senior tourism market?’’, ANZMAC ’98 Conference Proceedings, Dunedin, November 30-December 2, pp. 1634-48. Cooper, P. and Marshall, G. (1984), ‘‘Exploring senior life satisfaction via market segmentation development and value exchange: an initial study’’, in Smith, S. and Venkatesan, M. (Eds), Advances in Health Care Research, Brigham Young University Press, Provo, UT. j j VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH PAGE 159 Dann, G.M.S. (2000), ‘‘Senior tourism’’, Annals of Tourism Research, Vol. 28 No. 1, pp. 235-8. Dann, G.M.S. (2001), ‘‘Targeting seniors through the language of tourism’’, Journal of Hospitality and Leisure Marketing, Vol. 8 Nos 3/4, pp. 5-35. Dunn, W. (1994), ‘‘The Eisenhower years’’, American Demographics, Vol. 16, July, pp. 34-40. Dunne, J. and Turley, D. (1997), ‘‘Banking schemes for seniors – bonus or unwelcome badge?’’, Irish Marketing Review, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 27-35. Dychtwald, K. and Flower, J. (1989), Age Wave: How the Most Important Trends of Our Time Will Change Your Future, Bantam Books, New York, NY. Dychtwald, K. and Gable, G. (1990), ‘‘Portrait of a changing consumer’’, Business Horizons, Vol. 33, January/February, pp. 62-73. Faranda, W.T. and Schmidt, S.L. (1999), ‘‘Segmentation and the senior traveler: implications for today’s and tomorrow’s aging consumer’’, Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 3-27. George, L.K., Mutran, E.J. and Pennybacker, M.R. (1980), ‘‘The meaning and measurement of age identity’’, Experimental Aging Research, Vol. 6 No. 3, pp. 283-98. Gibson, H. (1995), ‘‘Patterns of tourist role preference among three groups of seniors: ages 55-65, ages 66-75, and ages 76 and over’’, Teoros: Colloques et Congrès, No. 7. Goldsmith, R.E. and Flynn, L.R. (1992), ‘‘Identifying innovators in consumer product markets’’, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 26 No. 12, pp. 42-55. Goldsmith, R.E. and Heiens, R.A. (1992), ‘‘Subjective age: a test of five hypotheses’’, The Gerontologist, Vol. 32 No. 3, pp. 312-7. Goldsmith, R.E. and Hofacker, C.F. (1991), ‘‘Measuring consumer innovativeness’’, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 19, pp. 209-21. Goldsmith, R.E., Kilsheimer, J. and Freiden, J. (1993), ‘‘Social values and female fashions leadership: a cross-cultural study’’, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 10 No. 5, pp. 399-412. González Fernández, A.M. and Bello Acebrón, L. (2002), ‘‘The construct lifestyle in market segmentation. The behaviour of tourist consumers’’, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 36 Nos 1/2, pp. 51-85. Grande Esteban, I. (1993), Marketing estratégico para la tercera edad: principios para atender a un segmento emergente, ESIC Editorial, Madrid. Guiot, D. (1999), ‘‘Âge subjectif et segmentation des señora’’, Décisions Marketing, Vol. 18, September-December, pp. 15-23. Guiot, D. (2001a), ‘‘Antecedents of subjective age biases among senior women’’, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 18 No. 10, pp. 1049-71. Guiot, D. (2001b), ‘‘Tendance d’âge subjectif: quelle validité predictive’’, Recherche et Applications en Marketing, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 25-43. Guptill, C.S. (1969), ‘‘A measure of age identification’’, The Gerontologist, Vol. 9, pp. 96-102. Gwinner, K.P. and Stephens, N. (2001), ‘‘Testing the Implied mediational role of cognitive age’’, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 18 No. 10, pp. 1031-48. Hagan, L.A.R. and Uysal, M. (1993), ‘‘An examination of motivation and activities of pre-retirement (50-64) and post-retirement (65-98) age group for a touring trip’’, Tourism: Building Credibility for a Credible Industry. Proceedings of the XXII Annual Conference, Long Beach, CA, June, pp. 381-6. Havighurst, R.J. and Albrecht, R. (1953), Older People, Longmans Green, New York, NY. Hsu, C.H.C. and Lee, E.J. (2002), ‘‘Segmentation of senior motorcoach travelers’’, Journal of Travel Research, Vol. 40, May, pp. 364-73. Javalgi, R.G., Thomas, E.G. and Rao, S.R. (1999), ‘‘Consumer behavior in the US. Pleasure travel marketplace: an analysis of senior and nonsenior travelers’’, in Pizam, A. and Mansfeld, J. (Eds), Consumer Behavior in Travel and Tourism, Haworth Hospitality Press, New York, NY, pp. 281-304. Jyrkilä, F. (1960), ‘‘Society and adjustment to old age’’, Transactions of the Westermarck Society, 5th ed., Ejnar Munksgaard, Copenhagen. j j PAGE 160 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 Kabanoff, B. (1982), ‘‘Occupational and sex differences in leisure needs and leisure satisfaction’’, Journal of Occupational Behaviour, Vol. 3, pp. 233-45. Kastenbaum, R., Durbin, V., Sabatini, P. and Artt, S. (1972), ‘‘The ages of me: toward personal and interpersonal definitions of functional aging’’, Aging and Human Development, Vol. 3, pp. 197-211. Kutner, B., Fanshel, D., Togo, A.M. and Langner, T.S. (1956), Five Hundred over Sixty, Russell Sage Foundation, New York, NY. Lambin, J.J. (1995), Marketing estratégico, 3rd ed., McGraw-Hill, Madrid. Lanquar, R. (1995), ‘‘Trends and challenges in seniors’ tourism’’, Teoros: Colloques et Congrès, No. 7, pp. 12-13. Loverseed, H. (1993), ‘‘US mature travelers market’’, Travel and Tourism Analyst, Vol. 1, pp. 51-64. Loverseed, H. (2000), ‘‘The mature market in North America’’, Travel and Tourism Analyst, Vol. 3, pp. 47-65. Marı́n Vidal, A. (1993), ‘‘El impacto del turismo de tercera edad en España’’, Primeros Encuentros Internacionales sobre el Turismo de la Tercera Edad, Gran Canaria, May 4-7. Mathur, A., Sherman, E. and Schiffman, L.G. (1998), ‘‘Opportunities for marketing travel services to new-age elderly’’, Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 12 No. 4, pp. 265-77. Martin, B., McGuire, F. and Allen, L. (1998), ‘‘Retirees’ attitudes toward tourism: implications for sustainable development’’, Tourism Analysis, Vol. 3, pp. 43-51. Morgan, C.M. and Levy, D.J. (1996), ‘‘Understanding mature consumers’’, Marketing Review, January Nos 12/13, pp. 25-8. Moschis, G.P. (1992), ‘‘Gerontographics: a scientific approach to analyzing and targeting the mature market’’, Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 6 No. 3, pp. 17-25. Moschis, G.P. (1994), Marketing Strategies for the Mature Market, Quorum Books, Westport, CT. Moschis, G.P. (1996), ‘‘Life stages of the mature market’’, American Demographics, Vol. 18 No. 9, pp. 44-50. Moschis, G.P. (2003), ‘‘Marketing to older adults: an updated overview of present knowledge and practice’’, Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 20 No. 6, pp. 516-25. Moschis, G.P., Bellenger, D. and Curasi, C.F. (2004), ‘‘Housing preferences of older consumers’’, Proceedings of the 10th Annual Conference of the Pacific Tim Real Society, Bangkok, January, pp. 25-8. Moschis, G.P., Mathur, A. and Smith, R.B. (1993), ‘‘Older consumers’ orientation toward age-based marketing stimuli’’, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 21 No. 3, pp. 195-205. Mutran, E. and George, L.K. (1982), ‘‘Alternative methods of measuring role-identity: a research note’’, Social Forces, Vol. 60, pp. 866-76. Ortega Martı́nez, E. (1989), ‘‘Las vacaciones de los españoles de la tercera edad’’, Estudios Turı́sticos, Vol. 102, pp. 75-111. Osgood, C.E. and Suci, G.J. (1955), ‘‘Factor analysis of meaning’’, Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 50, pp. 325-38. Phillips, B.S. (1956), ‘‘A role theory approach to predicting adjustment of the aged in two communities’’, unpublished PhD dissertation, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY. Phillips, B.S. (1957), ‘‘Role theory approach to adjustment in old age’’, American Sociology, Vol. 22, pp. 212-7. Phillips, B.S. (1961), ‘‘Role change, subjective age and adjustment: a correlational analysis’’, Journal of Gerontology, Vol. 16, pp. 347-52. Reece, W.S. (2004), ‘‘Are senior leisure travelers different?’’, Journal of Travel Research, Vol. 43, August, pp. 11-18. Rosow, I. (1967), Social Integration of the Aged, The Free Press, New York, NY. Rosow, I. (1974), Socialization to Old Age, University of California Press, Berkeley, CA. Schiffman, L.G. and Sherman, E. (1991), ‘‘Value orientations of new-age elderly: the coming of an ageless market’’, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 22, pp. 187-94. j j VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH PAGE 161 Sherman, E., Schiffman, L.G. and Mathur, A. (2001), ‘‘The influence of gender on the new-age elderly’s consumption orientation’’, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 18 No. 10, pp. 1073-89. Shoemaker, S. (2000), ‘‘Segmenting the mature market’’, Journal of Travel Research, Vol. 39 No. 1, pp. 11-26. Silvers, C. (1997), ‘‘Smashing old stereotypes of 50-plus America’’, Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 14 No. 4, pp. 303-9. Stephens, N. (1991), ‘‘Cognitive age: a useful concept for advertising?’’, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 20 No. 4, pp. 37-51. Szmigin, I. and Carrigan, M. (2000), ‘‘The older consumer as innovator: does cognitive age hold the key?’’, Journal of Marketing Management, Vol. 16, pp. 505-27. Szmigin, I. and Carrigan, M. (2001a), ‘‘Time, consumption and the older consumer: an interpretive study of the cognitively young’’, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 18 No. 10, pp. 1091-116. Tuckman, J. and Lavell, M. (1957), ‘‘Self-classification as old or not old’’, Geriatrics, Vol. 12, pp. 666-71. Tuckman, J. and Lorge, I. (1954), ‘‘Classification of the self as young, middle-aged or old’’, Geriatrics, Vol. 9, pp. 534-6. Tuckman, J., Lorge, I. and Zeman, F. (1961), ‘‘The self-image in aging’’, Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 99, pp. 317-21. Van Harssel, J.H. (1994), ‘‘The senior market: distinct, diverse and demanding’’, in Archibald, J. and Jolin, L. (Eds), Educational Tourism and the Needs of Older Adults: Proceedings of the Third Global Classroom Conference, Teoros Collection, Montreal, pp. 363-77. Viant, A. (1993), ‘‘Enticing the elderly to travel’’, Tourist Management, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 52-60. Vilchez, L.F. (1994), ‘‘Nuevos segmentos de demanda. Estrategias de marketing para los consumidores de mayor edad’’, Distribución y Consumo, Vol. 4 No. 18, pp. 102-7. Villanueva Orbáiz, M. (1997), ‘‘La edad autopercebida: una nueva aproximación al estudio del comportamiento consumidor de las personas de más edad’’, Esic Market, Vol. 96, pp. 57-79. Wang, Y., Norman, W.C. and McGuire, F.A. (2005), ‘‘A comparative study of leisure constraints perceived by mature and young travelers’’, Tourism Review International, Vol. 8, pp. 263-79. Wilkes, R.E. (1992), ‘‘A structural modeling approach to the measurement and meaning of cognitive age’’, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 19 No. 9, pp. 292-301. Whitford, M. (1998), ‘‘Market in motion’’, Hotel and Motel Management, Vol. 20 No. 1, pp. 67-75. Zola, I.K. (1962), ‘‘Feelings about age among older people’’, Journal of Gerontology, Vol. 17, pp. 65-8. Further reading Barak, B. and Gould, S. (1985), ‘‘Alternative age measures: a research agenda’’, Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 12, pp. 53-8. Barak, B. and Rahtz, D.R. (1990), ‘‘Cognitive age: demographic and psychographic dimensions’’, Journal of Ambulatory Care Marketing, Vol. 3 No. 2, pp. 51-65. Barak, B., Mathur, A., Lee, K., Zhang, Y. and Erondu, E. (2003), ‘‘Inner-age satisfaction in Africa and Asia: a cross-cultural exploration’’, Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics, Vol. 15 Nos 1/2, pp. 3-26. Baum, S.K. and Boxley, R.L. (1983), ‘‘Age identification in the elderly’’, The Gerontologist, Vol. 23 No. 5, pp. 532-7. Eastman, J.K. and Iyer, R. (2005), ‘‘The impact of cognitive age on internet use of the elderly: an introduction to the public policy implications’’, International Journal of Consumer Studies, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 125-36. Goulding, C. and Shankar, A. (2004), ‘‘Age is just a number. Rave culture and the cognitively young thirty-something’’, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 38 Nos 5/6, pp. 641-58. Johnson, E.B. (1996), ‘‘Cognitive age: understanding consumer alienation in the mature marketing’’, Review of Business, Vol. 17 No. 3, pp. 35-44. j j PAGE 162 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 Mathur, A. and Moschis, G.P. (2005), ‘‘Antecedents of cognitive age: a replication and extension’’, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 22 No. 12, pp. 969-94. Montepare, J.M. and Lachman, M.E. (1989), ‘‘You’re only as old as you feel. Self-perceptions of age, fears of aging and life satisfaction from adolescence to old age’’, Psychology and Aging, Vol. 4, pp. 73-89. Nelson, S.L. and Smith, R.B. (1988), ‘‘The influence of model age on older consumers’ reactions to print advertising’’, in Leigh, J.H. and Martin, C.R. Jr (Eds), Current Issues and Research in Advertising, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, pp. 189-212. Peters, G.R. (1971), ‘‘Self-conceptions of the aged, age identification, and aging’’, The Gerontologist, Winter, Part II, pp. 69-73. Ramos Soler, I. (2005), El estilo de vida de las personas mayores y la comunicación publicitaria, doctoral dissertation, University of Alicante, Alicante. Van Auken, S. and Barry, T.E. (1995), ‘‘An assessment of the trait validity of cognitive age measures’’, Journal of Consumer Psychology, Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 107-32. Wei, S.C. (2005), ‘‘Consumers’ demographic characteristics, cognitive ages, and innovativeness’’, Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 32, pp. 633-40. Appendix 1: Myths or stereotypes about the elderly B They lack purchasing power. B Their mental capacities are impaired or they are ailing. B They constitute a homogeneous group. B They see themselves as old. B They are reluctant to try new products or services. B They are not physically active. B They are uneducated. B They are unproductive. B They do not represent a substantial market. Appendix 2: Model proposed for measuring cognitive age I feel as though I am in my: I look as though I am in my: My health is as though I am in my: I do most things as though I am in my: I think as though I am in my: My interests are mostly those of a person in his/her: Response options: 30s, 40s, 50s, 60s, 70s, 80s, 90s (age groups). Appendix 3: Items included among motivations for tourism B When I travel, I enjoy group activities. B For me, traveling is a way to achieve personal enhancement. B I travel to get away from daily routine. B Traveling so as to get acquainted with other cultures is one of my pleasures. B I travel with the aim of learning something new. B I am attracted by the challenges that I may face when traveling. B When I am on vacation, I want to get away from it all. B When I travel, I try to relax from day-to-day stress. B What I like best about traveling is getting acquainted with new people and making new friends. j j VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH PAGE 163 B Traveling should be a way of strengthening family ties. B My journeys are important in keeping me fit. B I like traveling so as to practice foreign languages. Corresponding author Ana M. González is the corresponding author and can be contacted at: amgonf@unileon.es To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: reprints@emeraldinsight.com Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints j j PAGE 164 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF CULTURE, TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 2 2009 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.