Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale RUNNING HEAD: Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale Generalization and Limitation of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Jesse M. Dostal Cleveland States University 2004 1 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 2 ABSTRACT This study sought to examine the Schwartz Social Values Scale (SVS) through Classic Multidimensional Scaling (CMDS) and Weighted Multidimensional Scaling (WMDS) models which were to be analyzed via Multiple Discriminant Functions (MDF) and binary logistic regression. A normal adult sample from the United States, Poland, and Romania, composed of 98, 201, and 198 respondents respectively, was obtained. Due to issues of potential non-representative sampling in the three countries of interest, multiple linear regression was used to control for the effect of demographics on the individual SVS item scores. ALSCAL could not produce a satisfactory WMDS model, but did produce CMDS models for each country individually that were satisfactory. These models tended to offer partial support of the Schwartz theory. The implications of the results are that the SVS’s structure of item interrelationships differ by culture, but the grouping of values into 10 domains and two semi-bidirectional dimensions was supported. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 3 EXECUTIVE SUMMARY That peoples of separate cultures differ may be seen as almost self evident in nature. However, the quantification of these differences requires that the researcher find an objective measure theoretically tied to culture. Since values are an integral part of culture, this study focuses upon the quantification of cultural values. In so doing it provides reviews of three survey based methods of quantifying values; those of Rokeach, Hofstede, and Schwartz. Schwartz’s (1992) questionnaire and model, in the form of the Schwartz Values Scale (SVS), were selected as the instrument of choice due to it high level of validation and refined inclusion of previous researchers’ work. Schwartz (1992) specifies that the 56 value statements of the SVS fall into ten primary motivation categories. Further, he states that these ten primary motivators are arranged into two bidirectional dimensions; Self-Transcendence versus SelfEnhancement, and Conservation versus Openness to Change. However, the SVS is typically analyzed via oblique factor analysis or more commonly Smallest Space Analysis (an early form of Multidimensional Scaling (MDS) commonly referred to as, SSA). Both of these analysis techniques rely on principal components; as such they have several limitations. That is, these techniques can be very resource intense due to the large number of respondents required to achieve reliability. One of this study’s objectives is to examine the use of alternate MDS models that may require fewer respondents and provide an alternative examination of the SVS structure of inter-item association. As such, the hypotheses were tested that MDS solutions would be congruent between cultures and would verify the prior description of the Schwartz model. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 4 Respondents for this study were composed of a normal adult sample of 98 Americans, 201 Poles, and 198 Romanians. This study’s sample was obtained through convenience sampling methods. Questionnaires were put through a rigorous double back translation process. In this case, double back translation means that each country’s questionnaire was first composed in English and translated to its respective language. These translations were then back translated into English and any item showing heavy divergence from the original composition was translated again via another native speaker of the language. These new translations repeated the process of back translation to English through another back translator. This process was repeated until the questionnaires provided clear, concise, versions of the original English copy. The analysis of this study’s data was composed of four phases. Phase One involved the alignment of demographic questions for later statistical control. That is, the respondents having been obtained through convenience methods were potentially nonrepresentative of each country’s population as a whole. As such, Phase Two of this study involved the production of linear regression models for which the independent variables were the demographics and the dependent variable for each item of the SVS. Phase Three of the analysis involved the production of correlation matrices composed of SVS items for which the data were standardized residuals from the previously mentioned multiple regression models. These correlation matrices were then used to generate a Weighted Multidimensional Scaling (WMDS) model using the data from all three countries and Classic Multidimensional Scaling (CMDS) models for each country individually. The final phase of this analysis, Phase Four, involved the use of Multiple Discriminant Functions (MDF) to test for the presence of Schwartz’s (1992) ten primary Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 5 motivation types and binary logistic regression to test for the presence of the semibidirectional dimensions. Finally, the distances generated for each country via CMDS and the original indices of association for each individual country were tested for congruence. The Second Phase of this analysis, the demographic control of SVS items via linear regression, indicated that the demographics rarely achieved significance. Results of the Third Phase of this analysis indicate that the WMDS model was a poor fit to the data. However, the CMDS models generated for each country separately offered satisfactory solutions. The use of MDF to classify the value statement of the SVS via Schwartz’s (1992) model received mixed support. Indices of association between the correlation matrices and CMDS distances by country were low (under .25 in all cases). The implication of ALSCAL failing to produce a satisfactory WMDS model for the SVS values of all three countries and the illustration of poor congruence between the indices of association between the countries’ CMDS distances and association coefficients suggests that each country of interest in this study maintains unique value structures. This suggests that the primary motivation and semi-bidirectional dimension systems is better conceptualized as a taxonomy or classification of values. When the various CMDS models were tested via MDF and binary logistic regression, the Schwartz (1992) model of primary motivations appears relatively intact in the three countries. Notably, this taxonomy was best supported within the Unites States CMDS space, with Poland coming in second, and Romania third. Implications of these results are discussed. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 6 INTRODUCTION In an age of increasing globalization many of the social sciences have been faced with the impact of differing cultural backgrounds upon the particular issues being investigated. Fortunately, methodologies and theoretical paradigms are available to quantify and explain these differences. This study seeks to address the measurement of culture through a commonly used method, questionnaires, and through a commonly measured component of cultures, values. The purpose of this study is to measure the interrelationships of values in a cross-cultural context. Specifically, the present paper will test the use of one of these measures, the Schwartz Social Values Scale (SVS), under a different analysis model than was originally employed. Justification for this study’s choice of methodology and system of measurement for culture will be considered on in the course of this discussion. First to be discussed in this study will be the method of data collection or instrumentation and the underlying theory of culture and its relationship to values. Specifically, the first discussion will address the mode of data collection, questionnaires, and why they are used. The second discussion will give an overview of available theoretic models describing the data collected. Data Collection and Theory If one is to measure culture then an operational definition is needed. A common definition would be “the values, ethics, rituals, traditions, material objects, and services produced or valued by the members of a society (Solomon, 1999).” This definition suggests that one could measure a variety of subjects. For example, one could count the number of, material objects, such as cars possessed by members of a culture, amount of a Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 7 given behavior, as in the amount of time driving cars, or rate the importance of a given object or behavior, as in rating the importance of automobiles or the importance of driving, or one does when driving a car could be recorded. The methodologies that could be used to collect these data, raw counting, ethnography, or content analysis, are unimportant in this discussion as there is fundamental flaw associated with information collected: lack of generalizability. The concept of generalizability could be defined as whether the outside research is applicable to a new user’s situation (Guion, 1998). In and of themselves these behavioral data are situation specific. How does one know that behavior as measured in one situation takes on the same meaning in another with regard to overall cultural influence? These behaviors may be bound to the situation. Furthermore, problems are incurred researching these behaviors. If respondent recall is used as a measure reliability tends to be a problem (Hurt, Joseph, & Cook, 1977; Goldsmith & Hofacker, 1991). Further, from an intercultural standpoint values take on meaning only if they maintain the same definition by way of content. That is, a value is only meaningful if it maintains the same definition across several cultures (Bardi & Schwartz, 2001; Sverko, 1995). However, how can the researcher be assured that the definitions of each value within the context of a behavioral measure will maintain the same meaning across cultures? This problem of generalizability of measures is very similar to the one faced by innovativeness and opinion leadership research at a time when a similar debate raged in consumer psychology. The solution in this case is the same: a higher level of abstraction is needed. This abstraction might be obtained by using questionnaires in which respondents rate the overall importance of a given value. Remember that values are integral to the Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 8 definition of culture and therefore serve as a proxy for it. From this viewpoint, values are presumed to encapsulate the aspirations of individuals and societies: they pertain to what is desirable, to deeply engrained standards that determine future directions and justify past actions (Braithwaite & Scott, 1991; Bardi & Schwartz, 2001). The end result is that quantified values can be seen as a distillation of culture. Further, differences in the rated importance of values between two cultures allow researchers to identify the unique influences of social structure and culture. What Are Values? There are two competing views with regard to values. The first is that a value is an attribute of the person “doing the valuing.” The second is that a value is an attribute of an “object receiving the valuing.” To clarify, the first statement assumes that a value is based in the world of the individual or consensus of individuals and exists as a sort of conceptual definition. The second statement assumes that a value is an inherent feature of an object or class of objects. This study, like most other studies of social values, takes the former as opposed to the latter assumption (Braithwaite & Scott, 1991). From this viewpoint, values are defined as desirable transituational goals that vary in importance by individual (Braithwaite & Scott, 1991; Schwartz & Bardi, 2001). Further, values serve as guiding principles in people’s lives (Schwartz & Bardi, 2001). Under this methodology the assumption is made that by aggregating the values espoused by individuals, one may understand values within a given society. Note that this is a common assumption made by most cross-cultural researchers when studying values with relation to cultures (Braithwaite & Scott, 1991). Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 9 The Quantification of Values Now that values have been defined, a discussion of how they are quantified is presented. This discussion will present the theoretical justification for the choice of instruments to measure values and the selection of cultures to be studied. In the past three measures of values, those of Rokeach, Hofstede, and Schwartz, have been used in the study of values within Poland and the United States. Each of these measures compare the importance of values for these cultures. Note that at the date of this study no known research has been performed on the quantification of values for the third country of interest, Romania. The Rokeach Approach. Rokeach is seen as dominating values research due to his contributions to clarifying and integrating the initial concepts of this area (Braithwaite & Scott, 1991; Kelly, Silverman, & Cochrane, 1972). The Rokeach Value Survey relies on the rank ordering of 18 terminal values and 18 instrumental values (Solomon 2002; Braithwaite & Scott, 1991). A terminal value relates to a desirable end state of existence and its worthiness of attainment. An instrumental value relates to an enduring belief regarding a certain mode of conduct or manner of behaving. Both are viewed as necessary in the assessment of a single value. The theoretic rationale supporting the use of two types of statements revolves around the idea that while desirable end states are important so are the means of getting to these end states (Waters 1999; Schwartz, 1992). The values of Rokeach’s Values Survey are found in column one of Table 1. All values presented therein are rated on a mono-dimensional four-point scale. An incomplete sentence stem was, “It is now or will be important for me to (value Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 10 iteration).” Note that value iterations were used for instrumental values in a different form; examples are presented in the second column of Table 1. Rokeach’s Values Survey is composed of fifty-four items (Ferreir-Marques & Miranda, 1995). Finally, a four-point scale is used to measure the importance of each value iteration. These four points are anchored as follows: 1. Of little or no importance 2. Of some importance 3. Important 4. Very Important There are two criticisms of Rokeach’s Values Survey. The first concern is that this particular instrument is not considered to explain all of the values present in a culture. Second, Rokeach’s Values Survey is considered to be biased towards Western values (Spini, 2003; Braithwaite & Scott, 1991). That is, this instrument views values from a Western viewpoint alone, with only limited influences from Eastern viewpoints. The Hofstede Approach. Another values researcher that has looked at both Poland and the United States is Hofstede. The Hofstede Values Survey relies on five independent value dimensions (International Business Center, 2003; Middleton & Jones, 2000; Randall, Hwo, & Pawelk, 1993). Unlike the values measures of Schwartz and Rokeach, Hofstede’s values survey relies on bi-directional dimensions 1. These five value dimensions are Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, Individualism versus Collectivism, Masculinity versus 1 To clarify the concept of bi-directionality, Hofstede’s measure assumes that, Individualism is the opposite of Collectivism. Further, this measure presupposes that, as one values Individualism less, one must value Collectivism more. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 11 Femininity, and Long versus Short Term Time Orientation. For the purposes of later discussion each of these dimensions is defined in the following paragraphs Power Distance is the extent to which a society accepts unequal distribution of power in institutions and organizations. Although inequality may exist within any culture the degree to which it is accepted varies considerably between cultures. Power Distance measures how subordinates respond to power and authority. In high-Power Distance countries, subordinates tend to be afraid of their bosses, and bosses tend to be paternalistic and autocratic. In low-power distance countries, subordinates are more likely to challenge bosses and bosses tend to use a consultative style. The index of Uncertainty Avoidance focuses on the level of tolerance for uncertainty and ambiguity within a society for unstructured situations. When Uncertainty Avoidance is strong, a culture tends to perceive unknown situations as threatening and people tend to avoid them. This creates a rule-oriented society that institutes laws, rules, regulations, and controls in order to reduce the amount of ambiguity. A low Uncertainty Avoidance rating indicates the country has less concern about ambiguity and uncertainty and has more tolerance for a variety of opinions. Societies that are less rule-oriented, more readily accept change, take more, and greater risks are archetypal of a low rating on the Uncertainty Avoidance index. The next index in the Hofstede Values Survey is Individualism versus Collectivism. In Individualistic cultures, people are expected to look out for themselves. Everyone contributes to a common goal, but with little mutual pressure. The typical attributes endorsed in cultures associated with a high score on this index are personal time, freedom, and challenge. In Collectivistic countries, people are bound together Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 12 through strong personal and protective ties based on loyalty to the group during one’s lifetime and often beyond. Further, in Collectivistic cultures the typical attributes of importance are training, physical condition, and the use of skills. Masculinity versus Femininity is the fourth index in Hofstede’s model. Where the masculine index is high people tend to value having a high opportunity for earnings, getting the recognition they deserve when doing a good job, having an opportunity for advancement to higher-level jobs, and having challenging work to do in order to derive a sense of accomplishment. In cultures where feminine values are more important, people tend to value good working relationships with their supervisors. Feminine values under this framework involve working with people, cooperating well with one another, living in an area desirable to themselves and to their families, and having the security of working for their company as long as they want. The final index in Hofstede’s model is Long-Term versus Short-Term Time Orientation. A Long-Term Time Orientation is characterized by persistence and perseverance, a respect for hierarchical status based relationships (how status is assigned is not addressed in the available literature), thrift, and a sense of shame. A short-term time orientation is marked by a sense of security and stability, protection of one’s reputation, a respect for tradition, and a reciprocation of greetings; favors and gifts. As a side note, this final index was added later on to the Hofstede framework. Like Rokeach’s Value’s Survey, limitations to Hofstede’s system also exist. Hofstede’s values system was produced only within modernized Western nations. As such, it remains unclear how Eastern cultures would fare under this system. The next criticism revolves around whether each value maintains the same definition between Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 13 individual cultures and if the overall structure of the values holds up between cultures (Schwartz, 1992). To elucidate, it is unknown whether each culture defines each value the same way, or if these values maintain the same relationship between the individual dimensions. A further criticism of Hofstede’s measure is that when constructing his values survey Hofstede relied on a single multinational corporation. The validation of a values measure on one set of people cross-nationally limits the generalizability through selection bias. That is, no evidence has been presented explaining whether these values as developed, measured, and originally normed by Hofstede reflect those of only that multi-national corporation, or the cultures in which they were measured as a whole. Additional criticisms revolve around the measure’s age, the data used to generate Hofstede’s schema are from 1967 through 1973. Thus, there is the danger that these values reflect only the thinking of that period alone (Schwartz, 1994). This system of values also makes a rather large assumption that bi-directional scales are appropriate with regard to individualism versus collectivism and masculinity versus femininity. No validation of the bi-directionality assumption of these scales can be found within the literature at hand. As such, this instrument is only as good as this assumption (Schwartz, 1992). A final factor that is not addressed is the independence of each dimension with regard to the other dimensions. Are these dimensions interrelated? If these dimensions are interrelated then to what degree are they interrelated? Further, what would be the impact of these intercorrelations (Schwartz, 1992)? Schwartz. For Schwartz and many cross-cultural researchers, cultural values are measured on two levels: individual and cultural. The individual level variables in values research Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 14 measure the psychological dynamics from the framework of the lowest unit of culture, one person. Cultural level variables measure the solutions societies produce for regulating human activities (Schwartz, 1994). Note that this study will be measuring values at the individual level and not at the cultural level. While the interrelationships of the SVS values for Poland and the United States at the individual level are present in Schwartz (1992) in the form of an SSA perceptual space map, the need to justify the countries used in this study (addressed later) via aggregate value profiles are only available at the cultural level. As such, this distinction is presented here not only to clarify what level of measurement is being used, but also to inform the reader for a later portion of this introduction. The results of the cultural and individual levels of measurement on the SVS share a multitude of similarities. The same questionnaires are used to measure values at both levels. Further, the same analysis is used to observe the grouping of values for each level, smallest space multiple dimensional scaling based on Pearson correlation coefficients, also known as Smallest Space Analysis. This allows a graphical presentation of the intercorrelations of each scale as well as raw coefficients. Further, it solves the problem of assumed bi-directionality found in The Hofstede Values Scale by allowing all of the values to intercorrelate freely. Unlike the measures of Rokeach and Hofstede, the SVS was validated with data from two groups, schoolteachers and college undergraduates. Further, the SVS’s validation took place in a multitude of cultures. While convenience in obtaining a sample, on the part of the primary researchers, was an issue in the selection of the two validation groups other factors also played a role in this decision. First, teachers are one Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 15 of the primary groups to communicate values and students are typically seen as the target of this communication. Second, both populations are easily accessible to researchers. As such, secondary researchers would be able to easily be able to find respondents when comparing results (Schwartz, 1994; 1992). The SVS is composed of 56 single values that represent 10 overarching primary motivation types. Their descriptions are present in Table 2 of this study. Each item is measured on a scale composed of nine points varying from negative one to seven. Seven represents “supreme importance”, zero represents “not important”, finally, negative one represents “opposed to my values.” Note that two through six remain unlabelled through this instrument. The SVS is provided in its English form is provided in Appendix A. At a higher level of abstraction, the primary motivation types may be distilled into two basic value dimensions (Schwartz, 1992). These dimensions are Self-Transcendence versus Self-Enhancement and Openness to Change versus Conservation. The relationship of these value dimensions to the individual level motivation types is illustrated along with the position of each primary motivation type showing their interassociations in Figure 1. Self-Transcendence is akin to Hofstede’s Collectivism, while Self-Enhancement is similar to Collectivism. Openness to Change is just that, it relates how accepting an individual is to new ideas and viewpoints as well as how likely he or she is to change their viewpoint. Conservation is the degree of how steadfast a person is with regard to her beliefs. Note that these semi-bipolar value dimensions are not fully orthogonal in terms of their inter-associations (hence the term, “semi-bipolar”). Primary motivation types and the values that define them, found at the edges of these value dimensions may share Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 16 commonalities in their interpretation. That is, primary motivation types are not purely liberal or conservative with regard to Openness to change. For example, from Figure 1, the primary motivation type “Universalism” is not fully open with regard to Openness to change; rather it would appear to have a strong relationship to Conservation as well, especially when one moves progressively more clockwise through the motivation types 2. When evaluating cultures at the aggregate level another set of values is used. These values are Mastery, Hierarchy, Conservation, Harmony, Egalitarian Commitment, Intellectual Autonomy, and Affective Autonomy. The meanings of each of these values are taken from the 1994 study by Schwartz. Mastery emphasizes active mastery of the social environment through selfassertion. Mastery promotes active efforts to modify one’s surroundings, and get ahead of people. Hierarchy is the recognition of a legitimate social ladder and related resource allocation. Concepts related to Hierarchy would be humbleness and accepting ones place within society. Conservation revolves around maintenance of the status quo, propriety, and avoiding any actions that might disturb the traditional order. Under the rubric of Conservation falls the idea that the needs of the individual are inseparable from the needs of the group. Harmony emphasizes harmony with nature. Nature, in this case does not necessarily imply the natural world of plants and animals. Rather, Nature means a world at peace and social justice. This value stands as the antithesis of value types that promote 2 This is not covered by Schwartz directly in his 1992 or 1994 studies; rather it is an inference drawn from his works for the purposes of discussion. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 17 actively changing the world through exploitation of people or resources and through selfassertation. Harmony is further refined through its neutral stance with regard to individualism as opposed to collectivism. Egalitarian Commitment is typified by benevolence on a voluntary level for other people. Note that this denotes a commitment that can occur among equals and not the commitment that is represented by the value type Hierarchy. Intellectual Autonomy is typified by freedom of intellectual choice and selfdirection. This value type, like Affective Autonomy, which is described later, represents the degree to which a society views the individual to be entitled to pursue his or her own individual cerebral interests or desires. Affective Autonomy relates directly to hedonic pleasure and stimulation. As previously mentioned it also represents the degree of importance a society puts on the individual pursuing his or her own physical and emotional desires. The use of a single questionnaire to explain values at two levels of measurement could be seen as to beg the question of equivalence between the cultural and individual level values. Schwartz (1994) does not go so far as to explain how the two structures relate to each other at the empirical level. However, in both cases the same questionnaire is used to generate an analysis. Further, Schwartz does allude to the use of correlation between the two sets of primary motivations’ distances as empirical proof that the two relate to one another. Unfortunately, he does not elucidate on this point, as such the available literature is somewhat wanting in defining what the exact relationship between the two levels of measurement is. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 18 There are really only two major criticisms of the SVS. The first involves the use of only two groups in its development. It remains unclear if using only undergraduate students and teachers provides a representative sample of each society. However, in a world of limited resources the coordination of two different populations across 21 different nations would probably be enough in and of itself to exhaust innumerable research teams that have studied this scale. Further, the SVS relies on two relatively complex forms of statistical analysis, SSA and Oblique Factor analysis. Both SSA and Factor analysis rely on the same core analytic technique; principle components analysis (Astill, 1998). Principle components analysis relies on a tremendous number of respondents. Ratios of 5 or 10 respondents to each question suggest that sample ranges must not fall lower than 280 subjects for the SVS and really should exceed 570 for the purposes of generalizability of the factor structure to the population as a whole (Hair et al., 1998). While the acquisition of 570 respondents does not seem unreasonable, when one considers that comparable numbers of respondents must be acquired in two or more countries it becomes increasingly difficult to put together a meaningful study. Justification of Instrumentation The prior arguments against the SVS not withstanding, several reasons to use this instrument exist. Specifically, these arguments are that the SVS has shown itself to build on while overcome the failings of the systems developed by Rokeach and Hofstede and that this measure has been shown more stable in terms of its values maintaining the same definition across cultures. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 19 Further, the ten value types of the SVS have been shown to be nearly identical in terms of meaning both across and within cultures. Note that this classification system pertains to both Eastern and Western societies (Bardi & Schwartz, 2001; Schwartz, 1992; Schwartz, 1994). This means that the internal definitions may in fact reflect the universal values associated with the human condition and that their levels of importance vary by the culture being measured. Evidence of this type has not been reported for Rokeach or Hofstede’s systems. Schwartz (1992) addresses the issue of value definitions remaining constant among cultures through two methods: double back translation and constant interrelations of each value regardless of the culture studied. First, Schwartz employed a rigorous process of back translation in the process of implementing his study in each culture. While this does not show that the definitions are necessarily the same it does show that each culture was guaranteed to be exposed to the comparable instruments. Second, the relationships of each value as measured through SSA remained relatively constant. That is, each value with regard to its interrelationship with each other value remained in largely the same position. This rigorous study of interrelationships has given the SVS the distinction of being called the most heavily studied values scale to date (Todd & Lawson, 2003; Bilsky, 2002). When both of the factor of rigorous back translation and the factor of a constant structure of values are taken into consideration, this tends to indicate that each culture when exposed to the same instrument produced a highly similar profile. The Values Scale as developed by Rokeach, Hofstede’s Values Scale, and the SVS are to some degree interrelated. Two pieces of evidence will be given that draw these instruments together. The first type of evidence is conceptual and relates to Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 20 Rokeach’s Values Scale. The second piece of evidence is empirical and relates to Hofstede’s scale. At a very basic level these scales are interrelated in that they are meant to measure the same construct: values. However, at a more refined level these three value scales may be seen as interrelated through their development. As previously stated, Rokeach is seen as the father of modern values research (Braithwaite & Scott, 1991; Kelly et al., 1972). The SVS is based on Rokeach’s work and is viewed as an extension and refinement thereof (Braithwaite & Scott, 1991). Additionally, the SVS does not exclude the prior research of Rokeach and Hofstede; instead it tends to build on it. The use of the levels of abstraction with Openness to Change versus Conservation and SelfTranscendence versus Self-Enhancement allows compatibility with Rokeach. Further, in the validation process that Schwartz employed (1994) he compared his measure to Hofstede’s, thus allowing both theoretical and empirical comparisons. Empirical evidence tends to bind the Hofstede Values Scale and SVS too. Schwartz (1994) provides correlation coefficients for his scale to that of Hofstede. Conservatism from Hofstede’s scale is correlated with Schwartz’s Autonomy at .70. Transcendence from Hofstede’s scale is correlated with Schwartz’s Hierarchy and Mastery at .90. Hofstede’s individuality dimension is positively correlated with Autonomy (both Affective and Intellectual) and negatively correlated with Conservation. Unfortunately, Schwartz does not provide the exact level of correlation for the latter two dimensions and does not give correlations for the remaining indices and dimensions. However, all of the correlations reported by Schwartz are significant (p < .01). Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 21 Schwartz (1994) goes further to state the relationship between Hofstede’s Values Survey and the SVS at the individual level. He indicated that the individual level values for Femininity are akin to Benevolence and Universalism, Masculinity is akin to Power, Hierarchy, and Achievement with a broad concern for self-advancement, Autonomy versus Conformity (parallel to Individual Openness to change and Conservation) from the Schwartz scale is equivalent to Individualism vs. Collectivism from the Hofstede scale. Conceptual and empirical evidence has been offered that the values measures of Rokeach and Hofstede are related to the SVS. Two conclusions may be drawn from this; one involves the choice of instrument and the other expectations regarding what that instrument will find. First, no answers can be found regarding the criticism of the value scales developed by Rokeach and Hofstede. The SVS however, has been shown to not only overcome the bulk of these criticisms but to extend prior researchers’ work. Second, as the SVS extends both the work of Rokeach and Hofstede it can be expected that measurements made through these two instruments will most likely lead to differences on the SVS. Further discussion of the shortcomings of the SVS, specifically regarding its analysis, will be withheld until later in this study as they are to be addressed in the hypothesis section. The remainder of this introduction, leading up to the hypotheses, will revolve around the selection of cultures to be the subject of this study. The Difference between American, Polish, and Romanian Culture “It is widely believed that there are marked national differences in a number of psychological characteristics. This holds in particular for values, because the existence of national value patterns is considered almost self-evident (Sverko, 1995).” However, such an assumption without empirical proof could not only be considered arrogant but also Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 22 dangerous in the area of values research. That countries differ in many ways is simply a matter of everyday observation. To be more precise, these differences seem to explain the existence of the entities known as countries. This is particularly true of the ‘mentality’ of respective peoples, a complex set of beliefs, habits, attitudes, values, norms and specific behavioral patterns that are attributed to them (Trentini & Muzuio, 1995). Specifically, this section will address why it is expected that the SVS will detect differences in Polish and U.S. culture. Note that Romania was excluded from the prior paragraph involving cultures to be used in this study. At the time of this study no known research has been performed on Romanian culture with regard to values. As such, no direct evidence will be presented regarding differences between Romania and the United States or Poland. The justification for the use of Romania in this study is to provide contrast to measure of values in U.S. and Poland, and to further the use of social values research. Sverko (1995) measured the values of Australia, Belgium, English speaking Canadians, French speaking Canadians, Croatia, Italy, Japan, Portugal, Poland, English speaking South Africans, Afrikaans speaking South Africans, native language speaking South Africans, and the United States using the Rokeach Values Survey. The results were initially analyzed via hierarchical cluster analysis for which the cultures of interest (nationality in this case was synonymous with culture; however, different languages within the same country were considered as possibly representing different cultures) were clustered by their value scores3. Within this analysis, Poland and the United States 3 Note that Hierarchical Cluster Analysis is an iterative (that is, step by step) process by which objects that are similar, as measured by attribute or action, in this case values, are grouped together. The sooner an object, or set of objects, falls within the same group the more likely they are to be similar in terms of the attributes that measured them. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 23 clustered at the last of 25 steps. The stability of this cluster solution was also tested. It was found that the agglomerative schedule was considered stable as the use of alternate hierarchical cluster methods revealed largely the same results. 4 When an F-test was used on the resulting value scores by cluster, they ranged from 35 to 334 (all at, p < 0.0001 level) indicating broad differences in the samples on each value. This study also examined the alternative hypothesis that clustering was occurring based on language alone as opposed to differences based on larger cultural issues. This alternative hypothesis was not supported as English speaking and French Canadians clustered within three steps while the various language speakers of South Africa clustered within eight steps (one step for English and Afrikaans speaking South Africans, eight for Native speaking and the English-Afrikaans speakers). Further, double back translations were used to insure that the survey was properly translated, thus eliminating or at least reducing bias associated with the language in which the survey was delivered. This study offers evidence that differences will be found between Poland and U.S. on the SVS. As previously discussed Schwartz based the SVS on Rokeach’s research. If the relationship between Rokeach’s Values Survey and the SVS hold then one reasonably expect the SVS to reproduce these results. Scores from Hofstede’s Values Survey for Poland and the United States are summarized in Table 3 of this study as obtained from a study by Braithwaite and Scott (1991). Note that the values obtained for Poland are based on estimates derived from a student sample while the sample from the United States was derived from the same international corporation as the majority of the validation procedure. Poland was normed 4 Sverko (1995) did not present the alternate solutions in this case. As such, they are not discussed in this literature review. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 24 against the Canadian sample through equivalent sampling techniques. The largest differences are found in the realm of Individualism versus Collectivism and Uncertainty Avoidance. Not enough information is present from the existing sources to perform significance testing (Middleton & Jones, 2000). However, evidence has been given showing strong correlations between the SVS and Hofstede’s Values Survey, suggesting, again, differing values profiles for the two nations. If statistical testing were available for this one could no doubt be more certain; however, none are available for this particular study. A stronger set of evidence is presented by Robert, Probst, Martocchi, Drasgow, & Lawler (2000). In their study, Hofstede’s Values Scale was used along with the statistical testing in the form of structural equation modeling. Note that the Individuality versus Collectivism scale was mixed with the Power Distance scale to form Horizontal Collectivism, Horizontal Individualism, Vertical Individualism, and Vertical Collectivism. High Power Distance represents vertical societies while low Power Distance Represent horizontal societies in this case. Individualism versus Collectivism retained their original meaning. The results of this study are summarized in Table 4. Further, Hofstede’s Individualism versus Collectivism scale has been shown to correlate with Gross National Product on a per capita basis at .87 for teachers and .81 for students at the p < .01 level (Schwartz, 1994). At the time of this writing, the Gross National Product, per capita, for the United States is $36,300 (Central Intelligence Agency, 2003b), for Poland this figure is $9,500 (Central Intelligence Agency, 2003a) and within Romania this figure is $7,600 (Central Intelligence Agency, 2004). This means that if the relationship between per capita Gross National Product and Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 25 Collectivism versus Individualism remains intact, that levels of Collectivism in the United States, Poland, and Romania would be projected to be different. This should lead to differences on the semi-bipolar dimensions of Self-Transcendence versus SelfEnhancement on the SVS. Perhaps the strongest piece of evidence that a differential in the values profiles exists between the U.S. and Poland is offered by Schwartz himself. The United States has been found to be high in Mastery under Schwartz’s framework and not especially high on Affective Autonomy and Conservation. By contrast, Poland has been found to be higher in Affective Autonomy than the United States and high overall in Conservation (Schwartz, 1994). A full listing of the cultural level values profiles for Poland and the United States under the framework of the SVS is presented in Table 5 of this study. Note that these measurements were performed at the cultural level using the SVS. However, recall that regardless of level of measurement the same questionnaire is used to measure values under Schwartz’s model. Hypotheses So far this study has suggested that values are an integral part of culture. Additionally, it has been suggested that despite some shortcomings, the SVS is the instrument of choice in measuring values. Further, a variety of evidence indicates that Poland and the United States are different with regard to their values profiles. Romania remains unexplored in any study. This study seeks to address the shortcomings of SVS, specifically its reliance on principal component based analyses. Fortunately, other methods exist to analyze this data outside of SSA and oblique factor analysis. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 26 For instance, Todd & Lawson (2003) measured the values of frugal consumers with the SVS and the interrelationships of its items via PROXSCAL. The findings of this study supported a refinement the model in which the primary motivations of Conformity and Tradition were merged into one classification. Unfortunately, this study did not use all the items present in the full SVS. Additionally, only respondents from one culture, citizens of New Zealand, were used. It remains to be seen if the interrelationships of the items that compose the SVS as measured through MDS remain constant across cultures. Smallest Space Analysis and oblique factor analysis rely on correlational analysis; MDS is based upon dissimilarity between stimuli and is different. Specifically, MDS relies on a structure of interrelationships between items while factor analysis relies on covariation. It remains unclear if the primary motivations as specified by Schwartz (1992, 1994) can be measured via dissimilarity based MDS. From here a hypothesis may be formed: Hypothesis 1: Values as measured by the SVS will maintain the same structure as measured in prior studies regarding their primary motivations in the United States, Poland, and Romania when using dissimilarity based MDS methodology. Hypothesis 2: Values as measured by the SVS will maintain the same structure as measured in prior studies with regard to the semi-bipolar dimension of Conservation versus Openness to Change in the United States, Poland, and Romania when using dissimilarity based MDS methodology. Hypothesis 3: Values as measured by the SVS will maintain the same structure as measured in prior studies with regard to the semi-bipolar dimension of Self- Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 27 Transcendence versus Self-Enhancement in the United States, Poland, and Romania when using dissimilarity based MDS methodology. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 28 Method Respondents were recruited from three countries: the United States, Poland, and Romania. Note that all respondents were obtained through convenience methods. That is, friends, family, and contacts of friends and family groups were contacted and asked to fill out each country’s survey. A total of 136 United States respondents, 266 Polish respondents, and 258 Romanian respondents were recruited through this method. The primary thrust of the data collection process was to obtain as many respondents as possible regardless of subgroups with the caveats of not recruiting children, the imprisoned, or the mentally ill. Specifically, as long as a respondent was 18 or older he or she could be of any age, occupation, level of education, income level, or race. Note that for reasons which will be discussed in the analysis portion of this study, listwise deletion of respondents was imposed on the resultant data set. By listwise deletion, it is meant that if a respondent failed to answer one question within the variables of interest (for the purposes of this analysis the demographics sections and the SVS) he or she was eliminated from the data set as a whole. This resulted in a final data set with 98 United States respondents, 201 Polish respondents, and 128 Romanian respondents. The surveys used in this study contained not only the SVS but also other questions not directly related to measurement of values5. Outside of the SVS, the surveys contained different items by country. This means that three separate instruments were used. For instance, the Polish and Romanian survey contained the Marlowe-Crowne 5 It should be noted that one of the sections of the Polish survey contained the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSDS) and the Social Desirability Scale-17 (SDS-17). The original intent of including these scales was the validation of social desirability measures outside the countries of their origin. However, the manipulation failed. The results of a MANCOVA, Multiple One-Way ANOVAs, and Logistic Regression showed no, or minimal differences between the fake good and normal groups on not only the MCSDS and SDS-17 but also the SVS. As such both of these groups were combined to form the Polish sample. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 29 Social Desirability Scale and Social Desirability Scale-17 while the American survey did not. Copies of the full questionnaires, in English, may be found in Appendices B, C, and D of this study. Demographics were also included in both studies. The use of these demographics will be addressed in the Analysis portion of this study, as they will be used as statistical controls for non-representative samples. Originally, all three of the instruments for this study were composed in standard United States English. Once final versions of these instruments were completed, a rigorous double back translation process was implemented. Specifically, the respective questionnaires were translated into Polish and Romanian and back into English. Items differing significantly in their content upon back translation were then taken from their original English and translated again. This process was repeated until all of the items were translated with the appropriate content. Different translators were used at each step of the process to avoid memorization or learning of the items. Side by side comparisons of the Polish and Romanian versions of the SVS and demographics along in their respective languages as well as their original English versions and English back translations may be found in Appendices E and F of this study. Note that this study does not use the full 57 item SVS. The 57th item pertaining to Self-Indulgence was excluded as it was not present in the original study that Schwartz (1992) used to validate his instrument. Analysis For the purposes of discussion, the analysis of the questionnaires’ results may be separated into four phases. The first phase involves the alignment of demographic questions for later statistical control, the elimination of respondents who provided Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 30 incomplete data, and the selection of variables for demographic control. Phase Two involved the statistical control of demographics. The third phase centers around the production of Multidimensional Scaling Models (MDS). Phase Four involved the analysis of the primary motivation types within the MDS space as described previously by the Schwartz model through Multiple Discriminant Functions (MDF) and binary logistic regression. Phase One Demographic questions from all three countries were first standardized in terms of their name and coding within the data set. The next portion of this phase involved the elimination of respondents who did not answer all of the SVS or the needed demographic questions. At two points in this analysis statistical techniques were used that necessitated listwise deletion, controlling for demographics and the measure of association used in the production of the MDS models (covered in Phases Two and Three of this study respectively). The results of the demographic questions common to all three surveys were then compared with the purpose of selecting the variables which would offer the broadest span of statistical control yet also leave the largest group of surviving respondents with regard to missing data. Phase Two As previously stated the recruitment phase of this study involved finding as many respondents as possible, regardless of demographic background, to fill out this study’s questionnaires. This resulted in samples that were large enough for complex statistical testing but not necessarily representative of the populations of the countries of interest. As such, a statistical control was needed to eliminate the effects of these differing Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 31 backgrounds with regard to the results of the SVS. While the SVS was produced with the intent of the showing a universal values structure not only within but also between nations, studies have shown differences associated with demographics (Lee, 2003; Na & Duckitt, 2003; Feather, 1979). The decision was therefore made to control for demographics within each sample. The technique used to control influence of demographics was Multiple Linear Regression. Specifically, the dependent or predicted variables would be each of the 56 items of the SVS. The independent or predictor variables would be the demographics. Standardized residuals from this process would then be used in place of each item. Note that in the case of this study the impact of demographics on the SVS’s scores within cultures was not of interest. Rather this study’s thrust was the measurement of values and their differences in importance between cultures. Previous studies have used this technique under similar circumstances (Blake & Neuendorf, in press). The theoretical underpinnings of this process are detailed as follows. When plotting a linear regression line a line of best fit is placed within the plot of the data. Each predictor variable adjusts the slope of this line and in so doing accounts for the predicted variable’s variance. The space remaining between each data point and this line is called the residual. This distance represents the variance unaccounted for by the regression analysis. Therefore, in this study demographics were used as independent variables and SVS scores as dependent variables; the residuals were SVS scores with the influence of demographics subtracted. Specifically, in the case of this study, “controlled” means that the residual scores of this regression are adjusted for linear effects assuming Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 32 that there are no interactions among the demographics. The residuals from this portion of the analysis were used in place of the raw data for the remainder of the analysis. The regression models used the SPSS algorithm. Residuals may be produced only for subjects with data for the independent variables (demographics in this case) in SPSS. This along with analysis hurdles discussed in Phase 3 of this study necessitated listwise deletion of respondents with missing data. Specifics of the models used are as follows. The demographics (the independent variables) were entered in a block at single step. This ensured all of the demographics, regardless of their strength of association with a particular SVS score (the dependent variable), were entered into the equation. Note that the variables used did not vary by country. That is, if age were to be used in the United States model it would be used in the Polish and Romanian models too. A separate series of regressions were calculated for each national sample. Hence, the regression coefficients for a given predictor could vary from country to country. Phase Three The next phase of this analysis involved the production of a Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model (WMDS) that would produce a common space based on all of the variance from all of the countries used. Classic Multidimensional Scaling (CMDS) models were also to be constructed for each country individually. As two forms of MDS are used in this analysis, two separate designations are employed for the different techniques. CMDS refers to Multidimensional Scaling it’s most basic form, scaling of a single data matrix. WMDS refers to the technique that combines multiple data matrices to produce a common space that attempts to amalgamate all of the data Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 33 therein. Note that one of the intents of this study is to allow the interpretation of the SVS through SPSS as the data analysis platform. ALSCAL is the software that is used in SPSS to produce many of the types of MDS models. As such, from time to time allusions to ALSCAL as a software platform will be made. A brief description of both CMDS and WMDS is given here to insure that readers have the common background to understand these analyses as applied to the current study. However, if the reader is foreign to these statistics two good sources to begin familiarizing oneself would be Hair, Tatham, and Anderson (1998) and Myers (1996). CMDS is a statistical process by which the interrelationships between multiple variables may be examined. Specifically, measures of similarity or dissimilarity are employed for each unique pair of variables that the researcher wishes to examine. That is, if four variables are to be examined then six pairings of variables would be required; if five variables are to be examined ten pairings of variables would be required; and so forth. Typically, this measure is a rating of perceived similarity. The only restrictions to these measures of similarity are that they are at least ordinal in nature. For some cases Pearson Correlation Coefficients (r) are used; however, this study employs the use of different coefficient of similarity for reasons to be discussed later. These indicators of similarity produce a matrix of intervariable similarities. Drawing from the previous examples, if four variables are to be studied via MDS a four by four matrix of dissimilarity scores is produced is produced. This produces 16 distances in a matrix that are symmetrical about a dividing line of perfect similarity (perfect similarity in this case is signified through zero distance). This matrix is then used to determine a set of coordinates on a number of dimensions specified by the Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 34 researcher. These dimensions are for a hypothetical space and often produced as a graphical readout. The researcher then examines these coordinates, or the resulting plot of interpoint distances, if the results to be in the form of a graphical readout, hoping to discern the variables similarities. Typically, CMDS is performed through an iterative process by which a measure of variance relating both the original measure of dissimilarity and the locations of the variables on the new set of dimensions is produced. (Iterative in this case means a series of steps that are repeated. Each singular iteration, or pass through the instruction set, is one step.) In fact, one of the outputs of the CMDS algorithm is variance shared between to two sets of scores. Generally, this variance coefficient is similar to r2 with modifications for the specific algorithm. The interpretation of the variance coefficient produced by most CMDS software is the same as it would be for correlation squared. That is, a variation shared coefficient between a matrix of distances and the resulting CMDS output of .8211 would mean that 82.11% of the variance recorded in the scaled dissimilarity matrix may be found within the generated interpoint distances. Another measure that is also generally employed measures the level of distortion within this hypothetical space: Stress. A basic thought exercise explains the use of stress. Pretend that there are distances for three points that are known and coordinates for these three points are to be generated in two dimensions. Points one and two are ten inches apart. Point three is four inches away from point one and two. The results of these distances cannot be mapped in standard Euclidean space. Stress is the level of distortion existing in the space by the algorithm attempts to accommodate these distances. Note that standard Euclidean space is sometimes used; however other rules are sometimes used Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 35 to define these spaces. The CMDS algorithm works through various configurations of these points to minimize the stress of resulting coordinate set. Stress, like variance shared, varies between zero and one. With zero indicating no distortion within standard Euclidean space, or whichever other metric is being used, and one indicating complete and utter distortion. In most algorithms, there is an indirect relationship between variance shared and stress. Stated another way, by repeating the steps CMDS are typically iterative processes. The default maximum iterations available for these algorithms are 30 in SPSS. This limitation is arbitrary. The main reason for this limitation is to conserve processor time and is an artifact of the days when computing was performed on relatively slow mainframe computers. For the purposes of this study all MDS algorithms were set to the maximum number of allowed iterations, 999. Other measures that are used in MDS are S-stress convergence and minimum Sstress value. S-stress is squared stress, which is computed under a slightly different formula than stress though its interpretation remains largely the same. S-stress is calculated to gauge the minimum change in S-stress necessary to terminate the algorithm. That is, when a change between iterations of S-stress is encountered lower than the set level the algorithm will stop and deliver the derived coordinates. The default value for convergent S-stress in SPSS is .001. Like the maximum iterations allowed this number is arbitrary and was changed to .0001 (the minimum allowed through SPSS) to generate solutions with lesser degrees of stress. Minimum S-Stress is the lowest amount of stress that will be measured in the iterative MDS process. This is a way of measuring the level of precision involved in the Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 36 analysis. The Minimum S-Stress measure, like convergent S-Stress and Maximum iterations, are set to arbitrary levels. The default for Minimum S-stress in SPSS is 0.005. For this study, Minimum S-Stress will be set to a lower level to increase the precision of the findings. This study will use .0001, the lowest value allowed by SPSS. WMDS is used when two or more data sets are used to generate coordinates in a hypothetical space where this set of coordinates is meant to represent a model that fits these data sets as a whole. WMDS uses largely the same process described for CMDS. However, instead of fitting the data from a single matrix to a hypothetical space, multiple matrices are used. The hypothetical space that WMDS finally produces is referred to as “common space.” The term common space is used as this space demonstrates the variation present in all the data matrices used to develop it. Several measures are used in WMDS that are relevant to only this process. Specifically WMDS makes use of weirdness, importance, and subset weights. Note that r2 and stress are still produced for each individual matrix and still fall under the same rubric of interpretation as with CMDS. Weirdness is named relatively intuitively. This measure shows how divergent a given matrix is from an arbitrary midpoint of normalcy. Note that Weirdness varies between 0 and 1. A Weirdness score of 0.00 between two data sets means that they are effectively in total agreement or geometric congruence. A Weirdness score of 1.00 means that the two data matrices are in total disagreement. Another measure specific to WMDS is Importance. The Importance measure is intuitively named too. This measure shows the relative importance of a given dimension with regard to explaining interpoint dissimilarities. Like the weirdness measure, the Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 37 importance measure varies between 0 and 1. An importance score of 1 means that a given dimension explains all or nearly all of the given data sets’ variance. An importance score of 0 means that a given dimension is irrelevant or nearly irrelevant when evaluating a given dimension. Subject weights provide the basis for converting the common space to an individual space for each subject (that is, data matrix, here the nation sample). A weight is generated for each dimension for each “subject”; these weights are combined with the coordinates of each stimuli multiplicatively (more specifically, the square root of the weight) to obtain coordinates of a stimuli’s value on the dimension in question in the subject (in this case, nation sample) space. Note that solutions will be generated for one to six dimensions using CMDS and two to five dimensions using WMDS (these are the full range of solutions available through SPSS). Selection of which models will be used will be addressed after descriptions of each model are discussed. Many variations exist on the basic theme of developing these hypothetical spaces, such that differing algorithms may produce different levels of variance shared, stress, and hypothetical coordinates. However, the algorithms employed are generally stable in that they will always produce the same levels of variance shared, stress, and hypothetical coordinates for each individual data set. The data input to MDS is a matrix of inter-value dissimilarities (or similarities). In this study, the similarity is gauged by correlations between ratings of the perceived importance of the stimuli (value statements). Very often when one speaks of correlation in the social sciences he or she is referring to Pearson’s Correlation Coefficient (r). Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 38 Pearson’s r has enjoyed an incredible amount of use in the social sciences. However, the coefficient of choice for this study was Lin’s Concordance Coefficient, which will be referred to as rlc. The formula for rlc may be found in Figure 2. An introductory discussion of rlc may be found in The Content Analysis Guide Book (Neuendorf, 2002) while a more technical discussion is contained in Lin (1989). This study will give a brief discussion of and justification of its use within the performed CMDS and WMDS analyses. Specifically, consider the data sets presented in Table 6. Additionally, consider the scatterplots provided for this data in Chart 1. As indexed by r, the association for variables X and Y would be 1.00 (p < 0.001). Next consider the correlation between X and Z. Again, the correlation when calculated through r is 1.00 (p < 0.001). That is, through the use of r there appears to be no difference in the linear associations of these two pairs of variables. However, when examining this data it becomes clear that there is a constant associated with variable Z that tends to elevate its scores two units (in this case the unites are arbitrary but common to all three variables and assumed to be interval/ratio). Lin’s Concordance Coefficient considers this distortion but still has many of the same properties as r. Specifically, the same levels of significance are used for rlc as r. Additionally, just as r varies between -1.00 and 1.00 so does rlc. The interpretation of the two coefficients are largely the same with anchors occurring at -1.00, a strong negative relationship, 0, no relationship whatsoever, and 1.00 a strong positive relationship. Finally, rlc and r require the use of minimally interval level data. As such, rlc, for the hypothetical variables mentioned earlier, X and Y, would still be 1.00 (p < 0.001). However, rlc for variables X and Z would be 0.805 (p < .01). Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 39 This exercise is important when considering the use of correlation coefficients as a measure of similarity between two stimuli. Two items can be very different (as in X and Z in Chart 1), but still show a high r. Lin’s Concordance Coefficient is not prey to this problem to the same degree as it (unlike r) adjusts for elevation differences. In this case, which may be generalized to other situations involving the linear association between variables, rlc takes into account the issue of elevation. That is, X and Z’s relationship is perfect but is elevated two units by comparison to X and Y. As such, rlc is superior to r as it does not produce a figure that indicates a perfect correspondence when one does not exist. Unfortunately, programs that calculate rlc were not easily available at the time of this analysis. As such, this formula for rlc was programmed into Microsoft Excel as a formula and the association matrix needed for the later CMDS and WMDS models was generated there (Figure 3 shows the Excel formula used for this computation). Unfortunately, Excel does not support pairwise elimination of subjects with missing data. This hurdle, as well as the previously mentioned use of residuals, necessitated the use of a listwise elimination paradigm for subjects with missing data. Whether r or rlc is used a basic transform is need to convert the measure of association to dissimilarity. That is, both of these coefficients revolve around the assumption that 1 is shows perfect agreement, 0 no association, -1 perfect disagreement. If two objects are extremely similar, closer to 1 when interpreted through r or rlc, one would expect them to be closer together than two objects which are extremely different closer to -1 when interpreted through r or rlc, under the rubric of MDS in general. Therefore, a transform is needed to convert rlc to dissimilarity where 0 is no dissimilarity, Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 40 1 is a fairly large dissimilarity, and 2 is the maximum dissimilarity possible. The transform used in this study is shown in Figure 4, while its Excel equivalent is shown in Figure 5. Note that this transform is in line with typical transforms used to change coefficients of similarity to distances representing dissimilarity when using an MDS algorithm presented in Cox and Cox’s Multidimensional Scaling (2001). Phase Four Schwartz’s theory as demonstrated in Figure 1 assumes that arbitrary lines separate the primary motivation types by the values, which populate them. Neither CMDS nor WMDS are able to generate these artificial borders. An objective measure is needed to judge whether the maps produced through CMDS and WMDS can be separated into realms based on variance demonstrated through these models. Further, to maximize the amount of variance represented by the CMDS and WMDS models hyperspace dimensions will most likely be used. Hyperspace refers to dimensionalities, which exceed the standard three dimensions used by people in everyday life. While exceeding three dimensions will most likely account for a larger portion of the variance available in the original matrices of relationship discussed earlier, a limitation is encountered, as analysis via straight viewing is no longer possible. Again, an objective process or processes are needed that analyzes the relationships of the values in a space that cannot be typically comprehended by simple human observation. As such, the final phase of the analysis involves the use of a Multiple Discriminant Functions (MDF) and binary logistic regression to look into the resulting hypothetical space. MDF will be used in this study to determine how well the Schwartz model is maintained regarding the ten overarching value types for each country. Binary logistic regression will be used to evaluate how well Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 41 the dichotomies of Self-Enhancement versus Self-Transcendence and Openness to Change versus Conservation are predicted by the Schwartz models. This study will provide a brief discussion of MDF and binary logistic regression as several unique issues are presented in the case of this study. However, if the reader is unfamiliar with this form of analysis he or she should seek out an introductory multivariate text for a more complete description of these statistics (again, Hair et. al., 1998) is indispensable in this regard). MDF is a statistical analysis that uses a determinance model to predict categorical group membership from parametric data by use of canonical functions. To clarify, a determinance model is a model that uses a series of variables to predict another variable. Examples of determinance-based models include multiple regression and binary logistic regression. The essential attribute of determinance-based models is that commonalities between the independent variables (whether standardized in the case of variance or unstandardized in the form of variation) are entered in series to predict the dependent variable. Second, the independent variables used in this analysis can be continuous or categorical in nature while the dependent variable is usually categorical in nature. Third the data is fed through a canonical correlation process to produce functions, which differentiate between the groups as specified by the dependent variable. MDF produces loading of each variable on the functions that are used to discriminate between the categorical level variable. Typically, these coefficients are of great interest to researcher as they tend to indicate the relative impact of predictor variables in differentiating between categories of the predicted variable. However, in this case, the predictor variables will be the dimensions from various CMDS and WMDS Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 42 solutions. The composition of the CMDS or WMDS dimensions are inferred from the distances developed from the original data set, in this case via Lin’s Concordance Coefficient with regard to the SVS items controlled for demographic influence. This leads to a difficult interpretation of what these loadings will mean, as the CMDS and WMDS dimensions are developed based on the distances. Another output of the MDF are the centroids associated with each category of the dependent variable. These centroids are the arithmetic average of the function scores for each category. That is, a score on each function is developed for each category. Each category in this case is the primary motivation type that acts to classify individual values. The implication is that these centroids are the most typical individual value for each primary motivation type. However, whether the position of each centroid corresponds to a value which may be interpreted as something meaningful is debatable. For instance, what would one call a value that is two units away from clean but only one unit away from national security? The result is that these centroids will additionally be very difficult to interpret. Fortunately, the farther apart these centroids are from one another the better the discrimination offered by the MDF model, and this type of data regarding the model can be interpreted. The next output of MDF is the level of significance offered by each function. The practical significance level of this analysis is suspect. The input to the MDF model is an abstraction of value measures as derived from the various CMDS and WMDS solutions who themselves are derived of a theoretically limited population (the SVS scores). To clarify, Schwartz (1992) states that his values measure is exhaustive or near exhaustive in terms of the values that are measured. The question therefore becomes to what are Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 43 models to be generalized? Is this a generalization to a finite, relatively small set of values? Is this a generalization to an abstraction, via CMDS or WMDS, of a finite, relatively small set of values? This study takes the view that this method of analysis presumes the latter as opposed to the former paradigm. Specifically, the CMDS or WMDS solutions will only generalize as well as the variance it accounts for. However, care must be put forth in regard to which MDS solution best represents the data at hand. The final output of MDF is a hit and miss table that measures the number of objects (in this case values) correctly classified through the MDF formula. Typically, a derivation of Chi-Squared ( 2) called Press’ Q is used to evaluate the overall significance of the resultant classification scheme (Hair et al., 1998). However, this is a special circumstance in that 10 categories will be used to classify 56 objects. This suggests that the minimum number of objects per cell will fall below the minimum for a 2 formula. To clarify, Press’ Q evaluates the likelihood of the MDF model is classifying the individual values at a rate above chance. However, the assumptions on which Press’ Q are based may be violated. While it could be argued that data in this case are being collapsed from the full array of subjects, it could also be argued that there are simply not enough values to make the determination of the model’s overall divergence from chance. This study will therefore use two measures, Press’ Q and Cohen’s Kappa. Cohen’s Kappa ( ) was originally developed for methodologies such as ethnography and content analysis, as an alternative to percentage agreement. Typically, it is used when one wishes to judge the level of agreement beyond chance between two coders when coding the same variable Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 44 with the same stimuli (typically behavior of a human or animal). This study seeks to use Cohen’s Kappa to judge the percentage agreement beyond chance between Schwartz’s (1992) theoretical model and the current data. Note that Cohen’s Kappa varies like a normal percentage, falling between 1.00 and 0.00. That is, a Kappa value of 1.00 indicates total agreement and while a Kappa value of 0.00 indicates total disagreement (Neuendorf, 2002). The conceptual formula along with an example data set is provided in Figures 6 and 7 respectively; the Excel formula employed in Kappa’s calculation is available in Figure 8. As a baseline the basic percentage agreement between Schwartz’s (1992) theoretical model and the present data as analyzed by MDS are also provided. Further, this analysis report will use binary logistic regression to look into the CMDS and WMDS models. Binary logistic regression is similar to MDF in that both are determinance based and both rely on the dependent variable being categorical in nature. Binary logistic regression differentiates between categories of a dichotomous variable. The variables used as predictors in binary logistic regression may all be categorical in nature. Once again, this study will provide a brief overview of binary logistic regression and its unique application in this case. However, as binary logistic regression is complex in and of itself much less in this unique application, readers unfamiliar with this statistic should familiarize themselves with its methodology and implementation via an introductory multivariate statistics text. Binary logistic regression relies on adjusting the position of the function of a natural log (an S-shaped function) in differentiating between two categories of a dichotomous variable. Each independent, or predictor variable, adjusts the position of several S-shaped functions until an optimal solution is reached. The caveats discussed Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 45 regarding MDF still apply to this statistic as the output is very similar. However, the conceptualization of this statistic is much easier to understand with regard to how it applies to the various hypothetical spaces developed by the CMDS and WMDS models. Specifically, the S-shaped functions are used to differentiate between the two categories of interest. These functions would serve as dividing wall in the hyperspace solutions. They would be the equivalent to hyperspace barrier separating the two categories of the MDS (CMDS and WMDS) models arrived at in Phase Three of this study. An oddity of this study that must discussed regards prediction with regard to MDF and binary logistic regression. MDF and binary logistic regression are usually used to predict group membership. These predictions are then compared to the actual classification results. In this case, these statistics are being used to interpret a hypothetical space. As such, the results of these statistics with regard to this study become the actual results. While the Schwartz model, the model to which these results will be compared, become the predicted results when interpreting the resultant classification tables. To avoid confusion this study will use the label Empirically Expected when referring to results predicted under the Schwartz (1992) model and Observed for the results of the statistical analyses of this study. The final portion of this analysis seeks to measure the levels of similarity between the interrelationships of the SVS’s value statements between each country. To this end the raw coefficients of relationship for each value interrelationship will be measured between the United States, Poland, and Romania. That is, the rlc value for the first item of the SVS “Equality” will have been compared to the second item “Inner Harmony” will be compared between each nation. Each unique pair’s concordance coefficient will be Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 46 used as the data for a coefficient of relationship (this will be the values as developed from the 56 by 56 item matrix described in Phase 2 of this analysis). The requirements of this analysis suggest that not only could the rlc values be measured but also the distances as developed in through the CMDS algorithm. The distances developed through CMDS are interval/ratio in nature. As such, Pearson’s Correlations Coefficient (previously defined as r) may be employed in this case. However, the rlc values are of an ordinal nature. This means that Kendal’s Tau-b will be used6. Kendal’s Tau-b will also be used with the rlc values allowing comparisons between the original set of concordance values as well as the CMDS derived coordinate sets. This portion of the analysis will rely on only one half of the concordance matrices previously mentioned. That is, only a triangular matrix under the main diagonal of perfect correlations will be measured. In the case of the dimensions generated via CMDS an extrapolation of the Pythagorean Theorem will be used to generate interpoint distances7. Note that this will extrapolation assumes squared Euclidean distance; however, this analysis will use Euclidean distance. Results Just as in the Analysis portion of this report the Results section is divided into four phases. The first phase involved the alignment of demographic questions for later 6 Note Kendal’s Tau-b measures the number of inversions relative to rank order while compensating for a large number of ties. This statistic measures the number of times one score occurs before another when comparing two data sets and is conceptually similar to Gamma, Spearman’s Rho, and Pearson’s r (Brewer n.d.). The other possible statistic would have been Spearman’s Rho. Spearman’s Rho was rejected as this statistic as it was unknown what the number of ties was in the data set and Kendal’s Tau-b tends to be more appropriate when the data is more cross-tab like (Emerson n.d). 7 The classic Pythagorean Theorem is meant for two dimensions and may be illustrated as A2 + B2 = C2. Where the variable A represents the position of the variable on dimension one and B represents the position of the variable on dimension two. The extrapolation in this case is for six dimensions and may be illustrated as: A2 + B2 + C2 + D2 + E2 + F2 = G2. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 47 statistical control, the elimination of respondents who provided incomplete data, and the selection of variables for demographic control purposes. Phase Two involved the statistical control of demographics. The third phase centers on the production of MDS models. Phase Four involved the analysis of the primary motivation types within the MDS spaces as described previously by the Schwartz model through Multiple Discriminant Functions (MDF) and binary logistic regression. Phase One Appendix G of this study gives the SPSS Syntax used to align the demographic questions from the three countries surveyed. Note that Syntax related to Income questions from all three countries is included though this question was eventually excluded. At this point in the process no screening of demographic variables had taken place with regard to their feasibility and missing data. A summary of the demographics for the respondents used in this study is available in the 3rd, 4th, and 5th columns of Table 9. A series of trade offs were made with regard to which demographics were likely to best filter out the influence of non-national background and which demographics received such a poor response rate such that they would hinder further analysis. Details of this process are available in Tables 9, 10, and 11 of this study. Note that multiple models were considered with regard to the elimination of demographics. However, the variables with most missing data were household size and income. The choice was made to eliminate these two variables as otherwise their inclusion when applied listwise deletion issues discussed in the analysis portion of this study would have produced too small a set of respondents for analysis. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 48 Phase Two Recall that Phase Two of the Analysis involved the dummy coding of a select group of demographics to control for non-representative sampling. Categorical variables such as level of education and occupation type were dummy coded while age due to its ratio nature was left in its original format. A full breakdown of the dummy categories and their univariate descriptors is given in the first two columns of Table 9 of this study (it is suggested that the reader also view the instruments employed, in their original English, appropriate language, and English back translations, presented in Appendices H, I, and J for the United States, Poland, and Romania respectively). Each country had its own regression model. That is, separate models were employed using the same variables for each country. The use of separate models was necessary as it was unclear if each nation would be effected differently by the demographics available (see Blake and Neuendorf (in press) for an alternate perspective). Further, the same demographic questions from each country were used. That is, religion was asked in Poland but not in the United States. Therefore, as it is unclear what effect religion has in both Poland and the United States, it was excluded from the regression model. Note that the prototypes of the SPSS syntax employed in this analysis may be found in Appendix K. A total of the 168 regression models were employed in this study (56 for each country). As the inclusion of all 168 models would provide little usable information regarding this study’s hypotheses and grossly inflate the size of this report a sample of the regression models is available (in Tables 12 through 17). Further, certain statistics common to all of the regression models specifically the degrees of freedom, and Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 49 collinearity diagnostics, for each country’s overall model set are provided in Tables 18 and 19 respectively. Note that the overall models rarely achieve significance. This suggests the low variance accounted for relative to the number of variables acting as predictors indicates little predictive power of the demographics. Differences among individuals in their residual scores should be comparatively free of even these modest effects of respondent demographics. Phase Three Recall that to implement an MDS analysis coefficients of dissimilarity must be created for each variable to every other variable. As previously discussed the coefficient of choice for this analysis was Lin’s Concordance Coefficient. The resultant distributions of concordance ratios for this process are described by country in Table 20. Note that the overall distributions are roughly normal when measured by the Skewness and Kurtosis measures. Further, note that the ranges of Lin’s Concordance Ratio vary within a range of 0.813 and -0.413. Considering that this statistic penalizes relationships in which the variables are differentially elevated, it could be inferred that there are several strong relationships between the value measures. Note that the SPSS Syntax used in the production of the both the WMDS and CMDS solutions is available in Appendices L and M of this study. Initially, a WMDS solution was attempted that would consider the data from all three countries, the United, States, Poland, and Romania. The resulting R2 and stress coefficients may be found in Tables 21 and 22, respectively. Scatterplots of these statistics are available in Charts 2 and 3 respectively. Weights, Weirdness, and Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 50 Importance statistics may be found in Tables 23, 24 and 25, respectively. For reasons of reproducibility, the number of iterations used by ALSCAL to produce these solutions may be found in Table 26. The variance shared and stress coefficients are of particular concern. Overall, the commonality among the three data sets (matrices) is lower than anticipated. Typically, for an MDS solution to be considered of value, variance explained would need to exceed 0.80 or 0.90 as a guideline (Myers, 1996). When averaged, none of the solutions offered exceeds this mark. Of further concern is that the variance explained tends to be dominated by one or two countries for each dimension of the solution. The most unusual outcome of this analysis is that the five dimensional solution offers a lower amount of variance explained than the four dimensional solution. Overall, it seems that the Polish data set is dominating the analysis in terms of variance explained. The weights for each country by dimension offer more evidence of instability. Specifically, it appears that in all of the solutions except the five dimensional one, that each country dominates a separate dimension. That is, for the two-dimensional solution an even mix of dominance is held by the U.S. and Romania, while Poland dominates the second dimension. In the three-dimensional solution, Romania holds sway over dimension one, while the United States and Poland control dimension two. Poland, in the three dimensional solution, holds sway over dimension two. This effect is even more profound in the four dimensional solution, where Poland controls the first dimension, Romania the second, the United States the fourth, and the third dimension is a somewhat even mix. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 51 The Weirdness coefficients tell an even more interesting story. Overall, the highest Weirdness ratings are associated with the four dimensional solution, the one with the highest variance explained. While the three and five dimensional solutions are about even in terms of their levels of Weirdness, the two-dimensional solution, the one with the lowest variance explained, offers the lowest marks on this index. Overall, these indicators could be taken to indicate that ALSCAL had trouble arriving at a WMDS solution that could push the requirements of three data sets together. The best solution when one considers all of the available indicators would seem to be the five dimensional solution. This solution offers the highest mix of dominance when comparing country to dimension, lower levels of stress overall, and lower weirdness indicators by country. However, this solution has its pitfalls too. This solution offers the lowest representation of the Romanian data set both in terms of variance explained and stress. Alternate solutions were arrived at via CMDS. Three separate models were developed, one for each country. Note that when considering this methodology, comparisons in terms of distance cannot be made between countries. That is, since the models developed by CMDS consider the data of only one country, the results are applicable only to that one country. The implication is that separate models, in terms of multiple discriminant functions and binary logistic regressions must be considered in Phase Four. Variance explained and Stress by data set and dimensionality of solution are given in Table 27. Scatterplots of these data are available in Charts 4 and 5, respectively. Once again, for reasons of reproducibility the number of iterations used by the ALSCAL Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 52 algorithm for each country’s data set is available in Table 26. These solutions follow the guidelines of a roughly inverse relationship between variance explained and stress. Further, the rough inverse relationship between stress and dimensionality is also maintained. Finally, the direct relationship between dimensionality and variance explained is seen (that is, as the number of dimensions goes up so does the level of variance explained). Note that not much improvement is demonstrated by moving from a five to a six dimensional solution. However, for the purposes of verifying the Schwartz model every available amount of variance explained was taken into consideration. As such, the six dimensional solution was tested in Phase 4 of this study. Phase Four Tables 28 through 43 detail the development of the MDF models as they differentiate between primary motivation types under the Schwartz model. The SPSS syntax used to derive these solutions is available in Appendix N. Again, it must be emphasized that it is unclear what relation the loadings of each MDS dimension (whether this is a CMDS or WMDS solution) have with regard to each individual multiple discriminant function (these loadings are shown in Tables 28, 32, 36, and 40). It is notable, however, that the number of discriminant functions equals the number of WMDS or CMDS dimensions. This not withstanding, in most cases no one dimension dominates the contributions of variation to a discriminant function. The previous observation could be taken to mean that no one dimension within the MDS models is responsible for the differentiation of an overall motivation type or overall group of motivation types. This is given further credence when one examines the centroids for each primary motivation type (values for the centroids are available in Tables 30, 34, 38, and 42). That is, generally Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 53 speaking, no one primary motivation type comes out as more clearly classified at the aggregate level than any other. A notable exception to this statement is that the primary motivation type “Power” tends to have a high centroids value in the WMDS common space, and CMDS United States and Romanian space models. Perhaps, this suggests that this motivation type is clearly differentiated in these three models and stands out in terms of aggregate distance when compared to the other motivation types. Table 44 offers a summary of MDF results in the form of Press’s Q and Cohen’s Kappa for common space developed through the WMDS algorithm and the individual spaces developed for each CMDS algorithm by country. Notably, Press’s Q produced extraordinarily high values. Once again, caution is recommended in the interpretation of this statistic. One possible interpretation is that the assumptions of this statistic were violated due to low cell counts. However, Cohen’s Kappa offers a more enlightening look at these models viability. All of the models offer a classification rate near or above 50% even after controlling for chance. The beginnings of a trend are also notable in that the common space model produced the highest classification rate while Romania offers the lowest classification rate. The observation that Romania fared the worst of the four models set forth is apparent when one examines Tables 45 through 48. Remember when reading these results that in the case of this study MDF and binary logistic regression are being used to look into the MDS models that were generated in Phase 2. As such, the results of these models are labeled the Observed results. The results that are predicted by Schwartz are labeled Empirically Expected and are the analog to the predicted results. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 54 Again, the common space model, whose results are illustrated in Table 45, seemed to fair the best of the set with the United States coming in second. Of great interest is the area where the primary motivation types Benevolence, Tradition, Conformity, and Security intersect with regard to the expected and observed statistics. This area shows a low level of differentiation with a large number of values falling outside of the diagonal. This is not surprising when one considers that Tradition and Conformity tend to occupy the same piece of the SSA pie in the Schwartz model (Figure 1). The specifics of the binary logistic regression models (detailed in Tables 49 through 56) tend to repeat the story of the MDF analysis. The SPSS syntax used to derive these models is available in Appendix O. Each predictor variable, in this case each dimension, from each model produces its own function. However, in this case one or two WMDS or CMDS dimensions dominate each function. This suggests that the differentiation of Openness to Change versus Conservation and Self-Transcendence versus Self-Enhancement relies almost entirely on these variables. However, this may also have to do with the simplicity of the differentiation. That is, these are simplistic choices between two classifications and unlikely to require input from the various dimensions. Notably, the Romanian model with regard to differentiation between Openness to Change versus Conservation, detailed in Table 52, failed to achieve significance. Cross tabulations showing the hits and misses of the logistic models are given in Tables 57 through 64. These tend to reinforce the issue of the common space and United States model tending to more closely mimic the Schwartz model. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 55 Tables 65 and 66 are the logistic analogs to Table 44, for Openness to Change versus Conservation and Self-Transcendence versus Self-Enhancement. The values obtained for Press’s Q for all eight models are again extremely high (note that this statement considers each individual space model, of which there are three, and each common space model, for all of which there are two uses of binary logistic regression). However, unlike the MDF results, the statistic maintains its interpretation. Again, the trend mentioned with regard to the common space having the largest proportion of correctly classified individual level values is maintained with the exception of SelfTranscendence versus Self-Enhancement, in which the United States fairs the best. Notably, the kappa values are lower for the dichotomous differentiations than for the MDF differentiations. Further, Romania faired far worse than the United States and Poland with regard to classification of values (Table 44, percentage agreement and percentage agreement beyond chance). Tables 67 through 70 give listing of each item of the SVS as classified by their primary motivation types through MDF under the Schwartz model. Further, positions under the semi-bipolar dimensions of Self-Transcendence versus Self-Enhancement and Openness to Change versus Conservation, under the Schwartz model and as classified through CMDS solutions by binary logistic regression. Table 71 give a listing of the value statements that adhered to the expected motivation primary types and positions under the semi-bidirectional dimensions. While Table 72 and Chart 6 show the value statements that maintained the correct value primary motivation categories across all four of the space developed through MDS by country. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 56 A final result of keen interest is that Romania fared the worst of the three countries studied. Overall, Romania showed the widest fluctuations with regard to variance shared and stress in when the WMDS solution was attempted (Charts 2 and 3) and the lowest number of correct classifications of values under the bipolar value dimensions (Tables 57 and 64). Further, evidence of this is suggested when the similarities between value distances were measured within the CMDS models through multiple uses of Kendal’s Tau-b coefficients between countries (Table 73, Pearson’s Correlation Coefficients are provided in Table 74). Romania has the lowest level of association when compared to the U.S. and Poland. This is demonstrated again in Table 75 where r is used. In this table, Romania and the United States show the lowest level of similarity while the United States and Poland are the most similar. Note that Table 76 offers the equivalent Kendal’s Tau-b measures of association for comparability purposes between the concordance and CMDS distance measures. This results summarized in this table tend to suggest the same structure of relationships between the three countries. Discussion The results of this study support some, but not other, aspect of the Schwartz’s model. Several reasons for the lack of strong support may exist. This could be due to a number of factors such as the lack of stability regarding the overall architecture of the SVS’s interrelationships across cultures, a bias in the SVS itself towards western values or amalgam thereof, this study’s control of demographics, or a basic incompatibility of this study’s analysis with SSA. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 57 The results of this study suggest that the Schwartz model best fits an overall amalgamation of United States, Poland, and Romania but has trouble when applied to individual models. Unfortunately, the common space developed via ALSCAL’s WMDS algorithm does not appear to be a good fit to the data. Specifically, it appears that there is a large amount of instability in the common space solutions developed by this algorithm. This would mean that each space developed through the CMDS process for the United State, Poland, and Romania individually are very different in terms of their properties. Credence is given to this observation when one considers that Tables 72 through 74 show that the input data and corresponding CMDS distance solutions when compared between countries are highly disparate. This suggestion tends to be further supported by the results of the individual CMDS solutions with regard to the classification of values into their 10 primary motivation types via MDF. Further evidence is found in the two semi-bipolar value dimensions (Openness to Change versus Conservation and Self-Enhancement versus Self-Transcendence) for which Romania fared the worst and the U.S. common space fared the best. The pieces of evidence, disparity between the common space distances and concordance measures, and differing results from the discriminant regressions and logistic regressions strongly suggest that each common space is unique. First, the common space distances and concordance measures show that the dissimilarities between value statements when compared between countries are disparate. This means that each matrix (whether in their original form of concordance measures or in their scaled form via ALSCAL) simply are not comparable. This supported by the failure of ALSCAL to Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 58 render a satisfactory WMDS group space. Further, the patterns of values falling within the primary motivations of Schwartz’s model are different. This means that each space is conforming uniquely to the overarching theory Schwartz has set forth. The implication that each of these common spaces is unique in structure when compared to common spaces of other nations means that the interrelationships of the SVS’s value statements are not universal. However, the results of the MDF and binary logistic regression analyses (illustrated via hit rate tables) suggest that the overall classification of values retains a very high degree of predictive validity. That is, the higher level of abstraction offered by Schwartz (1992) in the form of Primary Motivators and two semi-bipolar dimensions of Self-Enhancement versus Self-Transcendence and Conservation versus Openness to Change may be universals of this model. While these constraints are important, this does not diminish the utility of the SVS. However, this implies that the SVS may not be a scale in the traditional sense. Rather, in light of these results it appears that the SVS represents a taxonomy of value types. That is, a scale implies that each value statement has a specific relationship with other value statements, with each value statement definitively falling within each primary motivation type. A taxonomy implies that the value statements tend to fall into a given primary motivation type. The distinction of the term taxonomy as opposed to scale centers on the idea that the primary motivation types are more categorical in nature as opposed to a strict continuum as described under the original model. The overall architecture of the SVS values may be more stable across cultures with regard to certain value statements, however. Table 71 shows the value statements that maintain their correct classification with regard to both primary motivations as Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 59 analyzed via MDF and the semi-bipolar dimensions as analyzed via binary logistic regression. Only 19 of the 56 statements meet the criteria of being correctly classified in the United States, Poland, and Romania. One of these value statements, item 43 “Capable,” was not correctly classified under the WMDS common space model. Note that there appears to be no commonality to these value statements. Notably, there is no apparent difference between whether these are end-states, such as “Sense of belonging,” or instrumental values, such as “Creativity.” Further, the binary logistic analysis played virtually no role in the correct classification of these values. That is, as long as any of the 56 values were classified correctly under the rubric of the ten primary motivation types, as analyzed via MDF, they were highly unlikely to have been misclassified under the rubrics of two semi-bipolar dimensions of Self-Enhancement versus Self-Transcendence and Conservation versus Openness to Change, as analyzed via binary logistic regression. Perhaps of equal concern to the issue of disparate value structures between nations is that the CMDS United States model best supports the Schwartz model (The common space derived from the WMDS algorithm is a better fit; however, this space has previously been discussed and deemed a poor fit to the data). Two conclusions result from this observation. First, it could be that the SVS was not translated properly. This seems unlikely. The translation process was quite rigorous and the back translations show a large amount of congruity to the original. The second option is that the Schwartz model is biased towards American or Western European style values. That is, the better performance of the United States could be an artifact of the scale’s original composition in English. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 60 Another possibility is that the statistical control of demographics may have altered the value scores. This suggests the Schwartz model is heavily correlated with demographics. Remember that the original studies by Schwartz used a highly homogeneous population composed of students and teachers. This may have “controlled” for the demographic influence. This hypothesis is very unfortunate as the logical conclusion would be that value structure, again, is not fully universal. Specifically, the issue would be that demographics within cultures unduly pollute this structure. However, this leads to an even more complex problem: Are the demographics effecting the values of those studied or are the values of those studied effecting their demographics (that is, by impacting one’s readiness to seek higher education or to engage in activities to enhance income)? This line of logic quickly becomes akin to the classic nature versus nurture debate. Generally speaking, it is neither nature nor nurture; rather it is the interaction of the two that make the individual, or in this case his or her values. The next possibility is that in using listwise deletion with regard to both demographics and the SVS this study selected for a specific class of respondents. Again, this leads back to the question of whether the SVS is truly universal. That is, if the SVS does truly tap the universal values structure guiding everyone then the use of respondents who choose to fill out an entire questionnaire as opposed to those who do not should be irrelevant. Another possibility is that CMDS and WMDS combined with MDF and binary logistic regression simply do not yield results comparable to SSA. This theory seems to be unlikely in explaining the level of divergence from Schwartz’s model. However, Todd & Lawson (2003) performed an MDS analysis of the SVS using PROXSCAL a Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 61 conceptual descendent of ALSCAL and confirmed the SVS’s structure through this method. Unfortunately, this study used a shortened version of the SVS. Additionally, this study did not use ALSCAL. Instead, PROXSCAL, a newer MDS algorithm, was used in the production of this space. Further, Todd & Lawson (2003) chose to use shortened scale8 and ipsatize9 the results by individual respondent. This would call into question the compatibility of the current study’s results with that of Todd & Lawson’s (2003). The lack of universality between the value statements interrelationships also means that countries may be classified by differential scores on SVS. That is the study by Sverko (1995), mentioned in the introduction as using Hofstede’s Values Scale and hierarchical clustering, could be repeated using the SVS. Specifically, this means that cultures could be classified via this instrument. Another research application of the previous discussion regarding SVS’s nonuniversal structure is that this tends to open a window that would allow the researcher to look into the impact of a culture’s values on various hypothetical constructs. That is, these individual differences in scores, as represented in miss-classifications under the various analysis models (MDF and binary logistic regression), could be correlated with such scales as the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960) or the Global Innovativeness Scale (Hurt, Joseph, & Cook, 1977). This could provide new insights as the nature of these constructs as they vary by culture. 8 This scale was composed of seven instead of nine points. Negative one was anchored with opposition to the respondent’s values while the opposite end, five, was anchored as highly important to the respondent’s values. 9 In this case, the operational definition of “ipsatize” is to subtract the mean score of each respondent’s overall values score from each individual values statement rating. This procedure is often alternately termed, “centering.” Hence, if a respondent had a mean score of two for the overall questionnaire used and had rated the first item five; the “ipsatized” score would be three. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 62 A final avenue of further research that was not pursued by this study would be to examine the dimensions of the resultant CMDS spaces. Specifically, one could look for a discernable pattern and use this pattern (if it exists) to reorganize the scale. Unfortunately, this would most likely require the use of more than three cultures’ data if one wished to propose an alternative to Schwartz’s taxonomical system. The three cultures examined in this study give but an initial glimpse into the potential diversity offered across the world. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 63 Works Cited Astill, B. R. (1998, July 04). Humanised statistics: An investigation of social values in a senior secondary school milieu. Retrieved March 15, 2004, from University of Adelaide AARE Conference Papers Publishing And Refereeing Web site: http://www.aare.edu.au/94pap/astib94.297 Bardi, A., & Schwartz S. H. (2001). Value hierarchies across cultures: Taking a similarities perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(3), 268-290. Bilsky, W. & Koch, M. (2002). On the Content and Structure of Values: Universals or Methodological Artefacts? Retrieved April 10, 2004, From The University of Münster, Department of Psychology Web site: http://wwwpsy.unimuenster.de/inst4/AEBilsky/pdf/Bilsky/Tagungsbeitraege/Koch.pdf Blake, B. F., & Neuendorf, K. (In Press). Cross national differences in website appeal: A framework for assessment. Journal of Computer Mediated Communication. Braithwaite, V., & Scott, W. (1991). Values. In J. Robinson, P. Shaver, & L. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes (pp. 651 – 753). San Diego, CA: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich Publisher. Brewer, M. (n.d.). Some useful statistics. Retrieved April 13, 2004, From Colby College Department of Government faculty Web site: http://www.colby.edu/~mbrewer/usefulstatistics.htm Central Intelligence Agency. (2003a, March 19). Poland. In The World Factbook 2002. Retrieved May 15, 2003, from http://www.cia.gov/cia/publications/factbook/geos/pl.html Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 64 Central Intelligence Agency. (2003b, March 19). United States. In The World Factbook 2002. Retrieved May 15, 2003, from http://www.cia.gov/cia/publications/factbook/geos/us.html Central Intelligence Agency. (2003, April 6). Romania. In The World Factbook 2003. Retrieved April 6, 2004, from http://www.cia.gov/cia/publications/factbook/geos/ro.html Cox, T. F., & Cox M. A. (2001). Introduction. In Multidimensional scaling (2nd ed.). New York: Chapman and Hall/CRC. Crowne, D. P. & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24(4), 349-354. Emerson, M. S. (n.d.). Masters in Public Administration (MPA) 504 Quantitative Methods. Retrieved May 6, 2004 From California State Polytechnic University Pomona Web site: http://www.csupomona.edu/~smemerson/quantitative/syllabus.htm Feather, N. T. (1979). Value correlates of Conservation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 37(9) 1617-1630. Ferreira-Marques, J., & Miranda, M. J. (1995). Developing the Work Importance Study. In V. Sverko & B. Super (Eds.), Life Roles, Values, and Careers (pp. 231-241). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Goldsmith, R., & Hofacker, C. (1991). Measuring consumer innovativeness. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 19(3) 209-221. Goodwin, R., & Tinker, M. (2002). Value priorities and preferences for a relationship partner. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(8) 1339-1349. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 65 Guion, R. (1998). Assessment, measurement, and prediction for personnel decisions. Mahway. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Hair, J. F. Jr., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data analysis with readings (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Hurt, H., Joseph, K., & Cook, C. (1977). Scales for the measurement of innovativeness. Human Communication Behavior, 4(1), 58-65. International Business Center. (2003). Geert Hofstede's Cultural Dimension Scores. Retrieved May 16, 2003 from The Web's Leading Resource for International Business Etiquette and Manners with Geert Hofstede’s Cultural Dimensional Web site: http://www.cyborlink.com/besite/geert_hofstede_dimensions.htm Kelly, K., Silverman, B. I., & Cochrane, R. (1972). Social desirability and the Rokeach Value Survey. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 6, 84-87. Lee, A. R. (2003). Stability and change in Korean values. Social Indicators Research, 62(1-3), 93-117. Lin, L. I. (1989). A concordance correlation coefficient to evaluate reproducibility. Biometrics, 45, 255-268. Middleton, K. L., & Jones, J. L. (2000). Socially desirable response sets: The impact of country culture. Psychology and Marketing, 17(2), 149-163. Myers, J. H. (1996). Segmentation and positioning for strategic marketing decisions. Chicago, Illinois: American Marketing Association Na, E. Y., & Duckitt, J. (2003). Value consensus and diversity between generations and genders. Social Indicators Research, 62(1-3), 411-435. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 66 Neuendorf, K. A. (2002). The content analysis guidebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Randall, D. M., Hwo, Y. P., & Pawelk, P. (1993). Social desirability bias in crosscultural ethics research. The International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 1(2), 185-202. Robert, C., Probst, T. M., Martocchio, J. J., Drasgow, F., & Lawler, J. L. (2000). Empowerment and continuous improvement in the United States, Mexico, Poland, and India: Predicting fit on the basis of the dimension of power distance and individualism. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(5), 643-658. Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology: Vol. 25 (pp. 1-65). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Beyond individualism/collectivism: New cultural dimensions of values. In U. Kim, H. Triandis, C. Kagicbasi, S. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.) Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Methods, and Applications (pp. 85-119). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Schwartz, S. H., Verkasalo, M., Autonovsky, A., & Sagiv, L. (1997). Value priorities and social desirability: Much substance, some style. British Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 3-18. Solomon, M. R. (1999). Glossary. In Consumer behavior (4th ed., p. 563). Uppersaddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 67 Spini, D. (2003). Measurement of equivalence of 10 value types from the Schwartz Values Survey across 21 countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34(1), 3-21. Sverko, B. (1995). The structure and hierarchy of values viewed cross-nationally. In Sverko, V. & Super, B. (Eds.), Life Roles, Values, and Careers (pp. 225-240). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Todd, S., & Lawson R. (2003). Towards an understanding of frugal consumers. Australasian Marketing Journal. 11(3), 8 -18. Trentini, G., & Muzio, G. B. (1995). Values in a cross-cultural perspective: A further analysis. In Sverko, V. & Super, B. (Eds.), Life Roles, Values, and Careers (pp. 241-251). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Waters, J. B. (1999, April 27). Fall, 2000 - Internet lecture assignment. In Performance Appraisal and Counseling. Retrieved May 18, 2003 from California State University, Domingez Hills faculty Web site: http://som.csudh.edu/jbell/303assignments/303ETHIC.doc Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 68 Figure 1 The Interrelationships of the Primary Motivation Types and Bipolar Value Dimensions for the Schwartz Social Values Scale From: Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology: Vol. 25 (pp. 1-65). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 69 Figure 2 Lin’s Concordance Coefficient (rlc) 2( rlc = a2 + n ab ) n b2 + (MeanA n Where a = Each deviation score (Variable A score minus the mean for A) b = Each deviation score (Variable B score minus the mean for B) n = The number of units scored in common to both variables. MeanB)2 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 70 Figure 3 Linn’s Concordance Coefficient as an Excel Formula rlc = ( 2 * COVAR ( A2 : A99 , B2 : B99 ) ) / ( VARP ( A2 : A99 ) + VARP ( B2 : B99 ) + ( AVERAGE( A2 : A99 ) - AVERAGE( B2 : B99 ) ) * ( AVERAGE ( A2 : A99 ) - AVERAGE ( B2 : B99 ) ) ) Where: The data set in question ranges from cell A2 to B99. The cell references A2 to A99 (A2:A99) specify the first subject’s data range. The cell references B2 toB99 (B2:B99) specify the second subject’s data range. AVERAGE ( ) = The arithmetic average function. COVAR ( ) = The covariance, the average of the products of deviations for each data point pair function. VARP ( ) = The variance for the entire population function. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 71 Figure 4 Concordance to Distance Transformation Distance = ( ABS | MA – 1 | ) Where: ABS | | = the absolute value for the value contained therein. MA = The measure of association, in this case Lin’s Concordance Coefficient (rlc) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 72 Figure 5 Concordance to Distance Transformation as an Excel Formula = ( ABS ( A1 - 1 ) ) Where: A1 is the cell of reference. ABS( ) = Returns the absolute value for the value contained therein. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 73 Figure 6 The Conceptual Formula for Cohen’s Kappa ( ) = PAO - PAE 1 - PAE Where: PAO = Percentage Agreement between the Schwartz model and this study’s data PAE = (1 / n2)( pmi) n = Number of Units pmi = Product of each individual marginal Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 74 Figure 7 An Example Computation for Cohen’s Kappa ( ) Using the example data set from Table 8 and the marginal calculations from Table 9. PAO = 2 + 3 + 2 = 7 PAE = = = = (1 / n2) ( pmi) (1 / 102) (9 + 15 + 8) (1 / 100) (32) (.32) Cohen’s kappa = = = PAO - PAE 1 - PAE .70 - .32 1 - .32 .38 .68 = .56 From Neuendorf, K. A. (2002). The content analysis guidebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 75 Figure 8 Cohen’s Kappa ( ) as an Excel Formula = ( ( ( SUM ( C60 , D61 , E62 , F63 , G64 , H65 , I66 , J67 , K68 , L69 ) ) / 56 ) - ( ( 1 / ( 56 ^ 2 ) ) * ( ( C70 * M60 ) + ( D70 * M61 ) + ( E70 * M62 ) + ( F70 * M63 ) + ( G70 * M64 ) + ( H70 * M65 ) + ( I70 * M66 ) + ( J70 * M67) + ( K70 * M68 ) + ( L70 * M69 ) ) ) ) / ( 1 - * ( ( 1 / ( 56 ^ 2 ) ) * ( ( C70 * M60 ) + ( D70 * M61 ) + ( E70 * M62 ) + ( F70 * M63 ) + ( G70 * M64 ) + ( H70 * M65 ) + ( I70 * M66 ) + ( J70 * M67 ) + ( K70 * M68 ) + ( L70 * M69 ) ) ) ) Where: The cell range is a cross tabulation matrix with marginal sums ranging from C60 to M70 (including marginal sums). Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 76 Table 1 Values and Example Instrumental Iterations of the Rokeach Values Survey Value Instrumental Iteration Ability Utilization Use my skill and knowledge Achievement Have results which show that I have done well Advancement Get ahead Aesthetics Make life more beautiful Altruism Help people with problems Authority Tell others what to do Autonomy Act on my own Creativity Discover, develop, or design new things Economics Have a high standard of living Life-Style Living according to my ideas Personal Development Develop as a person Physical Activity Get a lot of exercise Prestige Be admired for my knowledge and skills Risk Do risky things Social Interaction Do things with other people Social Relations Be with friends Variety Have every day different some way from the one before it. Working Conditions Have good space and light in which to work From: Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology: Vol. 25 (pp. 1-65). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 77 Table 2 Primary Motivations, Definitions of Primary motivations and Items that Compose the Primary Motivations for the Schwartz Social Values Scale Primary Definition Motivation Items That Compose the Primary Motivations on the Schwartz Social Values Scale Power Social status and prestige, Social Power, Authority, Wealth, control or dominance over Preserving my Public Image. people and of resources. Achievement Personal success through Successful, Capable, Ambitious, demonstrating competence Influential. according to social standards. Hedonism Pleasure and sensuous Pleasure, Enjoying Life. gratification for oneself. Stimulation Excitement, novelty, and Daring, A Varied Life, An Exciting challenge in life. Life. Self-Direction Independent thought and action Universalism Benevolence Creativity, Freedom, Independent, choosing, creating, exploring. Curious, Choosing own Goals. Understand appreciation, Broad-Minded, Wisdom, Social tolerance, and protection for the Justice, Equality, A World at Peace, welfare of all people and for A World of Beauty, Unity with nature. Nature, Protecting the Environment. Preservation and enhancement of Helpful, Honest, Forgiving, Loyal, the welfare of people with whom Responsible. one is in frequent personal contact. Tradition Respect, commitment, and Humble, Accepting my Portion in acceptance of the customs and Life, Devout, Respect for Tradition, ideas that traditional culture or Moderate. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 78 religion provides oneself. Table 2 (Continued) Primary Motivations, Definitions of Primary Motivations and Items that Compose the Primary Motivations for the Schwartz Social Values Scale Primary Definition Motivation Items That Compose the Primary Motivations on the Schwartz Social Values Scale Conformity Restraint of actions, inclinations, and Politeness, Obedient, Self- impulses likely to upset or harm others Discipline, Honoring Parents and violate social expectations or and Elders. norms. Security Safety, harmony, and stability of Family Security, National society, of relationships, and of self. Security, Social Order, Clean, Reciprocation of Favors. From: Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology: Vol. 25 (pp. 1-65). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 79 Table 3 Differential Value Ratings for Poland and the United States as measured by the Hofstede Values Survey Nation Bipolar Value Index Power Individuality Masculinity Uncertainty Long Term Distance versus versus Avoidance Orientation Collectivism Femininity Poland 55 60 65 78 37 United States 40 91 62 46 29 From: Braithwaite, V. & Scott, W. (1991). Values. In J. Robinson, P. Shaver, & L. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes (pp. 651 – 753). San Diego, CA: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich Publisher. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 80 Table 4 Differences in Vertical Versus Horizontal, Individualism and Collectivism between Poland and the United States Statistic Mean Standard Deviation Alpha Poland 27.20 4.60 0.60 Untied States 25.19 4.50 0.64 Poland 20.19 5.10 0.50 Untied States 18.26 5.60 0.68 Poland 40.05* 5.10 0.64 United States 37.80* 5.10 0.60 Poland 32.90 5.70 0.69 United States 33.75 5.70 0.77 Horizontal Collectivism Vertical Individualism Vertical Collectivism Horizontal Individualism *Significantly different as measured by ANOVA (p < 0.01). Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 81 Table 5 Mean cultural level values for Poland and the United States as measured by the Schwartz Social Values Scale Nation Cultural Level Value Conservatism Poland United States Affective Intellectual Hierarchy Mastery Egalitarian Autonomy Autonomy 4.31 3.13 4.09 2.53 4.00 4.82 3.90 3.65 4.20 2.39 4.34 5.03 Commitment From: Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology: Vol. 25 (pp. 1-65). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 82 Table 6 Hypothetical Variables for Association in Arbitrary Units Variables Observation X Y Z (N = 10) (N = 10) (N = 10) 1 1 1 3 2 2 2 4 3 3 3 5 4 4 4 6 5 5 5 7 6 6 6 8 7 7 7 9 8 8 8 10 9 9 9 11 10 10 10 12 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 83 Table 7 An Example Data Set for Cohen’s Kappa ( ) Coder B Coder A Total 1 2 3 1 2 1 0 3 2 0 3 0 3 3 1 1 2 4 Total 3 5 2 10 From Neuendorf, K. A. (2002). The content analysis guidebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 84 Table 8 Example Calculation of Marginal Products for the Example Data set Illustrating Cohen’s Kappa ( ) Category Marginals Products of Marginals Coder A Coder B AxB 1 3 3 9 2 5 3 15 3 2 4 8 Total 10 10 - From Neuendorf, K. A. (2002). The content analysis guidebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 85 Table 9 Means of Select Demographics for the Schwartz Social Values Survey (SVS) (After Listwise Deletion of Non-Responders to Demographics and the SVS) Original Variable Dummy Question Codes United States Poland Romania (n = 98) (n = 201) (n = 128) 38.388* 29.313* 22.125* .082 .050 .039 .571 .363 .164 .184 .075 .031 .020 .015 .047 .204 .458 .820 Home .051 .020 .008 Retired .020 .020 .008 Married .633 .313 .047 .061 .050 .023 .061 .343 .430 .010 .035 .039 .245 .075 .367 .429 .303 .094 .255 .000 .055 Age Age Employment Self-Employed Status Employed FullTime Employed PartTime Unemployed Student Marital Status Respondent Country Divorced Separated or Widowed Level of High School Education Technical School / Training Some College / University College / University Graduate or Professional School Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 86 Table 9 (Continued) Means of Select Demographics for the Schwartz Social Values Survey (SVS) (After Listwise Deletion of Non-Responders to Demographics and the SVS) Original Variable Dummy Respondent Original Variable Question Codes Country Question Dummy Codes .439 .239 .141 .122 .060 .070 Sales .041 .025 .094 Clerical .041 .139 .016 .020 .020 .031 .000 .015 .070 .663 .667 .547 Occupation Professional Managerial Executive Labor with Technical Training Labor without Technical Training Gender Gender (Female = 1) *Standard Deviations for the United States, Poland, and Romania, were 11.717, 14.296, and 3.299, respectively. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 87 Table 10 Missing Values Summary by Country for Demographics Variable Country United Poland Romania Total (n = 136) (n = 266) (n = 258) (N = 660) Age 3 18 17 38 Level of Education 2 17 20 40 Household Size 1 16 29 46 Employment Status 0 17 22 39 Marital Status 1 17 16 34 Level of Education 2 17 20 39 Occupation* 0 17 21 38 Income Level 7 40 26 73 States * The question regarding Occupation involved a skip pattern controlled by Employment Status. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 88 Table 11 Demographics Common to All Three of the Surveys for the United States, Poland, and Romania Question Age Data Level Ratio Number of Entered into Removed Level Categories Regression Not Yes Not Applicable Applicable Marital Status Categorical 3 to 4* Yes Single Categorical 2 Yes Male Ordinal/Categorical** 6 Yes Some High School Categorical 7 Yes Not Applicable*** Categorical 7 Yes Not Applicable***** Ordinal/Categorical** 7 to 14 No Not Applicable Ratio Not Applicable No Not Applicable Gender Level of Education Employment Status Occupation**** Income Level Household Size * The Marital Status question for the United States had four categories: Single, Married, Divorced/Separated, and Widowed. Poland and Romania had three categories: Single Married, and Divorced/Separated/Widowed. In order to standardize this question between countries the United States version of this question had its last two categories collapsed into one. ** Both Level of Education and Income Level could be interpreted as ordinal level variables. However, as regression does not make any differentiation between ordinal and categorical data levels, these two variables defaulted to the later data level. *** The question regarding employment status instructed respondents to check all categories that applied. While typically, when dummy coding categorical variables for entry into a parametric equation one deletes one category to avoid multi-colinearity, this condition allowed us to use all available levels from this question. **** Poland used a different skip pattern than the United States and Romania. For the former self-employment allowed the respondent to proceed to the Occupation question for the later two self-employment did not. As such, all data from Poland with regard to occupation for respondents who were self-employed was eliminated. While this did Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 89 result in the loss of data the issue of parallel or standardized levels of analysis between countries was deemed more important at this stage of the analysis. ***** Respondents were instructed to skip this question depending on their employment level. This alleviated the restriction of deleting one category for the purposes of entry to regression. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 90 Table 12 Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 7 “Sense of Belonging” United States Sample (n = 98) Model 1 Variable B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. (Constant) 4.243 .894 - 4.745 .000 Age -.011 .019 -.083 -.591 .556 Self Employment -.467 .721 -.081 -.648 .519 Full Employment .938 .554 .295 1.692 .095 Part-Time -.087 .570 -.021 -.153 .879 Unemployed 2.266 1.345 .203 1.685 .096 Student .811 .533 .207 1.523 .132 Homemaker 1.369 .803 .191 1.705 .092 Retired .941 1.264 .084 .744 .459 Married .429 .506 .131 .847 .399 Divorced Separated .223 .893 .034 .250 .803 High School -.586 .756 -.089 -.775 .441 Technical School or .104 1.669 .007 .062 .950 -.387 .463 -.106 -.836 .406 -.542 .423 -.150 -1.282 .204 Professional -.028 .415 -.009 -.068 .946 Managerial Executive .173 .596 .036 .290 .773 Employment or Widowed Training Some College / University Graduate or Professional School Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 91 Sales .791 .878 .099 .901 .371 Clerical -.739 .910 -.093 -.812 .419 Table 12 (Continued) Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 7 (Sense of Belonging) United States Sample (n = 98) Variable Model 1 Labor with (continued) Technical B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. 1.875 1.329 .168 1.412 .162 1.091 .379 .327 2.882 .005 Training Gender Note: R2 = .248 (p < 0.226) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 92 Table 13 Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 55 “Successful” United State Sample (n = 98) Variable B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. 4.450 .723 - 6.157 .000 Age .001 .015 .012 .087 .931 Self Employment .442 .582 .096 .758 .451 Full Employment .982 .448 .384 2.190 .032 Part-Time .748 .461 .229 1.623 .109 Unemployed 1.580 1.087 .176 1.453 .150 Student 1.092 .431 .348 2.536 .013 Homemaker .233 .649 .041 .359 .720 Retired 1.362 1.022 .152 1.333 .187 Married -.510 .409 -.194 -1.248 .216 Divorced Separated -.494 .722 -.094 -.684 .496 High School -.906 .611 -.172 -1.484 .142 Technical School or -1.553 1.349 -.123 -1.151 .253 .172 .374 .058 .459 .648 -.192 .342 -.066 -.561 .577 Professional .633 .335 .248 1.888 .063 Managerial Executive .980 .482 .254 2.035 .045 Sales 1.383 .710 .216 1.949 .055 Clerical .270 .736 .042 .368 .714 1 Model (Constant) Employment or Widowed Training Some College / University Graduate or Professional School Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 93 Table 13 (Continued) Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 55 “Successful” United State Sample (n = 98) Variable Model 1 Labor with (Continued) Technical B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. .492 1.074 .055 .458 .648 -.296 .306 -.110 -.967 .337 Training Gender Note: R2 = .239 (p < 0.272) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 94 Table 14 Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 1 “Equality” Polish Sample (n = 201) Variable B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. 5.014 .499 - 10.054 .000 Age -.021 .019 -.163 -1.108 .270 Self-Employed -.867 .807 -.104 -1.074 .284 Employed Full Time .367 .751 .097 .489 .625 Employed Part Time .745 .765 .108 .974 .331 Unemployed -.321 1.093 -.021 -.293 .770 Homemaker .626 1.155 .048 .542 .588 Retired -1.048 1.075 -.081 -.975 .331 Married .429 .486 .110 .883 .379 Divorced Separated 1.092 .765 .131 1.428 .155 High school .601 .354 .157 1.701 .091 Technical School / -.380 .824 -.038 -.461 .645 .805 .687 .116 1.172 .243 College / University 1.119 .555 .283 2.016 .045 Professional -.715 .470 -.168 -1.520 .130 Managerial Executive -1.240 .725 -.162 -1.710 .089 Sales -.276 .974 -.024 -.284 .777 Clerical -1.059 .591 -.202 -1.792 .075 Model 1 (Constant) or Widowed Training Some College / University Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 95 Labor with Technical .582 1.077 .045 .541 .589 -.857 1.069 -.057 -.802 .424 Training Labor without Technical Training Table 14 (Continued) Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 1 “Equality” Polish Sample (n = 201) Variable Model 1 Gender (Continued) Note: R2 = .123 (p < 0.207) B Std. Error Beta t-value Sig. .534 .298 .138 1.789 .075 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 96 Table 15 Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 25 “A Varied Life” Polish Sample (n = 210) Variable B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. 4.646 .402 - 11.559 .000 Age -.004 .015 -.035 -.234 .815 Self-Employed .208 .651 .031 .320 .749 Employed Full Time .522 .605 .174 .862 .390 Employed Part Time .630 .617 .115 1.021 .309 Unemployed .538 .881 .045 .611 .542 Homemaker .102 .931 .010 .110 .912 Retired -.823 .866 -.080 -.949 .344 Married -.509 .392 -.164 -1.300 .195 Divorced Separated .596 .616 .090 .966 .335 High School .258 .285 .085 .905 .367 Technical School / 1.007 .664 .128 1.516 .131 .122 .553 .022 .220 .826 College / University -.107 .447 -.034 -.238 .812 Professional .166 .379 .049 .438 .662 Managerial Executive .412 .584 .068 .705 .482 Sales .381 .785 .041 .486 .628 Clerical -.392 .476 -.094 -.824 .411 Labor with Technical .583 .868 .057 .672 .502 Model 1 (Constant) or Widowed Training Some College / University Training Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 97 Labor without Technical Training .815 .861 .069 .946 .345 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 98 Table 15 (Continued) Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 25 “A Varied Life” Polish Sample (n = 210) Variable Model 1 Gender (Continued) Note: R2 = .091 (p < 0.585) B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. .306 .241 .100 1.271 .205 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale 99 Table 16 Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 1 “Equality” Romanian Sample (n = 128) Variable B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. 4.817 3.907 - 1.233 .220 .049 .126 .065 .390 .697 Self-Employed -1.997 1.603 -.155 -1.246 .216 Employed Full Time 1.408 1.497 .209 .940 .349 Employed Part Time 2.123 2.123 .148 1.000 .320 Unemployed -.232 1.256 -.020 -.185 .854 Student .665 1.309 .103 .508 .612 Retired 1.310 4.791 .046 .273 .785 Married -.922 1.225 -.078 -.753 .453 Divorced Separated or .522 1.660 .032 .314 .754 High school -.922 1.903 -.183 -.485 .629 Technical School / -3.200 2.250 -.249 -1.422 .158 -1.072 1.904 -.208 -.563 .574 College / University -.877 2.131 -.103 -.411 .682 Graduate or -.868 2.273 -.079 -.382 .703 Professional -.120 .806 -.017 -.149 .882 Managerial Executive .816 1.077 .084 .758 .450 Sales -1.560 1.266 -.183 -1.232 .221 Clerical -1.939 1.864 -.097 -1.040 .301 Labor with Technical -1.121 1.323 -.078 -.847 .399 Model 1 (Constant) Age Widowed Training Some College / University Professional School Training Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale100 Table 16 (Continued) Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 1 “Equality” Romanian Sample (n = 128) Model 1 Variable B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. Gender -.399 .976 -.041 -.409 .683 -.576 .516 -.115 -1.116 .267 (Continued) Labor without Technical Training Note: R2 = .151 (p < 0.595) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale101 Table 17 Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 26 (Wisdom) Romanian Sample (n = 128) Variable B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. 4.912 2.938 - 1.672 .098 Age .075 .095 .133 .789 .432 Self-Employed .261 1.205 .028 .217 .829 Employed Full Time .765 1.126 .154 .679 .498 Employed Part Time .529 1.597 .050 .331 .741 Unemployed -.624 .944 -.072 -.660 .510 Student .035 .984 .007 .035 .972 Retired -4.857 3.603 -.232 -1.348 .181 Married -.904 .921 -.104 -.982 .329 Divorced Separated .422 1.249 .035 .338 .736 High school -.695 1.431 -.187 -.486 .628 Technical School / -1.362 1.692 -.143 -.805 .423 -.699 1.432 -.183 -.488 .627 College / University -1.335 1.603 -.211 -.833 .407 Graduate or -.478 1.710 -.059 -.279 .780 Professional -.133 .606 -.025 -.220 .827 Managerial Executive -.163 .810 -.023 -.201 .841 Sales -.679 .952 -.108 -.714 .477 Model 1 (Constant) or Widowed Training Some College / University Professional School Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale102 Clerical -.793 1.402 -.053 -.566 .573 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale103 Table 17 (Continued) Summary of Straight Entry Linear Regression for Selected Demographics Predicting Schwartz Social Values Scale Item 26 (Wisdom) Romanian Sample (n = 128) Variable Model 1 Labor with B Std. Error Beta T-Value Sig. .586 .995 .055 .589 .557 .144 .734 .020 .196 .845 -.636 .388 -.172 -1.638 .104 (Continued) Technical Training Labor With Out Technical Training Gender Note: R2 = .122 (p < 0.824) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale104 Table 18 Degrees of Freedom Summary for Demographic Regression Models by Country Source Country United States (n = 98) Poland Romania (n = 201) (n = 128) Regression 20 20 21 Residual 77 180 106 Total 97 200 127 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale105 Table 19 Independent Variable Collinearity Statistics for Hierarchical Regression by Country Independent Variable Tolerance United Poland VIF Romania States United Poland Romania States Age .501 .224 .290 1.996 4.465 3.449 Self-Employed .623 .522 .515 1.605 1.914 1.943 Employed Full-Time .322 .123 .162 3.105 8.108 6.190 Employed Part-Time .498 .398 .364 2.009 2.515 2.750 Unemployed .671 .916 .705 1.491 1.092 1.419 Student .526 .618 .197 1.900 1.617 5.084 Home .777 .714 .279 1.287 1.401 3.585 Retired .759 .316 .741 1.317 3.160 1.350 Married .408 .582 .787 2.453 1.719 1.271 Divorced Separated or .528 .571 .056 1.892 1.752 17.870 High School .739 .705 .261 1.353 1.419 3.828 Technical School / Training .862 .494 .059 1.160 2.023 16.971 Some College .612 .247 .129 1.634 4.044 7.773 Graduate or Professional .714 .400 .186 1.400 2.498 5.383 Professional .572 .545 .632 1.747 1.833 1.582 Managerial Executive .636 .700 .655 1.573 1.429 1.528 Sales .803 .384 .365 1.245 2.601 2.742 Clerical .748 .711 .929 1.337 1.406 1.076 Labor with Technical .687 .958 .936 1.455 1.044 1.068 .757 .813 .797 1.321 1.230 1.255 Widowed School Training Gender Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale106 Table 20 Univariate Descriptors of the 56 by 56 Matrix of Lin’s Concordance Coefficients for the Schwartz Social Values Scale Measure Country United States Poland Romania Mean 0.1991 0.3201 0.3773 Median 0.1951 0.3316 0.3811 Std. Deviation 0.1728 0.1309 0.1485 Variance 0.0299 0.0171 0.0221 Skewness 0.0692 -0.3609 -0.2172 Std. Error of Skewness 0.0624 0.0624 0.0624 Kurtosis -0.1255 -0.1657 0.1527 Std. Error of Kurtosis 0.1246 0.1246 0.1246 Range 1.1462 0.7548 1.0244 Minimum -0.4127 -0.0856 -0.2117 Maximum 0.7335 0.6691 0.8127 25 0.0811 0.2294 0.2899 50 0.1951 0.3316 0.3811 75 0.3162 0.4177 0.4744 Percentiles Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale107 Table 21 Variance Shared by Solution for Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model by Country Number of Dimensions Variance Shared Average United States Poland Romania 2 0.409 0.344 0.571 0.312 3 0.530 0.362 0.621 0.607 4 0.594 0.53 0.648 0.604 5 0.564 0.636 0.714 0.343 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale108 Table 22 Stress by Solution for Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model Number of Dimensions Stress Average United States Poland Romania 2 0.346 0.364 0.293 0.374 3 0.261 0.302 0.231 0.246 4 0.211 0.219 0.197 0.215 5 0.186 0.165 0.163 0.224 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale109 Table 23 Country Weight by Dimension and Dimensionality of Solution for the Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Models Full Country Dimensionality Country Weight by Dimension 1 2 3 4 5 United States .4617 .3617 - - - Poland .5054 .5617 - - - Romania .4313 .3543 - - - United States .2271 .3379 .4430 - - Poland .3350 .5411 .4652 - - Romania .6548 .3176 .2782 - - United States .3205 .2092 .3157 .5327 - Poland .5367 .3145 .4742 .1912 - Romania .3249 .6115 .3003 .1861 - United States .3139 .2445 .2684 .5230 .3636 Poland .5815 .4705 .3035 .1754 .1771 Romania .2753 .2502 .3634 .1885 .1920 of Solution 2 3 4 5 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale110 Table 24 Weirdness by Dimension and Dimensionality of Solution for the Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model Country Weirdness by Dimensionality of Solution 2 3 4 5 United States .0976 .2775 .4075 .3147 Poland .1239 .2034 .2788 .3061 Romania .0676 .3931 .3588 .2002 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale111 Table 25 Importance by Dimension and Dimensionality of Solution for the Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model Full Dimensionality Importance of Each Dimension by Dimension Number 1 2 3 4 5 2 .2182 .1906 - - - 3 .1975 .1693 .1633 - - 4 .1654 .1722 .1382 .1183 - 5 .1708 .1146 .0987 .1133 .0668 of Solution Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale112 Table 26 Iterations to Convergence for the Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model and Classic Multidimensional Scaling Models by Country Versus Number of Dimensions Number of Dimensions Iterations to Convergence Common United States Poland Romania Space 1 - 5 6 4 2 6 5 5 7 3 7 6 6 5 4 23 9 6 7 5 32 6 8 6 6 - 10 7 6 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale113 Table 27 Stress and Variance Shared by Country Versus Number of Dimensions Number of Dimensions Stress United Poland Variance Shared Romania States United Poland Romania States 1 .45380 .41320 .38512 .42520 .56493 .61324 2 .27102 .24293 .26843 .62267 .74186 .69643 3 .19378 .18193 .19085 .71340 .79633 .78326 4 .14902 .14099 .15174 .77658 .84460 .82188 5 .11320 .11423 .11648 .83524 .87395 .86707 6 .09503 .09588 .09506 .86345 .89504 .89511 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale114 Table 28 Standardized Discriminant Function Loadings for the Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model’s Common Space Primary Motivation Type classification Variable Discriminant Function 1 2 3 4 5 Common Space Dimension 1 0.715 0.695 -0.046 -0.370 -0.014 Common Space Dimension 2 -0.577 0.716 -0.365 0.350 -0.076 Common Space Dimension 3 0.340 0.414 0.701 0.550 -0.036 Common Space Dimension 4 -0.559 0.214 0.392 -0.375 0.768 Common Space Dimension 5 0.877 -0.238 -0.453 0.471 0.404 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale115 Table 29 Correlations between Discriminant Functions and Dimensions for the Weighted MultiDimensional Scaling Model’s Common Space and the Schwartz Social Values Scale Variable Function 1 2 3 4 5 Common Space Dimension 1 .472 .616* -.113 -.616 -.071 Common Space Dimension 2 -.380 .601* -.529 .460 .051 Common Space Dimension 3 .150 .211 .759* .596 -.045 Common Space Dimension 4 -.193 .135 .202 -.179 .934* Common Space Dimension 5 .325 -.103 -.421 .459 .704* * Largest absolute correlation between each variable and any discriminant function. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale116 Table 30 Mean Scores for each primary Motivation Type (centroids) for the Discriminant Functions Analyzing the Weighted Multidimensional Scaling model of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Primary Motivation Type Common Space Model Discriminant Function 1 2 3 4 5 Power 3.594 2.450 0.011 0.123 -0.015 Achievement 2.296 0.287 -0.507 -0.709 0.659 Hedonism 2.960 -1.822 1.044 -0.417 -0.421 Stimulation 2.507 -1.231 0.138 -1.609 -0.714 Self-Direction 1.013 -1.653 -0.294 0.549 0.011 Universalism -0.861 -1.520 -0.827 0.376 -0.008 Benevolence -2.608 -0.600 0.723 -0.523 0.110 Tradition -2.077 2.336 -0.887 -0.248 -0.577 Conformity -2.152 1.700 0.081 -0.244 0.411 Security -0.032 0.915 0.882 1.013 -0.041 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale117 Table 31 Common Space Tests of Significance for Multiple Discriminant Regression Analyzing the Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model’s Common Space of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Test of Function(s) Discriminant Functions 1 through 5 2 through 5 3 through 5 4 through 5 Wilks' Lambda 5 0.016 0.100 0.385 0.598 0.881 197.080 109.463 45.385 24.460 5.994 45 32 21 12 5 Significance 0.000 0.000 0.002 0.018 0.307 Canonical Correlation 0.918 0.861 0.597 0.568 0.344 Chi-square Degrees of Freedom Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale118 Table 32 Standardized Discriminant Function Loadings for the Classic Multidimensional Scaling United States Model’s Primary Motivation Type Classifications Variable Discriminant Function 1 2 3 4 5 6 United States Space Dimension 1 0.774 -0.796 0.017 0.152 -0.335 0.423 United States Space Dimension 2 1.068 0.302 0.082 -0.190 0.130 -0.093 United States Space Dimension 3 -0.036 1.070 -0.164 0.080 -0.350 0.294 United States Space Dimension 4 -0.181 0.195 0.924 0.066 0.170 0.308 United States Space Dimension 5 0.323 0.139 0.135 0.882 -0.056 -0.380 United States Space Dimension 6 0.095 0.064 -0.468 0.374 0.683 0.447 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale119 Table 33 Correlations between Discriminant Functions and Dimensions for the Classic MultiDimensional Scaling Model for the United Space and the Schwartz Social Values Scale Variable Function 1 2 3 4 5 6 United States Space Dimension 1 .698* .283 .133 -.422 .390 -.292 United States Space Dimension 2 -.069 .088 .853* .083 .257 .432 United States Space Dimension 3 .099 .055 .122 .861* -.098 -.471 United States Space Dimension 4 .024 .031 -.328 .301 .739* .505 United States Space Dimension 5 .001 .554 -.192 .131 -.600* .528 United States Space Dimension 6 .268 -.371 -.003 .211 -.554 .662* * Largest absolute correlation between each variable and any discriminant function. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale120 Table 34 United States Mean scores (centroids) for each primary Motivation Type with Regard to Discriminant Functions for Classic Multidimensional Scaling Model of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Primary Motivation Type United States Model Discriminant Function 1 2 3 4 5 6 Power -4.340 -0.331 1.040 0.071 -0.377 0.216 Achievement -1.311 -0.277 -1.313 1.342 0.261 0.016 Hedonism -1.201 1.896 -0.276 -1.309 0.035 -0.356 Stimulation -1.226 1.393 0.475 -0.083 0.621 -0.892 Self-Direction 0.169 1.784 -0.332 -0.325 0.035 0.168 Universalism 1.252 1.994 0.242 0.456 -0.196 0.039 Benevolence 2.174 -0.887 0.133 -0.310 0.012 0.199 Tradition 0.824 -1.546 1.380 0.388 0.517 -0.072 Conformity 1.160 -2.185 -0.325 0.178 -0.817 -0.488 Security -0.924 -1.631 -0.774 -0.666 0.190 0.099 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale121 Table 35 Tests of Significance for the Multiple Discriminant Regression Analyzing the Classic Multidimensional Scaling United States’ Space Model of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Test of Function(s) Wilks' Lambda Chi-Square Degrees of Freedom Significance Canonical Correlations Discriminant Functions 1 through 6 2 through 6 3 through 6 4 through 6 5 through 6 6 0.017 0.087 0.332 0.551 0.810 0.923 190.170 115.001 51.828 28.038 9.910 3.749 54 40 28 18 10 4 0.000 0.000 0.004 0.061 0.448 0.441 0.893 0.860 0.630 0.566 0.351 0.277 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale122 Table 36 Standardized Discriminant Function Loadings for the Classic Multidimensional Scaling Polish Space Model’s Common Space Primary Motivation Type Classification Variable Discriminant Function 1 2 3 4 5 6 Polish Space Dimension 1 0.698 0.772 -0.132 0.077 -0.106 -0.021 Polish Space Dimension 2 -0.838 0.614 -0.229 -0.036 0.048 0.090 Polish Space Dimension 3 -0.206 0.297 0.619 0.739 0.232 0.010 Polish Space Dimension 4 0.176 0.477 0.698 -0.520 0.300 0.226 Polish Space Dimension 5 0.220 -0.262 -0.089 0.181 -0.284 0.918 Polish Space Dimension 6 0.431 -0.210 -0.552 0.044 0.766 0.214 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale123 Table 37 Correlations between Discriminant Functions and Dimensions for Classic MultiDimensional Scaling Model Polish Space and the Schwartz Social Values Scale Variable Function 1 2 3 4 5 6 Polish Space Dimension 1 -.639* .539 -.423 -.082 .119 .318 Polish Space Dimension 2 .544 .688* -.285 .212 -.307 -.098 Polish Space Dimension 3 -.065 .094 .431 .836* .320 .018 Polish Space Dimension 4 .055 .169 .561* -.642 .365 .329 Polish Space Dimension 5 .126 -.058 -.372 .085 .897* .178 Polish Space Dimension 6 .033 -.066 -.022 .142 -.321 .933* * Largest absolute correlation between each variable and any discriminant function. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale124 Table 38 Polish Mean scores (centroids) for each Primary Motivation Type for Discriminant Functions Analyzing the Classic Multidimensional Scaling (WMDS) Model of the Schwartz Social Values Scale (SVS) Primary Motivation Type Polish Model Discriminant Function 1 2 3 4 5 6 Power -1.581 -3.206 0.304 -0.707 0.367 0.088 Achievement -2.124 -0.954 0.273 0.510 -0.266 -0.306 Hedonism -4.147 1.183 0.271 -0.330 -0.762 0.213 Stimulation -3.214 0.943 1.271 0.860 1.092 0.097 Self-Direction -0.674 0.910 -1.044 0.381 -0.039 0.124 Universalism 0.740 1.729 0.351 -0.272 0.162 -0.240 Benevolence 1.105 1.527 0.388 -0.246 -0.002 0.170 Tradition 2.184 -1.750 1.328 0.165 -0.418 -0.022 Conformity 2.444 0.688 -1.431 -0.834 -0.050 -1.718 0.644 -0.166 0.083 0.028 0.196 -0.093 Security Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale125 Table 39 Tests of Significance for Multiple Discriminant Regression of the Classic Multidimensional Scaling Polish Space Model Analyzing the Schwartz Social Values Scale Test of Function(s) Wilks' Lambda Chi-Square Discriminant Functions 1 through 6 2 through 6 3 through 6 4 through 6 5 through 6 6 0.018 0.087 0.357 0.698 0.847 0.963 189.320 115.032 48.370 16.895 7.806 1.769 54 40 28 18 10 4 0.000 0.000 0.010 0.530 0.648 0.778 0.891 0.871 0.699 0.419 0.347 0.192 Degrees of Freedom Significance Canonical Correlations Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale126 Table 40 Standardized Discriminant Function Loadings for the Classic Multidimensional Scaling Analysis of the Romanian Space Model’s Common Space Classifying the Primary Motivation Types Variable Discriminant Function 1 2 3 4 5 6 Romanian Space Dimension 1 -0.923 0.321 0.240 0.491 -0.043 0.257 Romanian Space Dimension 2 -0.111 -0.888 0.303 0.209 0.280 0.144 Romanian Space Dimension 3 1.039 0.175 0.295 0.259 -0.139 0.255 Romanian Space Dimension 4 0.218 0.005 -0.806 0.253 0.275 0.443 Romanian Space Dimension 5 0.132 0.480 0.300 -0.081 0.834 -0.055 Romanian Space Dimension 6 -0.238 0.004 0.204 -0.639 -0.080 0.717 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale127 Table 41 Correlations between Discriminant Functions and Dimensions for Classic MultiDimensional Scaling Model Romanian Space and the Schwartz Social Values Scale Variable Function 1 2 3 4 5 6 Romanian Space Dimension 1 .551* .173 .440 .457 -.272 .434 Romanian Space Dimension 2 -.017 -.799* .314 .271 .389 .193 Romanian Space Dimension 3 .068 -.016 -.748* .265 .327 .508 Romanian Space Dimension 4 -.417 .329 .273 .691* -.130 .384 Romanian Space Dimension 5 .046 .321 .244 -.127 .902* -.069 Romanian Space Dimension 6 -.081 .015 .163 -.639 -.068 .744* * Largest absolute correlation between each variable and any discriminant function. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale128 Table 42 Romanian Mean Scores (centroids) for each Primary Motivation Type with regard to Discriminant Functions for Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Primary Motivation Type Romanian Model Discriminant Function 1 2 3 4 5 6 Power -3.477 -1.059 0.349 0.152 -0.083 0.140 Achievement -0.915 0.335 -0.791 0.112 -0.194 -0.105 Hedonism -1.903 0.478 0.184 -0.198 -0.354 -0.430 Stimulation -2.342 0.318 0.061 -0.949 0.550 0.128 Self-Direction -0.359 0.663 -0.644 0.330 0.367 0.031 Universalism 0.927 0.148 0.414 -0.363 0.167 -0.065 Benevolence 0.903 0.333 -0.181 -0.234 -0.349 0.183 Tradition 1.731 -1.789 -0.451 -0.044 0.110 -0.094 Conformity 1.318 -0.155 0.044 0.327 -0.100 0.004 Security 0.638 0.306 0.861 0.449 0.039 0.005 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale129 Table 43 Tests of Significance for Multiple discriminant Regression of the Classic Multidimensional Scaling Romanian Space Model of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Test of Function(s) Wilks' Lambda Chi-square Discriminant Functions 1 through 6 2 through 6 3 through 6 4 through 6 5 through 6 6 0.099 0.373 0.601 0.795 0.905 0.976 108.647 46.327 23.923 10.809 4.693 1.141 54 40 28 18 10 4 0.000 0.228 0.686 0.902 0.911 0.888 0.857 0.616 0.493 0.349 0.270 0.155 Degrees of Freedom Significance Canonical Correlations Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale130 Table 44 Multiple Discriminant Regression Analyses Overall Significance of and Percentage Agreement Beyond Chance for the Schwartz Social Values Scale’s Ten Primary Motivation Types Country Statistic Press’s Q - Cohen’s Kappa ( ) - Significance of Discriminant Percent Agreement Modela Beyond Chance (N = 56) (N = 56) Common Space 323.8** 79.8% United States 248.6** 70.0% Poland 195.6** 61.6% Romania 128.0** 49.9% **p < 0.001 a Note that Press’ Q is based on Chi- Squared ( 2). Within the matrix generated by this analysis there were more than five cells with a count of zero. This calls into question the validity of this statistic for this application. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale131 Table 45 Observed versus Empirically Expected Categorization of Values by Primary Motivation for Common Space of the Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Analyzed by Multiple Discriminant Regression Observed Primary Motivation Type Primary Motivation Type as Empirically Expected 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1. Power 5 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 6 2. Achievement 0 3 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 4 3. Hedonism 0 0 3 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 4 4. Stimulation 0 0 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 2 5. Self- 0 1 0 0 6 0 0 0 0 0 Direction 7 6. Universalism 0 0 0 0 0 8 0 0 0 0 8 7. Benevolence 0 0 0 0 0 1 8 0 0 0 9 8. Tradition 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 3 1 0 4 9. Conformity 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 2 3 0 5 10. Security 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 6 7 5 5 3 2 7 9 9 5 4 7 56 Total Table 46 Observed Versus Empirically Expected Categorization of Values by Primary Motivation for the United States’ Space Classic Multidimensional Scaling Model Analyzed By Multiple Discriminant Regression Observed Primary Motivation Type Primary Motivation Type as Empirically Expected 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1. Power 5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 5 2. Achievement 0 4 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 1 6 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale132 3. Hedonism 0 0 2 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 4 4. Stimulation 0 0 1 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 3 5. Self-Direction 0 0 0 0 4 1 0 0 0 0 5 6. Universalism 0 0 0 0 1 8 0 0 0 0 9 7. Benevolence 0 0 0 0 0 0 5 0 0 0 5 8. Tradition 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 3 1 0 5 9. Conformity 0 0 0 0 0 0 3 0 3 0 6 10. Security 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 6 8 5 5 3 2 7 9 9 5 4 7 56 Total Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale133 Table 47 Observed versus Empirically Expected Categorization of Values by Primary Motivation for the Polish Space Classic Multidimensional Scaling Model Analyzed by Multiple Discriminant Regression Observed Primary Motivation Type Primary Motivation Type as Empirically Expected 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1. Power 4 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 2. Achievement 0 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 3. Hedonism 0 0 3 0 0 0 0 0 0 4. Stimulation 0 0 0 2 0 0 0 0 0 5. Self-Direction 0 1 0 0 5 1 1 0 0 6. Universalism 0 0 0 0 1 5 3 0 0 7. Benevolence 0 0 0 0 0 2 4 0 0 8. Tradition 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 3 1 9. Conformity 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 3 10. Security 1 0 0 0 1 1 0 1 0 5 5 3 2 7 9 9 5 4 Total Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale134 Table 48 Observed Versus Empirically Expected Categorization of Values by Primary Motivation for the Romanian Space Classic Multidimensional Scaling Model Analyzed my Multiple Discriminant Regression Observed Primary Motivation Type Primary Motivation Type as Empirically Expected 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1. Power 3 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 4 2. Achievement 1 3 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 6 3. Hedonism 1 0 2 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 5 4. Stimulation 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 2 5. Self- 0 1 0 0 4 0 0 0 1 3 9 Direction 6. Universalism 0 0 0 0 0 5 1 0 0 0 6 7. Benevolence 0 0 0 0 1 1 4 0 0 0 6 8. Tradition 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 4 1 0 6 9. Conformity 0 0 0 0 0 0 2 1 1 0 4 10. Security 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 0 1 4 8 5 5 3 2 7 9 9 5 4 7 56 Total Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale135 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale136 Table 49 Logistic Regression Classification Results for Openness to Change Versus Conservation of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Common Space Model generated by Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Model’s Common Space Independent Variable B S.E. Wald Sig. Exp (B) Common Space Dimensions 1 .522 .498 1.097 .295 1.685 Common Space Dimensions 2 3.924 1.436 7.471 .006 50.614 Common Space Dimensions 3 2.657 1.047 6.438 .011 14.248 Common Space Dimensions 4 2.356 .952 6.125 .013 10.546 Common Space Dimensions 5 -2.787 .960 8.432 .004 .062 .630 .635 .984 .321 1.877 Constant -2 Log Likelihood 20.039 Chi Square 57.308 df = 5 p = 0.001 Cox & Snell r2 0.641 Nagelkerke r2 0.856 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale137 Table 50 Logistic Regression Classification Results for Openness to Change Versus Conservation of the Schwartz Social Values Scale’s Individual Space as Generated by Classic Multidimensional Scaling Model for the United States Independent Variables B S.E. Wald Sig. Exp (B) United States Space Dimension 1 2.132 .696 9.374 .002 8.435 United States Space Dimension 2 -.869 .556 2.447 .118 .419 United States Space Dimension 3 -3.620 1.152 9.880 .002 .027 United States Space Dimension 4 .316 .523 .365 .546 1.372 United States Space Dimension 5 -1.447 .685 4.457 .035 .235 United States Space Dimension 6 -1.255 .772 2.646 .104 .285 .660 .575 1.317 .251 1.934 Constant -2 Log Likelihood 23.181 Chi Square 54.165 df = 6 p = 0.001 Cox & Snell r2 0.620 Nagelkerke r2 0.828 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale138 Table 51 Logistic Regression Classification Results for Openness to Change Versus Conservation of the Schwartz Social Values Scale in the Common Space Generated by Classic Multidimensional Scaling for Poland Independent Variables B S.E. Wald Sig. Exp(B) Polish Space Dimension 1 -.001 .258 .000 .998 .999 Polish Space Dimension 2 -1.987 .573 12.022 .001 .137 Polish Space Dimension 3 -.792 .455 3.036 .081 .453 Polish Space Dimension 4 -.038 .431 .008 .929 .962 Polish Space Dimension 5 .833 .518 2.580 .108 2.300 Polish Space Dimension 6 .934 .598 2.444 .118 2.545 Constant .217 .397 .299 .585 1.242 -2 Log Likelihood 43.084 Chi Square 34.263 df = 6 p = 0.001 Cox & Snell r2 0.458 Nagelkerke r2 0.611 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale139 Table 52 Logistic Regression Classification Results for Openness to Change Versus Conservation of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for the Common Space Generated by Classic Multidimensional Scaling for Romania Independent Variables B S.E. Wald Sig. Exp(B) Romanian Space Dimension 1 -.150 .221 .464 .496 .860 Romanian Space Dimension 2 .665 .304 4.807 .028 1.945 Romanian Space Dimension 3 .534 .309 2.989 .084 1.705 Romanian Space Dimension 4 -.191 .352 .296 .586 .826 Romanian Space Dimension 5 -.592 .387 2.336 .126 .553 Romanian Space Dimension 6 -.013 .368 .001 .972 .987 Constant .210 .300 .490 .484 1.234 -2 Log Likelihood 66.248 Chi Square 11.098 df = 6 p = 0.085 Cox & Snell r2 0.180 Nagelkerke r2 0.240 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale140 Table 53 Logistic Regression Classification Results for Self-Enhancement Versus SelfTranscendence of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for the Common Space Generated by Weighted Multidimensional Scaling Independent Variables B S.E. Wald Sig. Exp(B) Common Space Dimensions 1 -7.714 3.249 5.636 .018 .000 Common Space Dimensions 2 2.569 1.195 4.617 .032 13.048 Common Space Dimensions 3 -4.864 2.113 5.301 .021 .008 Common Space Dimensions 4 .309 .732 .178 .673 1.362 Common Space Dimensions 5 -1.924 .991 3.769 .052 .146 Constant 1.586 .934 2.881 .090 4.883 -2 Log Likelihood 17.386 Chi Square 57.655 df = 5 p < 0.001 Cox & Snell r2 0.643 Nagelkerke r2 0.871 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale141 Table 54 Logistic Regression Classification Results for Self-Enhancement Versus SelfTranscendence of the Schwartz Social Values Scale as Generated by Classic Multidimensional Scaling for the United States Independent Variables B S.E. Wald Sig. Exp(B) United States Space Dimension 1 2.332 .835 7.798 .005 10.294 United States Space Dimension 2 4.373 1.473 8.808 .003 79.282 United States Space Dimension 3 .300 .738 .165 .684 1.350 United States Space Dimension 4 1.686 .850 3.936 .047 5.397 United States Space Dimension 5 1.904 .998 3.644 .056 6.716 United States Space Dimension 6 .113 .791 .020 .887 1.119 Constant 2.001 .923 4.701 .030 7.398 -2 Log Likelihood 20.514 Chi Square 54.526 df = 6 p < 0.001 Cox & Snell r2 0.622 Nagelkerke r2 0.843 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale142 Table 55 Logistic Regression Classification Results for openness Self-Enhancement Versus SelfTranscendence of the Schwartz Social Values Scale (SVS) in common space as generated by Classic Multidimensional Scaling (CMDS) model’s Polish space Independent Variables B S.E. Wald Sig. Exp(B) Polish Space Dimension 1 4.003 1.384 8.369 .004 54.781 Polish Space Dimension 2 -1.115 .533 4.370 .037 .328 Polish Space Dimension 3 1.415 .756 3.504 .061 4.116 Polish Space Dimension 4 2.217 1.054 4.421 .035 9.181 Polish Space Dimension 5 .544 .744 .535 .465 1.723 Polish Space Dimension 6 -.581 .669 .755 .385 .559 Constant 1.365 .801 2.900 .089 3.914 -2 Log Likelihood 25.653 Chi Square 49.388 df = 6 p < 0.001 Cox & Snell r2 0.586 Nagelkerke r2 0.794 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale143 Table 56 Logistic regression classification results for openness to change versus Conservation of the Schwartz Social Values Scale (SVS) in common space as generated by Classic Multidimensional Scaling (CMDS) model’s Romanian space Independent Variables B S.E. Wald Sig. Exp(B) Romanian Space Dimension 1 -1.478 .509 8.434 .004 .228 Romanian Space Dimension 2 -.610 .442 1.904 .168 .543 Romanian Space Dimension 3 1.485 .535 7.689 .006 4.413 Romanian Space Dimension 4 .954 .467 4.179 .041 2.597 Romanian Space Dimension 5 .199 .435 .208 .648 1.220 Romanian Space Dimension 6 -.355 .512 .481 .488 .701 Constant .373 .377 .982 .322 1.452 -2 Log Likelihood 47.511 Chi Square 27.530 df = 6 p < 0.001 Cox & Snell r2 0.388 Nagelkerke r2 0.526 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale144 Table 57 A Cross tabulation of the Logistic Regression Results for the Classification of Values by Openness to Change and Conservation in Common space Empirically Expected Observed Total Openness to Change Conservation Openness to Change 24 2 26 Conservation 2 28 30 Total 26 30 56 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale145 Table 58 A Cross tabulation of the Logistic Regression Results for the Classification of Values by Openness to Change and Conservation in United States space Empirically Expected Observed Total Openness to Change Conservation Openness to Change 23 3 26 Conservation 2 28 30 Total 25 31 56 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale146 Table 59 A Cross Tabulation of the Logistic Regression Results for the Classification of Values by Openness to Change and Conservation in Polish space Empirically Expected Observed Total Openness to Change Conservation Openness to Change 19 7 26 Conservation 4 26 30 Total 23 33 56 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale147 Table 60 A Cross Tabulation of the Logistic Regression Results for the Classification of Values by Openness to Change and Conservation in Romanian Space Empirically Expected Observed Total Openness to Change Conservation Openness to Change 17 9 26 Conservation 8 22 30 Total 25 31 56 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale148 Table 61 A Cross Tabulation of the Logistic Regression Results for the Classification of Values by Self-Enhancement and Self-Transcendence in Common space Empirically Expected Observed Total Self-Enhancement Self-Transcendence Self-Enhancement 19 3 22 Self-Transcendence 2 32 34 Total 21 35 56 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale149 Table 62 A Cross tabulation of the Logistic Regression Results for the Classification of Values by Self-Enhancement and Self-Transcendence in United States Space Empirically Expected Observed Total Self-Enhancement Self-Transcendence Self-Enhancement 20 2 22 Self-Transcendence 2 32 34 Total 22 34 56 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale150 Table 63 Cross Tabulation of the Logistic Regression Results for the Classification of Values by Self-Enhancement and Self-Transcendence in Polish Space Empirically Expected Observed Total Self-Enhancement Self-Transcendence Self-Enhancement 18 4 22 Self-Transcendence 3 31 34 Total 21 35 56 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale151 Table 64 A Cross Tabulation of the Logistic Regression Results for the Classification of Values by Self-Enhancement and Self-Transcendence in Romanian Space Empirically Expected Observed Total Self-Enhancement Self-Transcendence Self-Enhancement 13 9 22 Self-Transcendence 5 29 34 Total 18 38 56 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale152 Table 65 Logistic Regression Models’ Significance and Percentage Agreement Beyond Chance for the Schwartz Social Values Scale when Classifying between Openness to Change and Conservation by Multidimensional Scaling Model Country Statistic Press’s Q Value - Percentage Cohen’s Kappa ( ) - Significance of Agreement Percent Agreement Discriminant (N = 56) Beyond Chance Model (N = 56) (N = 56) Common Space 3,844.6** 42.9% 85.6% United States 3,680.6** 41.1% 82.0% Poland 2,772.1** 33.9% 60.2% Romania 1,992.1** 30.4% 38.8% **p < 0.001 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale153 Table 66 Logistic Regression Models’ Significance and Percentage Agreement Beyond Chance for the Schwartz Social Values Scale When Classifying Between Self-Enhancement and SelfTranscendence by Multidimensional Scaling Model Country Statistic Press’s Q Value - Percentage Cohen’s Kappa ( ) - Significance of Agreement Percent Agreement Discriminant (N = 56) Beyond Chance Model (N = 56) (N = 56) Common Space 3,680.6** 33.9% 81.1% United States 3,844.6** 35.7% 85.0% Poland 3,363.5** 32.1% 73.6% Romania 2,366.0** 23.2% 45.9% **p < 0.001 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale154 Table 67 Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in Common Space Item Item Label Number Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Type Motivation Type 1 Equality Universalism Universalism 2 Inner Universalism Universalism 3 Social Power Power 4 Pleasure Hedonism Hedonism 5 Freedom Self-Direction Self-Directions 6 Spiritual Benevolence Benevolence 7 Sense Security Security 8 Social Security Security 9 Exciting Stimulation Hedonism 10 Meaning Benevolence Security 11 Politeness Conformity Conformity 12 Wealth Power Power 13 National Security Security 14 Self-Respect Self-Direction Self-Direction 15 Reciprocation Security Security 16 Creativity Self-Direction Stimulation 17 World Universalism Universalismo 18 Respect Tradition Conformitys 19 Mature Benevolence Benevolence 20 Self-Discipline Conformity Tradition 21 Privacy Self-Direction Self-Direction 22 Family Security Securitys 23 Social Power Powero Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale155 24 Unity With Nature Universalism Universalism Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale156 Table 67 (Continued) Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in Common Space Item Number Item Label Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Motivation Type Type 25 Varied Life Hedonism Hedonism 26 Wisdom Universalism Benevolence 27 Authority Power Power 28 True Love Benevolence Benevolence 29 World Universalism Universalism 30 Social Universalism Universalism 31 Independent Self-Direction Self-Direction 32 Moderate Tradition Tradition 33 Loyal Benevolence Benevolenceo 34 Ambitious Achievement Achievement 35 Broad-minded Universalism Universalism 36 Humble Tradition Tradition 37 Daring Stimulation Stimulation 38 Protecting Universalism Universalism 39 Influential Achievement Powero 40 Honoring Conformity Conformity 41 Choosing Self-Direction Self-Direction 42 Healthy Security Securitys 43 Capable Achievement Self-Directions 44 Accepting Tradition Tradition 45 Honest Benevolence Benevolence 46 Preserving Power Power 47 Obedient Conformity Conformity Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale157 48 Intelligent Achievement Achievement 49 Helpful Benevolence Benevolence Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale158 Table 67 (Continued) Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in Common Space Item Number Item Label Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Motivation Type Type 50 Enjoying Hedonism Hedonism 51 Devout Tradition Conformity 52 Responsible Benevolence Benevolence 53 Curious Self-Direction Self-Direction 54 Forgiving Benevolence Benevolence 55 Successful Achievement Achievement 56 Clean Security Achievement s Indicates that the item was inappropriately classified with regard to self-enhancement versus self-transcendence classification when Schwartz’s prediction was checked via logistic regression. o Indicates that the item was inappropriately classified with regard to openness to change versus Conservation classification when Schwartz’s prediction was checked via logistic regression. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale159 Table 68 Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in the United States’ Space Item Item Label Number Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Motivation Type Type 1 Equality Universalism Universalism 2 Inner Harmony Universalism Self-Direction 3 Social Power Power Power 4 Pleasure Hedonism Hedonism 5 Freedom Self-Direction Hedonismo 6 Spiritual Life Benevolence Benevolence 7 Sense of Belonging Security Security 8 Social Order Security Security 9 Exciting Life Stimulation Hedonism 10 Meaning in Life Benevolence Tradition 11 Politeness Conformity Conformity 12 Wealth Power Powero 13 National Security Security Security 14 Self-Respect Self-Direction Self-Direction 15 Reciprocation of Favors Security Security 16 Creativity Self-Direction Self-Direction 17 World at Peace Universalism Universalism 18 Respect for Tradition Tradition Security 19 Mature Love Benevolence Benevolence 20 Self-Discipline Conformity Tradition 21 Privacy Self-Direction Self-Direction 22 Family Security Security Securitys 23 Social Recognition Power Power Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale160 24 Unity with Nature Universalism Universalism 25 Varied Life Hedonism Stimulation Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale161 Table 68 (Continued) Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in the United States’ Space Item Item Label Number Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Motivation Type Type 26 Wisdom Universalism Universalism 27 Authority Power Power 28 True Friendship Benevolence Benevolence 29 World of Beauty Universalism Universalism 30 Social Justice Universalism Universalism 31 Independent Self-Direction Self-Directions 32 Moderate Tradition Stimulation 33 Loyal Benevolence Conformity 34 Ambitious Achievement Achievemento 35 Broad-minded Universalism Universalism 36 Humble Tradition Tradition 37 Daring Stimulation Stimulations 38 Protecting the Environment Universalism Universalism 39 Influential Achievement Securityo 40 Honoring of Parent Conformity Conformity 41 Choosing own Goals Self-Direction Achievements 42 Healthy Security Securityo 43 Capable Achievement Achievement 44 Accepting my Portion in Life Tradition Tradition 45 Honest Benevolence Conformity 46 Preserving my Public Image Power Power Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale162 Table 68 (Continued) Empirically Expected versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type for United States Discriminant Regression and Classic Multidimensional Scaling (CMDS) Model Item Item Label Number Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Motivation Type Type 47 Obedient Conformity Conformity 48 Intelligent Achievement Achievement 49 Helpful Benevolence Benevolence 50 Enjoying Life Hedonism Hedonism 51 Devout Tradition Tradition 52 Responsible Benevolence Conformity 53 Curious Self-Direction Universalism 54 Forgiving Benevolence Benevolence 55 Successful Achievement Achievement 56 Clean Security Achievement s Indicates that the item was inappropriately classified with regard to self-enhancement versus self-transcendence classification when Schwartz’s prediction was checked via logistic regression. o Indicates that the item was inappropriately classified with regard to openness to change versus Conservation classification when Schwartz’s prediction was checked via logistic regression. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale163 Table 69 Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in Polish Space Item Item Label Number Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Motivation Type Type 1 Equality Universalism Benevolence 2 Inner Harmony Universalism Benevolenceo 3 Social Power Power Power 4 Pleasure Hedonism Hedonism 5 Freedom Self-Direction Self-Direction 6 Spiritual Life Benevolence Benevolence 7 Sense of Belonging Security Security 8 Social Order Security Securitys 9 Exciting Life Stimulation Stimulation 10 Meaning in Life Benevolence Benevolence 11 Politeness Conformity Conformity 12 Wealth Power Power 13 National Security Security Security 14 Self-Respect Self-Direction Self-Directionso 15 Reciprocation of Favors Security Security 16 Creativity Self-Direction Self-Directions 17 World at Peace Universalism Securityo 18 Respect for Tradition Tradition Security 19 Mature Love Benevolence Universalismo 20 Self-Discipline Conformity Conformity 21 Privacy Self-Direction Securityso 22 Family Security Security Self-Directions Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale164 23 Social Recognition Power Security 24 Unity with Nature Universalism Universalism Table 69 (Continued) Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in Polish Space Item Item Label Number Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Type Motivation Type 25 Varied Life Hedonism Hedonism 26 Wisdom Universalism Self-Direction 27 Authority Power Power 28 True Friendship Benevolence Universalismo 29 World of Beauty Universalism Universalism 30 Social Justice Universalism Universalismo 31 Independent Self-Direction Self-Direction 32 Moderate Tradition Tradition 33 Loyal Benevolence Universalismo 34 Ambitious Achievement Achievement 35 Broad-minded Universalism Universalism 36 Humble Tradition Conformity 37 Daring Stimulation Stimulation 38 Protecting the Environment Universalism Universalismo 39 Influential Achievement Power 40 Honoring of Parent Conformity Conformity 41 Choosing own Goals Self-Direction Universalism 42 Healthy Security Securitys 43 Capable Achievement Achievement 44 Accepting my Portion in Tradition Tradition Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale165 Life 45 Honest Benevolence Benevolence 46 Preserving my Public Image Power Power Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale166 Table 69 (Continued) Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in Polish Space Item Item Label Number Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Type Motivation Type 47 Obedient Conformity Tradition 48 Intelligent Achievement Self-Directions 49 Helpful Benevolence Benevolenceo 50 Enjoying Life Hedonism Hedonism 51 Devout Tradition Tradition 52 Responsible Benevolence Self-Direction 53 Curious Self-Direction Self-Direction 54 Forgiving Benevolence Tradition 55 Successful Achievement Powero 56 Clean Security Security s Indicates that the item was inappropriately classified with regard to self-enhancement versus self-transcendence classification when Schwartz’s prediction was checked via logistic regression. o Indicates that the item was inappropriately classified with regard to openness to change versus Conservation classification when Schwartz’s prediction was checked via logistic regression. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale167 Table 70 Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in Romanian Space Item Item Label Number Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Type Motivation Type 1 Equality Universalism Securityo 2 Inner Harmony Universalism Benevolenceo 3 Social Power Power Power 4 Pleasure Hedonism Hedonism 5 Freedom Self-Direction Self-Directions 6 Spiritual Life Benevolence Conformity 7 Sense of Belonging Security Security 8 Social Order Security Security 9 Exciting Life Stimulation Hedonism 10 Meaning in Life Benevolence Securitys 11 Politeness Conformity Security 12 Wealth Power Powero 13 National Security Security Securitys 14 Self-Respect Self-Direction Security 15 Reciprocation of Favors Security Securitys 16 Creativity Self-Direction Self-Direction 17 World at Peace Universalism Traditiono 18 Respect for Tradition Tradition Conformity 19 Mature Love Benevolence Conformity 20 Self-Discipline Conformity Conformity 21 Privacy Self-Direction Self-Direction 22 Family Security Security Self-Directionso Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale168 23 Social Recognition Power Hedonismo 24 Unity with Nature Universalism Universalismo Table 70 (Continued) Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in Romanian Space Item Item Label Number Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Motivation Type Type 25 Varied Life Hedonism Hedonism 26 Wisdom Universalism Universalism 27 Authority Power Power 28 True Friendship Benevolence Hedonismso 29 World of Beauty Universalism Universalismo 30 Social Justice Universalism Universalism 31 Independent Self-Direction Self-Direction 32 Moderate Tradition Tradition 33 Loyal Benevolence Universalismo 34 Ambitious Achievement Self-Directions 35 Broad-minded Universalism Stimulations 36 Humble Tradition Tradition 37 Daring Stimulation Stimulations 38 Protecting the Universalism Universalismo Environment 39 Influential Achievement Powero 40 Honoring of Parent Conformity Self-Direction 41 Choosing own Goals Self-Direction Achievements 42 Healthy Security Self-Directionso Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale169 43 Capable Achievement Achievemento 44 Accepting my Portion in Tradition Tradition Benevolence Benevolence Life 45 Honest Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale170 Table 70 (Continued) Empirically Expected Versus Observed Classification of Primary Motivation Type and Bipolar Value Dimensions of the Schwartz Social Values Scale for Multiple Discriminant Functions and Logistic Regression Analysis in Romanian Space Item Item Label Number 46 Preserving my Public Empirically Expected Observed Motivation Motivation Type Type Power Achievement Image 47 Obedient Conformity Tradition 48 Intelligent Achievement Achievements 49 Helpful Benevolence Benevolence 50 Enjoying Life Hedonism Achievement 51 Devout Tradition Tradition 52 Responsible Benevolence Benevolenceo 53 Curious Self-Direction Benevolenceo 54 Forgiving Benevolence Benevolence 55 Successful Achievement Achievements 56 Clean Security Self-Directionso s Indicates that the item was inappropriately classified with regard to self-enhancement versus self-transcendence classification when Schwartz’s prediction was checked via logistic regression. o Indicates that the item was inappropriately classified with regard to openness to change versus Conservation classification when Schwartz’s prediction was checked via logistic regression. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale171 Table 71 Value Statements from Schwartz Social Values Scale that maintain the Same Primary Motivation Type between the United States, Poland, Romania, and Common Space Multidimensional Scaling solutions Item Number Item Label Empirically Expected Motivation Type 3 Social Power Power 4 Pleasure Hedonism 7 Sense of Belonging Security 8 Social Order Security 12 Wealth Power 13 National Security Security 15 Reciprocation of Favors Security 16 Creativity Self-Direction 24 Unity with Nature Universalism 27 Authority Power 29 World of Beauty Universalism 30 Social Justice Universalism 31 Independent Self-Direction 37 Daring Stimulation 38 Protecting the Environment Universalism 43 Capable Achievement* 44 Accepting my Portion in Life Tradition 49 Helpful Benevolence 51 Devout Tradition *Item 43 was classified as falling under the primary motivation of Self-Direction under the common space solution of the weighted multidimensional scaling (WMDS) model. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale172 Table 72 Raw Counts and Percentage Agreement of Value Statements Versus Primary Motivation Type for each Space Space Primary Schwartz's Model Motivation Type United States Poland Romania 5 5 4 3 100.0% 100.0% 80.0% 60.0% 3 4 2 3 60.0% 80.0% 40.0% 60.0% 3 2 3 2 100.0% 66.7% 100.0% 66.7% 1 1 2 1 50.0% 50.0% 100.0% 50.0% 6 4 5 4 85.7% 57.1% 71.4% 57.1% 8 8 5 5 88.9% 88.9% 55.6% 55.6% 8 5 4 4 88.9% 55.6% 44.4% 44.4% 3 3 3 4 60.0% 60.0% 60.0% 80.0% 3 3 3 1 75.0% 75.0% 75.0% 25.0% 6 6 6 4 85.7% 85.7% 85.7% 57.1% Common Space (WMDS) Power Achievement Hedonism Stimulation Self-Direction Universalism Benevolence Tradition Conformity Security 5 5 3 2 7 9 9 5 4 7 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale173 Table 73 Correlations of Lin’s Concordance between the 56 Value Statements of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Ratios via Kendall’s Tau-b by Country Country 1 1. United States 2 3 - 2. Poland .243* - 3. Romania .173* .381* * Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). - Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale174 Table 74 Correlations of Lin’s Concordance between the 56 Value Statements of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Ratios via Pearson’s r by Country Country 1. 1. United States 2. 3. - 2. Poland .399** - 3. Romanian .288** .579** ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). - Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale175 Table 75 Correlations of Distances Derived through Classic Multidimensional Scaling between the 56 Value Statements of the Schwartz Social Values Scale via Pearson’s r by Country Country 1 1. United States 2 3 - 2. Poland .194* - 3. Romania .189* .218* *Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) - Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale176 Table 76 Correlations of Distances Derived through Classic Multidimensional Scaling via Kendall’s Tau-b between the 56 Value Statements of the Schwartz Social Values Scale by Country Country 1 1. United States 2 3 - 2. Poland .114* - 3. Romania .121* .144* *Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) - Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale177 Appendix A The English version of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Item Item Number 1 Equality (equal opportunity for all) 2 Inner Harmony (at peace with myself) 3 Social Power (control over others, dominance) 4 Pleasure (gratification of desires) 5 Freedom (freedom of action and thought) 6 A Spiritual Life (emphasis on spiritual not material matters) 7 Sense of Belonging (feeling that others care about me) 8 Social Order (stability of society) 9 An Exciting Life (stimulating experiences) 10 Meaning in Life (a purpose in life) 11 Politeness (courtesy, good manners) 12 Wealth (material possessions, money) 13 National Security (protection of my nation from enemies) 14 Self-Respect (belief in one’s own worth) 15 Reciprocation of Favors (avoidance of indebtedness) 16 Creativity (uniqueness, imagination) 17 A World at Peace (free of war and conflict) 18 Respect for Tradition (preservation of time-honored customs) 19 Mature Love (deep emotional and spiritual intimacy) 20 Self-Discipline (self-restraint, resistance to temptation) 21 Privacy (the right to have a private sphere) 22 Family Security (safety for loved ones) 23 Social Recognition (respect, approval by others) 24 Unity with Nature (fitting into nature) 25 A Varied Life (filled with challenge, novelty, and change) 26 Wisdom (a mature understanding of life) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale178 Appendix A (Continued) The English version of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Item Item Number 27 Authority (the right to lead or command) 28 True Friendship (close, supportive friends) 29 A World of Beauty (beauty of nature and the arts) 30 Social Justice (correcting injustice, care for the weak) 31 Independent (self-reliant, self-sufficient) 32 Moderate (avoiding extremes of feeling and action) 33 Loyal (faithful to my friends, group) 34 Ambitious (hardworking, aspiring) 35 Broad-minded (tolerant of different ideas and beliefs) 36 Humble (modest, self-effacing) 37 Daring (seeking adventure, risk) 38 Protecting the Environment (preserving nature) 39 Influential (having an impact on people and events) 40 Honoring of Parent and Elders (showing respect) 41 Choosing Own Goals (selecting own purposes) 42 Healthy (not being sick physically or mentally) 43 Capable (competent, effective, efficient) 44 Accepting my Portion in Life (submitting to life’s circumstances) 45 Honest (genuine, sincere) 46 Preserving my Public Image (protecting my “face”) 47 Obedient (dutiful, meeting obligations) 48 Intelligent (logical, thinking) 49 Helpful (working for the welfare of others) 50 Enjoying Life (enjoying food, sex, leisure, etc.) 51 Devout (holding to religious faith and belief) 52 Responsible (dependable, reliable) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale179 Appendix A (Continued) The English version of the Schwartz Social Values Scale Item Item Number 53 Curious (interested in everything, exploring) 54 Forgiving (willing to pardon others) 55 Successful (achieving goals) 56 Clean (neat, tidy) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale180 Appendix B The Full United States Survey SECTION I: INTERNET USAGE 1 O O O O O 2 O O O O O O 3 O O O O O 4 O O O O O O 5 O O O O O O O O O About how long have you been using the Internet? Less than 3 months 4-12 months 1-3 years 4-6 years 7 years or more On average, how many hours per week, if any, do you use the Internet? 0 1-5 6 - 10 11 – 15 16 - 20 21 - or more About what percentage of your friends, relatives, and acquaintances would you guess use the Internet at least once a week? None 1 – 25% 26 – 50% 51 – 75% 76 – 100% How often, if ever, do you go online to shop (look for information about products or make a purchase? Never Less than once a month 1-2 times a month 3-5 times a month 6-9 times a month 10 or more times a month As far as you know, how many years has online shopping been available to people in the United States? 1 year 2 years 3 years 4 years 5 years 6 years 7 years 8 years 9 years or more Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale181 6 O O O O O O O O O O O O 7 O O O O O O O O O O 8 O O O O O 9 What was the first year that people around you could find products of interest to them for sale through the Internet? Before 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 or later About how long ago did your friends, family, or neighbors learn that they could shop for products through the Internet? 9 years ago or more 8 years ago 7 years ago 6 years ago 5 years ago 4 years ago 3 years ago 2 years ago 1 year ago This current year About what percentage of your friends, relatives, and acquaintances shop online? None 1 – 25% 26 – 50% 51 – 75% 76 – 100% Compared to other ways of shopping, how unusual or novel do you personally find online shopping to be? Use a scale of 1-7, where 1 means not at all novel or unusual and 7 means very novel or unusual. Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale182 Not at all Novel or Unusual 1 O Very Novel or Unusual 2 O 3 O 4 O 5 O O 6 O 7 10 In general, how different is shopping online compared to shopping in traditional stores? Use a scale of 1-7, where 1 means not at all different and 7 means very different. Not at all Different 1 O 11 3 O 4 O 5 O O 6 2 O 3 O 4 O 5 O O 6 Very Unique 7 O In general, how innovative is shopping online compared to shopping at a traditional store? Use a scale of 1-7, where 1 means not at all innovative and 7 means very innovative. Not at all Innovative 1 O 13 O In general, how unique is shopping online compared to shopping at a traditional store? Use a scale of 1-7, where 1 means not at all unique and 7 means very unique. Not at all Unique 1 O 12 2 Very Different 7 O 2 O 3 O 4 O 5 O 6 O Very Innovative 7 O Please indicate how much you agree or disagree with the following statements about your reactions to online shopping for those particular products/services of interest to you personally. Please indicate one answer for each statement, and react to all of the statements. a In general, I am among the last in my circle of friends to visit a shopping website when it appears. b If I heard that a new website was available for online shopping, I would be interested enough to visit it. c Compared to my friends, I have visited few online shopping websites. d I will visit an online shopping website even if I know practically nothing about it. e I know the names of new online shopping sites before other people do. f In general, I am the last in my circle of friends to know about new websites. Strongly Disagree Disagree Neither Agree nor Disagree Agree Strongly Agree O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale183 14 On average, how often do you shop (searching for product or service information, or making a purchase) on the Internet? O Never [IF NEVER, CLICK THE BUTTON AND THEN CLICK HERE TO SKIP TO QUESTION #19] O Rarely O Less than once a month O About once a month O About once a week O Daily 15 Please indicate how much you agree or disagree with the following statements about your reactions to online shopping for those particular products/services of interest to you personally. Please indicate one answer for each statement, and react to all of the statements. a My opinion on online shopping seems not to count with other people. b When I consider online shopping, I ask other people for advice. c People that I know pick shopping sites based on what I have told them. d I don't need to talk to others before I do online shopping. e When they choose to do online shopping, other people do not turn to me for advice. f I rarely ask other people what online websites to shop. * g I often persuade people to try the online websites that I look at. h I like to get other's opinions before I shop at an online site. i I often influence people’s opinions about online shopping. j I feel more comfortable shopping at an online website after I have gotten other people’s opinions on it. k Other people rarely come to me for advice about using an online shopping site. l When choosing an online shopping site, other people's opinions are not important to me. Strongly Disagree Disagree Neither Agree nor Disagree Agree Strongly Agree O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale184 16 a b c d e f g h i j k How often, if at all, do you VISIT each type of web site (WITHOUT purchasing) in order to help you to make a purchase decision? Use any number from 1 (never) to 5 (regularly). [INDICATE ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] Clothing / Accessories. Books / Magazines. Travel. Health & Medical. Financial Services. Consumer electronics (TV, VCR, stereo, cellular phone) Entertainment (compact disks, videos, concert tickets) Computer hardware or software. Food / Beverage / Groceries. Home Appliances (refrigerator, dishwasher). Other. 17 2O 2O 2O 2O 2O Sometimes 3O 3O 3O 3O 3O j Home Appliances (refrigerator, dishwasher). k Other. 4O 4O 4O 4O 4O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 1O 1O 1O 1O 2O 2O 2O 2O 3O 3O 3O 3O 4O 4O 4O 4O 5O 5O 5O 5O 5O Never 1O 1O 1O 1O 1O 2O 2O 2O 2O 2O Sometimes 3O 3O 3O 3O 3O 4O 4O 4O 4O 4O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 1O 1O 1O 1O 2O 2O 2O 2O 3O 3O 3O 3O 4O 4O 4O 4O 5O 5O 5O 5O Regularly 5O 5O 5O 5O 5O How much would the following encourage you to shop (visit or purchase) at a particular website? Use any number from 1 (strongly discourages me) to 7 (strongly encourages me). [INDICATE ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] 1 = Strongly Discourages Me a b Regularly 5O 5O 5O 5O How often, if at all, do you PURCHASE any of the following items/services (and not just look for information) online? Use any number from 1 (never) to 5 (regularly) . [INDICATE ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] a Clothing / Accessories. b Books / Magazines. c Travel. d Health & Medical. e Financial Services. f Consumer electronics (TV, VCR, stereo, cellular phone). g Entertainment (compact disks, videos, concert tickets). h Computer hardware or software. i Food / Beverage / Groceries. ) 18 Never 1O 1O 1O 1O 1O The order process is easy to use. The products I am looking for are easy to find 1O 1O 2O 2O 4 = Neither Encourages Nor Discourages Me 3O 4O 5O 3O 4O 5O 7 = Strongly Encourages Me 6O 6O 7O 7O Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale185 c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w The website is new and different Product price. Provides customer feedback (that is, the site provides a place for you to learn about other customer’s evaluation of the product)) My friends and family have been happy when they have shopped there Reputation and credibility of the company on the web. It is enjoyable to visit. The delivery time is short. The site is in my primary language. My friends and family will like to know my opinions of the site. A wide selection and variety of products. Low or no charge for shipping and handling. It has entertaining graphics and displays. Provides product information, including FAQs – frequently asked questions. A good place to find a bargain. Providing credit card safety. Fast response time from customer service. I hear about it on the radio, television or in newspapers . The download speed of the page. A return policy that is easy to understand and us. Price incentives (coupons, future sale items, frequent shopper program, etc.). Interactive web design (try it on, design your product / services). 1O 1O 2O 2O 3O 3O 4O 4O 5O 5O 6O 6O 7O 7O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O 1O 1O 1O 2O 2O 2O 3O 3O 3O 4O 4O 4O 5O 5O 5O 6O 6O 6O 7O 7O 7O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O 1O 1O 1O 2O 2O 2O 3O 3O 3O 4O 4O 4O 5O 5O 5O 6O 6O 6O 7O 7O 7O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O 1O 1O 1O 2O 2O 2O 3O 3O 3O 4O 4O 4O 5O 5O 5O 6O 6O 6O 7O 7O 7O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O 1O 1O 2O 2O 3O 3O 4O 4O 5O 5O 6O 6O 7O 7O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O *YOU ARE HALF WAY THROUGH THE SURVEY, THANK YOU FOR YOUR PATIENCE. SECTION II: OPINIONS AND BELIEFS 19 a. b. c. d. e. f. g. h. i. Please indicate whether the following statements are true or false of you. True I sometimes litter. T O Before voting I thoroughly investigate the qualifications of all the T O candidates. I always admit my mistakes openly and face the potential T O negative consequences. I never hesitate to go out of my way to help someone in trouble. T O In traffic I am always polite and considerate of others. T O It is hard for me to go on with my work if I am not encouraged. T O I always accept others’ opinions, even when they don’t agree T O with my own. I have never intensely disliked anyone. T O I take out my bad moods on others now and then. T O False F O F O F O O F O F O F F O O F O F Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale186 j. k. l. m. n. o. p. q. r. s. t. u. v. w. x. y. z. aa. bb. cc. dd. ee. ff. gg. hh. ii. jj. kk. ll. mm. nn. oo. pp. qq. rr. ss. tt. uu. On occasion I have doubts about my ability to succeed in life. There has been an occasion when I took advantage of someone else. I sometimes feel resentful when I don’t get my way. In conversations I always listen attentively and let others finish their sentences. I am always careful about my manner or dress. I never hesitate to help someone in case of emergency. My table manners at home are as good as when I eat out in a restaurant. If I could get into a movie without paying and be sure that I was not seen I would probably do it. When I have made a promise, I keep it – no ifs, ands, or buts. On a few occasions, I have given up doing something because I thought too little of my ability. I occasionally speak badly of others behind their backs. I like to gossip at times. I would never live off/at other people’s expense. There have been times when I felt like rebelling against people in authority even though I knew they were right. I always stay friendly and courteous with other people, even when I am stressed out. No matter who I am talking to, I’m always a good listener. During arguments I always stay objective and matter-of-fact. I can remember “playing sick” to get out of something. There has been at least one occasion when I failed to return an item that I borrowed. There have been occasions when I took advantage of someone. I always eat a healthy diet. I’m always willing to admit when I make a mistake. Sometimes I only help because I expect something in return. I always try to practice what I preach. I don’t find it particularly difficult to get along with loud-mouthed obnoxious people. I sometimes try to get even rather than to forgive and forget. When I don’t know something I don’t at all mind admitting it. I am courteous even to people who are disagreeable. At times I have really insisted on having things my own way. There have been occasions when I felt like smashing things. I would never think of letting someone else be punished for my wrongdoings. I never resent being asked to return a favor. I have never been irked when people expressed ideas very different from my own. I never make a long trip without checking the safety of my car. There have been times when I have been quite jealous of the good fortune of others. I have almost never felt the urge to tell someone off. I am sometimes irritated by people who ask favors of me. I have never felt that I was punished without cause. T O F O T O F O T O F O T O F O T O O F O O T O F O T O F O T O F O T O F O T O T O T O T F O F O F O F T O F O T O F O O T O T O T T O O T O T O T O T O T T O O T O T O T O T O T O F O F O F F O O F O F O F O F O F F O O F O F O F O F O F T O F O T O F O T O F O T O F O T O F O O O T O O O F O T F T F Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale187 vv. ww. I sometimes think that when people have a misfortune they only got what they deserved. I have never deliberately said something that hurt someone’s feelings. T O F O T O F O SECTION III: VALUES 20 The following items deal with what values YOU THINK are important. Please rate each value as a guiding principle IN YOUR LIFE, using a scale from –1 to 7, where –1 means “opposed to my values” and 7 means “of supreme importance”. Please indicate one number for each value concept. -1 = opposed to my values a Equality (equal opportunity for all) b Inner Harmony (at peace with myself) c Social Power (control over others, dominance) d Pleasure (gratification of desires) e Freedom (freedom of action and thought) f A Spiritual Life (emphasis on spiritual not material 7 = of supreme importance -1 O 0O 1 O 2O 3O 4 O 5O 6 O 7O -1 O 0O 1 O 2O 3O 4 O 5O 6 O 7O -1 O 0O 1 O 2O 3O 4 O 5O 6 O 7O -1 O 0O 1 O 2O 3O 4 O 5O 6 O 7O -1O 0O 1 O 2O 3O 4 O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1 O 2O 3O 4 O 5O 6O 7O Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale188 matters) g Sense of Belonging (feeling that others care about me) h Social Order (stability of society) i An Exciting Life (stimulating experiences) j Meaning in Life (a purpose in life) k Politeness (courtesy, good manners) l Wealth (material possessions, money) m National Security (protection of my nation from enemies) n Self-Respect (belief in one’s own worth) o Reciprocation of Favors (avoidance of indebtedness) p Creativity (uniqueness, imagination) q A World at Peace (free of war and conflict) r Respect for Tradition (preservation of timehonored customs) s Mature Love (deep emotional and spiritual intimacy) t Self-Discipline (selfrestraint, resistance to temptation) u Privacy (the right to have a private sphere) -1O 0O 1 O 2O 3O 4 O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1 O 2O 3O 4 O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1 O 2O 3O 4 O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O 7 = of supreme importance -1 = opposed to my values v Family Security (safety for loved ones) w Social Recognition (respect, approval by others) x Unity with Nature (fitting into nature) y A Varied Life (filled with challenge, novelty, and -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale189 change) z Wisdom (a mature understanding of life aa Authority (the right to lead or command) bb True Friendship (close, supportive friends) cc A World of Beauty (beauty of nature and the arts) dd Social Justice (correcting injustice, care for the weak) ee Independent (self-reliant, self-sufficient) ff Moderate (avoiding extremes of feeling and action) gg Loyal (faithful to my friends, group) hh Ambitious (hardworking, aspiring) ii Broad-minded (tolerant of different ideas and beliefs) jj Humble (modest, selfeffacing) kk Daring (seeking adventure, risk) ll Protecting the Environment (preserving nature) mm Influential (having an impact on people and events) nn Honoring of Parent and Elders (showing respect) oo Choosing Own Goals (selecting own purpose) pp Healthy (not being sick physically or mentally) qq Capable (competent, effective, efficient) rr Accepting my Portion in Life (submitting to life’s circumstances) ss Honest (genuine, sincere) tt Preserving my Public Image (protecting my “face”) uu Obedient (dutiful, meeting obligations) -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale190 7 = of supreme importance -1 = opposed to my values vv Intelligent (logical, thinking) ww Helpful (working for the welfare of others) xx Enjoying Life (enjoying food, sex, leisure, etc.) yy Devout (holding to religious faith and belief) zz . Responsible (dependable, reliable) aaa Curious (interested in everything, exploring) bbb Forgiving (willing to pardon others) ccc Successful (achieving goals) ddd Clean (neat, tidy) eee Self-Indulgent (doing pleasant things) -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O -1O 0O 1O 2O 3O 4O 5O 6O 7O *JUST A FEW MORE QUESTIONS, YOU ARE ALMOST FINISHED. SECTION IV: BACKGROUND INFORMATION 21 O O 22 What is your gender? Male Female How old are you (in years)? _________________ O O O O 23 What is your marital status? Single, never been married Married Separated/Divorced Widowed 24 In what state is your permanent address at this current time? 25 Were your grandparents born in the U.S.A.? Yes, all four of them Yes, 1, 2, or 3 of them O O Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale191 O O 26 O O O O O 27 O O O 28 O O O O O O O O O O O O 29 O O O O O O 30 None of them Don’t know Were your parents born in the U.S.A.? Neither My mother My father Both Don’t know Were you born in the U.S.A.? Yes No Don’t know Would you describe your family as mainly: African American Taiwanese American Chinese (mainland) American Iranian American Palestinian American German/Austrian American Hispanic American Polish American Greek American Romanian American Canadian Other (please specify) ____________________ What was the last year of education you completed? Some high school High school Technical School/Training (such as auto mechanic) Some college/university College/university graduate Graduate or professional school What is your current employment? [CHECK ALL THAT APPLY] Employed-full time [GO TO Q36] Employed-part time [GO TO Q36] Self employed [GO TO Q36] (self) Temporarily unemployed [GO TO Q37] Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale192 Student [GO TO Q37] Homemaker/housewife [GO TO Q37] Retired [GO TO Q37] 31 O O O O O O O 32 O O O O O O O O (IF EMPLOYED) What is your occupation? Professional Managerial/Executive Sales Clerical Labor with technical training Labor without technical training Other (please specify) _____________________ Please indicate which of the following categories best represents your annual household income before taxes. (income) $10,000 or less $10,001 to $20,000 $20,001 to $30,000 $30,001 to $40,000 $40,001 to $50,000 $50,001 to $75,000 $75,001 to $100,000 more than $100,000 33 How many people live in your household, including yourself (please enter the number)? __________________ 34 Please indicate whether you own each of the following items. [INDICATE ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH] Yes No Don’t Know a A personal computer O O O b A DVD player O O O c A high-definition TV (HDTV) O O O A Personal Digital Assistant O O O d (PDA) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale193 Appendix C The Full Polish Survey in its Original English INTERNET USAGE 1. Is there a personal computer in your home? [CHECK ONE] 1 Yes 2 No 2. About what percentage of your friends, relatives, and acquaintances would you guess use the Internet at least once a week? [CHECK ONE] 1 None 2 1 - 25% 3 26 - 50% 4 51 - 75% 5 76 - 100% 3. About how many, if any, of your friends, relatives, and acquaintances shop online? [CHECK ONE] 1 None 2 1 - 25% 3 26 - 50% 4 51 - 75% 5 76 - 100% 4. About how long ago did your friends, family, or neighbors, learn that they could purchase products on the Internet? [CHECK YOUR ONE BEST GUESS] 1 Over 10 years ago 2 10 years ago 3 9 years ago 4 8 years ago 5 7 years ago 6 6 years ago 7 5 years ago 8 4 years ago 9 3 years ago 10 2 years ago 11 1 year ago 12 This current year 5. What was the first year that people living in your hometown could find products of interest to them for sale on the Internet? [CHECK ONE] Before 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 1990 2000 or later 6. AS FAR AS YOU KNOW, for about how many years has online shopping been available to people in Poland? [CHECK ONE] 1 year 2 years 3 years 4 years 5 years 6 years 7 years 8 years 9 years 9 years Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale194 or more 7. Have you ever used the World-Wide Web (WWW) before? [CHECK ONE] 1 Yes [GO TO Q8] 2 No [GO TO Q27] 8. Where do you use the Internet most often? [CHECK ONE] 1 Home 2 School 3 Work 4 Other 9. On average, how many hours per week, if any, do you use the Internet? [CHECK ONE] 1 0 2 1 - 5 3 6 - 10 4 11 - 15 5 16 - 20 6 21 - or more 10. About how long have you been using the Internet? [CHECK ONE] 1 Less than 3 months 2 4 - 12 months 3 1 - 3 years 4 4 - 6 years 5 7 - 9 years 6 10 years or more 11. On average, how often do you do each of the following on the Internet? [CHECK ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] Rarely or Never a. Shopping (that is, searching for product or service information, or making a purchase) b. Entertainment (game playing, etc.) c. Communication with others (E-mail, Voice-mail, BBS, ICQ, Instant Messenger, etc.) d. Education and newspaper reading (e-books, emagazines, etc.) Less Than Once a Month About Once A Month About Once A Week Daily 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale195 DISTANCE SHOPPING INFORMATION Distance shopping could be defined as any form of shopping that is not practiced through the traditional store. Distance shopping could be considered shopping through magazines, catalogs, telemarketing, online etc. 12. How often, if ever do you do any distance shopping through methods other than the Internet? For the following items, 1 means rarely or never and 5 means daily. [CHECK ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] Rarely or Never a. Through catalogs, brochures, or magazines. b. Through television or infomercials. c. Through phone orders exclusively (examples: supermarkets or pizza delivery). 1 1 1 Less Than Once a Month 2 2 2 About Once a Month 3 3 3 SHOPPING ONLINE “Shopping online,” means using the Internet to look for information about products, services, manufacturers, or companies and/or making purchases of products, services, etc. 13. Given what you get from Internet and traditional stores, what do you think the prices of the Internet stores ought to be? [CHECK ONE] 1 The price ought to be a lot higher than those of the traditional store. 2 The price ought to be a little bit higher than those of the traditional store. 3 The prices ought to be the same with those of the traditional store. 4 The prices ought to be a bit lower than those of the traditional store. 5 The prices ought be a lot lower than those of the traditional store About Once a Week 4 4 4 Daily 5 5 5 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale196 14. How much would the following encourage you to shop (visit or purchase) at a particular website? For the following items, 1 means strongly discourages me and 7 means strongly encourages me. [CHECK ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] 1 = Strongly Discourages Me a. The order process is easy to use b. Easy to find the product I am looking for c. The website is new and different d. Product price e. Provides customer feedback (that is, the site provides a place for you to learn about other customer’s evaluations of the product). f. My friends and family have been happy when they have shopped there g. Reputation and credibility of the company on the web h. It is enjoyable to visit i. The delivery time is short j. The site is enjoyable to visit k. My friends and family will like to know my opinions of the site l. A wide selection and variety of products m. Low or no shipping charges n. It has entertaining graphics and displays o. Provides product information, including FAQ – frequently asked questions p. A good place to find a bargain q. Provides credit card safety r. Fast response time from customer service s. I hear about it on the radio, television, or in newspapers t. The download speed of the pages u. The return policy is easy to use v. Offers good price incentives (coupons, featured sale items, frequent shopper program, etc.) w. Interactive website design (try it on, design your product/services) 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 4 = Neither Encourages Nor Discourages Me 3 4 3 4 3 4 3 4 3 4 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 1 1 1 2 2 2 3 3 3 4 4 4 5 5 5 6 6 6 7 7 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 15. How often, if ever, do you go online to shop (look for information or make a purchase)? [CHECK ONE] 1 Never [GO TO Q25] 2 Less than once a month [GO TO Q16] 3 1-2 times/month [GO TO Q16] 7 = Strongly Encourages Me 5 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 7 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale197 4 5 6 3-5times/month [GO TO Q16] 6-9 times/month [GO TO Q16] 10 or more times/month [GO TO Q16] 16. How often, if at all, do you VISIT each type of web site (WITHOUT purchasing) in order to help you to make a purchase decision? [CHECK ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] Never a. Clothing / Accessories. b. Books / Magazines. c. Travel. d. Health & Medical. e. Financial Services. f. Consumer electronics (TV, VCR, stereo, cellular phone) g. Entertainment (compact disks, videos, concert tickets). h. Computer hardware or software. i. Food / Beverage / Groceries. j. Home Appliances (refrigerator, dishwasher). k. Other. Seldom Sometimes Often 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 1 2 2 2 3 3 3 4 4 4 5 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 17. All in all, how similar is shopping online to going to a traditional store? Using a scale of 1-7, where 1 means very different and 7 means very similar. [CHECK ONE] Very Different 1 Very Similar 2 3 4 5 6 7 18. Compared to other ways of shopping, how unusual or novel do you personally find online shopping to be? Using a scale of 1-7, where 1 means not at all novel or unusual and 7 means very novel or unusual. [CHECK ONE] Not at All Novel or Unusual 1 Very Novel or Unusual 2 3 4 5 6 7 19. All in all, how unique is shopping online to going to a traditional store? Using a scale of 1-7, where 1 means not at all unique and 7 means very unique. [CHECK ONE] Not at all Unique Very Often Very Unique Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale198 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 20. Compared to what you find in traditional stores, how often are you surprised by the ads, products, prices, displays, and other features of online shopping? [CHECK ONE] 1 I am surprised much more often online. 2 I am surprised somewhat more often online 3 I am surprised slightly more often online 4 I am surprised equally often online and in traditional stores 5 I am surprised slightly more often in traditional stores 6 I am surprised somewhat more often in traditional stores 7 I am surprised much more often in traditional stores 8 Don’t Know 21. All in all, how innovative is shopping online to going to a traditional store? Using a scale of 1-7, where 1 means not at all innovative and 7 means highly innovative. [CHECK ONE] Not at all Innovative 1 Highly Innovative 2 3 4 5 6 7 22. Do you find shopping through the Internet pleasant or unpleasant? Using a scale of 1-7, where 1 means unpleasant and 7 means pleasant. [CHECK ONE] Unpleasant 1 Pleasant 2 3 4 5 6 7 23. Overall, how satisfied or dissatisfied are you with shopping in the Internet? Using a scale of 17, where 1 means not very at all satisfied and 7 means very satisfied. [CHECK ONE] Very Dissatisfied 1 2 3 Neither Satisfied nor Dissatisfied 4 Very Satisfied 5 6 7 24. How often, if ever, do you PURCHASE (and not just look for information) online? [CHECK ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] Never a. Clothing / Accessories. b. Books / Magazines. c. Travel. 1 1 1 Seldom 2 2 2 Sometimes 3 3 3 Often 4 4 4 Very Often 5 5 5 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale199 d. Health & Medical. e. Financial Services. f. Consumer electronics (TV, VCR, stereo, cellular phone) g. Entertainment (compact disks, videos, concert tickets). h. Computer hardware or software. i. Food / Beverage / Groceries. j. Home Appliances (refrigerator, dishwasher). k. Other. 1 1 1 2 2 2 3 3 3 4 4 4 5 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 1 2 2 2 3 3 3 4 4 4 5 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 25. How much do you agree or disagree with the following statements about your reactions to online shopping for those particular products/services of interest to you personally? [CHECK ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] Strongly Disagree a. My opinion on online shopping seems not to count with other people. b. When I consider online shopping, I ask other people for advice. c. In general, I am among the last in my circle of friends to visit a shopping website when it appears. d. People that I know pick shopping sites based on what I have told them. e. I don’t need to talk to others before I do online shopping. f. If I heard that a new website was available for online shopping, I would be interested enough to visit it. g. When they choose to do online shopping, other people do not turn to me for advice. h. I rarely ask other people what online websites to shop. i. Compared to my friends, I have visited few online shopping websites. j. I often persuade people to try the online shopping websites that I look at. k. I like to get other’s opinions before I shop at an online site. l. I will visit an online shopping website even if I know practically nothing about it. m. I often influence people’s opinions about online shopping. n. I feel more comfortable shopping at an online website after I have gotten other people’s opinions on it. o. I know the names of new online shopping sites before other people do. p. Other people rarely come to me for advice about using an online shopping site. q. When choosing an online shopping site, other people’s opinions are not important to me. Disagree Neither Agree Nor Disagree Agree Strong Agree 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale200 r. Usually, my friends know the name of a new shopping website before I do. 1 2 3 4 5 ATTITUDES AND OPINIONS 26. How much do you agree or disagree about the following statements with regard to yourself? [CHECK ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] Strongly Disagree a. I am suspicious of new inventions and ways of thinking b. I am reluctant about adopting new ways of doing things until I see them working for people around me c. I rarely trust new ideas until I can see whether the vast majority of people around me accept them d. I am generally cautious about accepting new ideas e. I must see other people using new innovations before I will consider them f. I often find my self skeptical of new ideas g. I am aware that I am usually one of the last people in my group to accept something new h. I tend to feel that the traditional way of living and doing things is the best way i. I consider myself to be creative and original in my thinking and behavior j. I am an inventive kind of person k. I seek out new ways to do things l. I enjoy trying out new things m. I am challenged by ambiguities and unsolved problems n. I find it stimulating to be original in my thinking and behavior. o. I am receptive to new ideas p. I frequently improvise methods for solving a problem when an answer is not apparent q. I fell that I am an influential member of my peer group r. My peers often ask me for advice or information Disagree Neither Agree nor Disagree Agree Strongl Agree 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale201 s. I enjoy taking part in the leadership responsibilities of the groups I belong to t. I am challenged by unanswered questions 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 27. For the following statements check True for those that are truth about yourself and False for those that do not represent yourself. [CHECK ONE RESPONSE FOR EACH ITEM] True a. I sometimes litter. b. I always admit my mistakes openly and face the potential negative consequences. c. In traffic I am always polite and considerate of others. d. I always accept others’ opinions, even when they don’t agree with my own. e. I take out my bad moods on others now and then. f. There has been an occasion when I took advantage of someone else. g. In conversations I always listen attentively and let others finish their sentences. h. I never hesitate to help someone in case of emergency. i. When I have made a promise, I keep it – no ifs, ands, or buts. j. I occasionally speak badly of others behind their backs. k. I would never live off/at other people’s expense. l. I always stay friendly and courteous with other people, even when I am stressed out. m. During arguments I always stay objective and matter-of-fact. n. There has been at least one occasion when I failed to return an item that I borrowed o. I always eat a health diet. p. Sometimes I only help because I expect something in return q. Before voting I thoroughly investigate the qualifications of all the candidates. r. I never hesitate to go out of my way to help someone in trouble. s. It is sometimes hard for me to go on with my work if I am not encouraged. t. I have never intensely disliked anyone. u. On occasions I have had doubts about my ability to succeed in life. v. I sometimes feel resentful when I don’t get my way. w. I am always careful about my manner of dress. x. My table manners at home are as good as when I eat out in a restaurant. y. If I could get into a movie without paying and be sure I was not seen I would probably do it. z. On a few occasions, I have give up something because I thought too little of my ability. aa. I like to gossip at times. bb. There have been times when I felt like rebelling against people in authority even though I knew they were right. cc. No matter who I’m talking to, I’m always a good listener. dd. I can remember “playing sick” to get out of something. ee. There have been occasions when I have taken advantage of someone. ff. I’m always willing to admit it when I make a mistake. gg. I always try to practice what I preach. hh. I don’t find it particularly difficult to get along with loudmouthed, obnoxious people. ii. I sometimes try to get even rather than forgive and forget. jj. When I don’t know something I don’t mind at all admitting it. False 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale202 kk. I am always courteous, even to people who are disagreeable. ll. At times I have really insisted on having things my own way. mm. There have been occasions when I felt like smashing things. nn. I would never think of letting someone else be punished for my wrong-doings. oo. I never resent being asked to return a favor. pp. I have never been irked when people expressed ideas very different from my own. qq. I never make a long trip without checking the safety of my car. rr. There have been times when I was quite jealous of the good fortune of others. ss. I have almost never felt the urge to tell someone off. tt. I am sometimes irritated by people who ask favors of me. uu. I have never felt that I was punished without cause. vv. I sometimes think when people have a misfortune they only got what they deserved. ww. I have never deliberately said something that hurt someone’s feelings. 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 28. This section is composed of 57 items concerning what values YOU THINK are important. Please rate each value as a guiding principle IN YOUR LIFE, using a scale from 7 (of supreme importance) to 0 (not important) and -1 (opposed to my values). Please indicate one number for each value concept: -1 = Opposed to My Values a. Equality (equal opportunity -1 for all) b. Inner Harmony (at peace -1 with myself) c. Social Power (control over -1 others, dominance) d. Pleasure (gratification of -1 desires) e. Freedom (freedom of action -1 and thought) -1 f. A Spiritual Life (emphasis on spiritual not material matters) g. Sense of Belonging (feeling -1 that others care about me) h. Social Order (stability of -1 society) i. An Exciting Life (stimulating -1 experiences) j. Meaning in Life (a purpose -1 in life) k. Politeness (courtesy, good -1 manners) l. Wealth (material -1 possessions, money) m. National Security -1 (protection of my nation from enemies) n. Self-Respect (belief in -1 0 = Not Important 0 1 7 = Supremely Important 6 7 2 3 4 5 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale203 one’s own worth) o. Reciprocation of Favors (avoidance of indebtedness) p. Creativity (uniqueness, imagination) q. A World at Peace (free of war and conflict) r. Respect for Tradition (preservation of time-honored customs) s. Mature Love (deep emotional and spiritual intimacy) -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 = Opposed to My Values -1 t. Self-Discipline (self- 0 = Not Important 0 1 7 = Supremely Important 6 7 2 3 4 5 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 restraint, resistance to temptation) u. Privacy (the right to have a private sphere) v. Family Security (safety for loved ones) w. Social Recognition (respect, approval by others) x. Unity with Nature (fitting into nature) y. A Varied Life (filled with challenge, novelty, and change) z. Wisdom (a mature understanding of life) aa. Authority (the right to lead or command) bb. True Friendship (close, supportive friends) cc. A World of Beauty (beauty of nature and the arts) dd. Social Justice (correcting injustice, care for the weak) ee. Independent (self-reliant, self-sufficient) ff. Moderate (avoiding extremes of feeling and action) Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale204 gg. Loyal (faithful to my -1 friends, group) hh. Ambitious (hardworking, -1 aspiring) ii. Broad-minded (tolerant of -1 different ideas and beliefs) jj. Humble (modest, self-1 effacing) kk. Daring (seeking -1 adventure, risk) ll. Protecting the Environment -1 (preserving nature) mm. Influential (having an -1 impact on people and events) nn. Honoring of Parent and -1 Elders (showing respect) oo. Choosing Own Goals -1 (selecting own purposes) pp. Healthy (not being sick -1 physically or mentally) qq. Capable (competent, -1 effective, efficient) rr. Accepting my Portion in -1 Life (submitting to life’s circumstances) -1 = Opposed to My Values ss. Honest (genuine, sincere) -1 tt. Preserving my Public -1 Image (protecting my “face”) uu. Obedient (dutiful, meeting -1 obligations) vv. Intelligent (logical, -1 thinking) ww. Helpful (working for the -1 welfare of others) xx. Enjoying Life (enjoying -1 food, sex, leisure, etc.) yy. Devout (holding to -1 religious faith and belief) zz. Responsible (dependable, -1 reliable) aaa. Curious (interested in -1 everything, exploring) bbb. Forgiving (willing to -1 pardon others) ccc. Successful (achieving -1 goals) ddd. Clean (neat, tidy) -1 eee. Self-Indulgent (doing -1 pleasant things) 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 = Not Important 0 1 0 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 0 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 6 6 7 7 BACKGROUND INFORMATION 29. Is there a DVD player in your home? [CHECK ONE] 7 = Supremely Important 6 7 6 7 Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale205 1 2 Yes No 30. Is there a Digital TV in your home? [CHECK ONE] 1 Yes 2 No 31. Do you own a PDA (Personal Digital Assistant)? [CHECK ONE] 1 Yes 2 No 32. Do you have any major credit cards in your name (e.g., American Express, Diners Club, MasterCard)? [CHECK ONE] 1 Yes 2 No 33. Were you born within the Poland? [CHECK ONE] 1 Yes 2 No 34. Were your parents born in Poland [CHECK ONE] 1 Neither 2 My mother 3 My father 4 Both 35. Were your grandparents born in Poland? [CHECK ONE] 1 Yes, all four of them 2 Yes, 1, 2, or 3 of them 3 None of them 4 Don’t know 36. Where is your permanent address at this time? [CHECK ONE] 1 Warsaw 2 Within the Mazowieckie Vovoidship 3 Outside of the Mazowieckie Vovoidship 37. How many people live in your household, including yourself? ______ 38. How many of the people in your household are 17 years old or younger? ______ 39. How old are you? ______ 40. What is your gender? 1 Male 2 Female 41. What is your marital status? [CHECK ONE] 1 Single, never been married 2 Married Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale206 3 Separated, Divorced, or Widowed 42. What was the last year of education you completed? [CHECK ONE] 1 Some high school 2 High school 3 Technical School/Training (such as auto mechanic) 4 Some College/University 5 College/University Graduate 6 Graduate or professional school 43. What is your religious affiliation? [CHECK ONE] 1 Roman Catholic 2 Protestant 3 Russian Orthodox 4 Other Christian 5 Agnostic 6 Atheist 7 None 8 Other (please specify) __________________ 44. What is your current employment? [CHECK ALL THAT APPLY] 1 Employed – Full Time [GO TO Q45] 2 Employed – Part Time [GO TO Q45] 3 Temporarily Unemployed [GO TO Q45] 4 Self-Employed [GO TO Q45] 5 Student [GO TO Q46] 6 Homemaker/Housewife [GO TO Q46] 7 Retired [GO TO Q46] 45. [IF EMPLOYED] What is your occupation? [CHECK ONE] 1 Professional [CONTINUE TO Q46] 2 Managerial/Executive [CONTINUE TO Q46] 3 Sales [CONTINUE TO Q46] 4 Clerical [CONTINUE TO Q46] 5 Labor with Technical Training [CONTINUE TO Q46] 6 Labor without Technical Training [CONTINUE TO Q46] 46. Please indicate which of the following categories best represents your monthly household income before taxes? [CHECK ONE] 1 0 - 500 zlotny 2 500 - 1000 zl 3 1001 - 1500 zl 4 1501 - 1750 zl 5 1751 - 2000 zl 6 2001 - 2250 zl 7 2251 - 2500 zl 8 2501 - 2750 zl 9 2751 - 3000 zl 10 3001 - 3250 zl 11 3251 - 3500 zl 12 3501 - 4000 zl Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale207 13 14 4001 - 4500 zl 4501 or more per month Generalization Schwartz Social Values Scale208