Leadership: Perspectives in Theory and Research

advertisement
Leadership: Perspectives in Theory and Research
Author(s): Arthur G. Jago
Source: Management Science, Vol. 28, No. 3 (Mar., 1982), pp. 315-336
Published by: INFORMS
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2630884 .
Accessed: 06/01/2015 19:39
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
.
INFORMS is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Management Science.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MANAGEMENT SCIENCE
Vol. 28, No. 3, March 1982
Prinited in U.S.A.
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND
RESEARCH*
ARTHUR G. JAGOt
Prominent trends in leadership research are reviewed. Theoretical perspectives are organized
in a four-fold typology based on the dominating assumptions of the research effort: (1) the
focus on a universally appropriate set of leadership traits, (2) the focus on a universally
appropriate behavioral style, (3) the focus on situationally contingent leadership traits, and (4)
the focus on situationally contingent behavioral styles. Potential organizational prescriptions
following from each perspective are identified (e.g., selection, placement, training). It is argued
that existing research has mapped only a portion of the domain of leadership phenomena due
to a concentration on relatively few leadership constructs and because of the popularity of a
limited set of empirical methodologies. Recent developments, promising new directions, and
novel methods in leadership research are described.
(LEADERSHIP; ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR)
1. Introduction
Unlike "harder" sciences (e.g., physics, chemistry, biology) where well accepted "laws" may govern
phenomena, the soft science of behavior in organizations remains an imprecise, inexact exploration into the
causes and consequences of complex human interactions. Equivocal explanations of organizational events
abound; each competing explanation, of course, often having unique implications for the practicing (or
aspiring) manager trying desperately to apply social science knowledge toward solving the problems
encountered in his or her organizational role.
Leadership research is no exception to this rule. Although thousands of empirical investigations of leaders
have been conducted in the last 75 years, no clear and unequivocal understanding exists as to what
distinguishes leaders from nonleaders and, perhaps more importantly, what distinguishes effective leaders
from ineffective leaders. Multiple interpretations of leadership phenomena exist, each providing some insight
into the role of leader but each remaining an incomplete and wholly inadequate explanation of complex
relationships. Although behavioral scientists have granted few topical areas greater research attention, the
results of these efforts remain a bewildering melange for even the most serious student of organizations.
Sadly, the survey of leadership research to follow fails to resolve certain nagging contradictions produced
in these years of research, and falls far short of filling some conspicuous holes in our accumulated
knowledge. Nonetheless, this review does represent an attempt: (1) to organize and discuss a broad range of
theories influencing leadership research over the years, (2) to identify important similarities and differences
among these theories, and (3) to outline the major organizational implications stemming from each
theoretical perspective. Clearly, no attempt is made to provide a comprehensive review of all the empirical
evidence pertaining to each position. Indeed, such an in-depth critique of such evidence is beyond the scope
of the present article and is available elsewhere [35], [54], [108].
2.
Concepts and Definitions
Stogdill astutely notes that "there are almost as many definitions of leadership as there are persons who
have attempted to define the concept" [108, p. 7]. At the risk of being criticized for merely adding to this list,
the following definition is offered in the hope of incorporating important aspects of several of its
predecessors:
Leadership is both a process and a property. The process of leadership is the use of
noncoercive influence to direct and coordinate the activities of the members of an organized
group toward the accomplishment of group objectives. As a property, leadership is the set of
qualities or characteristics attributed to those who are perceived to successfully employ such
influence.
*Accepted by Arie Y. Lewin; received August 24, 1979. This paper has been with the author 3 months for
I revision.
tUniversity of Houston.
315
0025-1909/82/2803/0
315 $01.25
Copyright ?( 1982. The Institute of Malnagement Sciences
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
316
ARTHUR G. JAGO
It is important to recognize what this definition includes and what it excludes. Leadership is not only some
quality or characteristic that one possesses or is perceived to possess, it can be something that one does. It
therefore can describe an act as well as a person. Leadership does not involve the use of force, coercion or
domination and is not necessarily implied by the use of such titles as manager, supervisor, or superior. In
this respect, the definition provides a conceptual distinction between leadership processes and motivational
processes, the latter being the more appropriate domain for any discussion of the administrationi of
discretionary rewards and punishments made possible by some formal authority structure. Leadership is
therefore distinct from "supervision" or what might be termed "headship" [35], [58].
Leadership is expressed or displayed through interaction between people and necessarily implies its
complement, "followership." For one to influence, another must permit himself to be influenced. Moreover,
leader and follower(s) must be at least loosely organized arounldsome common or agreed uponl purpose or
mission, the achievement of which is perceived to depend in part on the leader-follower relationship. A
person who causes panic by shouting "Fire!" in a crowded theatre may indeed be influenicing many lives.
However, by our definition the individual would not be exhibiting leadership. On the other hand, a second
person who may calm the same crowd and direct the achievement of an orderly evacuation would be
exhibiting leadership.
Perhaps the most important feature of the above definition is that it does not restrict the role of leader to
one formally designated member of a group or, for that matter, to any single group member. Leadership is
an evolving, dynamic process. At times, leaders become followers and followers become leaders [50].
Moreover multiple leader roles may coexist in groups, each such role serving a different leadership purpose
or function [3]. Although most of the research on leadership to be reviewed has been confined only to the
study of managers and their structurally defined subordinates in work settings, our definition is far less
restrictive than these studies would seem to imply.
With this definition of leadership in mind, it now becomes possible to sort, classify and categorize
alternative theories of leadership. For this purpose two distinctions must be made:
Universal vs. Contingent Theor ies
Certain perspectives make the implicit assumption that what constitutes successful or effective leadership
does not depend on the characteristics of the situation in which the leader operates. Leadership is proposed
to be a general as opposed to specific phenomenon, that is, what constitutes effective leadership for the
corporation president is essentially the same as that for the shopfloor foreman, clergyman, or Cub Scout den
mother. Moreover, leadership is invariant within, as well as between, roles. Different circumstances
encountered by the leader are not necessarily seen as requiring different forms of leadership. Because they
propose that there exists a "one-best-way" to lead, such perspectives attempt to offer uiniversalprescriptions
for leadership.
On the other hand, alternative approaches propose that effective leadership depends on specific features
of the leader's situation (e.g., characterisitcs of the task, characteristics of followers). These approaches
propose certain situational variables that, when assessed, provide a situational diagnosis on which leadership
prescriptions
are based. These theories therefore
provide contingent
prescriptions
for leadership;
that is,
prescriptions contingent on certain situational factors.
Traits vs. Behaviors
Secondly, perspectives differ in the way the leadership construct is conceptualized. It is possible to view
leadership primarily in terms of relatively stable and enduring characteristics of people. Leadership can be
viewed as a t/ait (or set of traits) distributed in some way among the population. In this sense, leadership is
viewed as a measurable and quantifiable property possessed in different amounts by different people.
Alternatively, it is possible to focus on observable leader behlaviorsrather than on inherent traits. From such
a perspective, leadership exists primarily in the actions of the leader. Leadership is expressed in terms of
overt behavior patterns rather than in terms of some intrinsic property or characteristic.
TABLE 1
A Typologyof LeadershipPerspectives
Theoretical Approach
Focal
Leadership
Construct
Universal
Contingent
Leader
Traits
Type
I
Type
III
Leader
Behaviors
Type
II
Type
IV
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
317
Table I combines these concepts to form a matrix of four ways in which to view leadership. From a Type
I perspective, for example, leadership is viewed as a trait or characteristic likely to be possessed by the
effective leader found in any group or organizational context. From a Type IV persepctive, on the other
hand, leadership is viewed primarily as a behavior or act, its effectiveness determined in part by situational
features. Each of the four perspectives is entirely compatible with our definition of leadership and represents
a potentially promising way of looking at leadership. As the following sections outline, moreover, each has
stimulated substantial empirical and theoretical development.
3. Type I Perspectives: The Search for Universal LeadershipTraits
From the turn of the century through the 1940's leadership research was dominated by attempts to show
that leaders possessed some intrinsic quality or characteristic that differentiated them from followers. The
search was directed toward identifying that property possessed by the likes of Napoleon, Hitler, Lincoln,
Gandhi, Kennedy (and their lesser known counterparts in educational, military and industrial settings) that
would ultimately prove to be the essence of successful and effective leadership. Research concentrated on
the measurement and quantification of leadership traits and the relationship between such traits and criteria
of leader effectiveness.
Leadership was not viewed as an independent property that was totally distinct and separate from the
other 17,000 trait names used in our language to identify differences among people [1]. On the contrary,
leadership was viewed as an abstract property the existence of which was explainable in terms of other more
basic or fundamental traits distinguishing individuals. In this sense leadership was treated as a second-level
trait construct composed of, or highly related to, more fundamental first-level trait constructs that included
physical and constitutional factors, skills and abilities, personality characteristics and social characteristics.
Empirical research was directed toward identifying these first-level traits, the cumulative outcome of which
was a large number of personal characteristics apparently associated with and contributing to leadership.
Table 2 lists the prominent first-level traits which the research evidence suggests are related to leadership
[4], [34], [62], [82], [108]. Presumably the more qualities or attributes contained in the list a person possesses,
the more he or she is likely to be an effective leader.
The organizational implications stemming from a Type I perspective are potentially far-reaching. If
leadership is indeed governed by the traits contained in Table 2, the selection of people most likely to be
successful in positions of leadership-including political officials, industrial managers, PTA presidentscould be made in a relatively straightforward and mechanical manner. Replacing subjective methods and
educated guesswork, a "leadership test" could be constructed to assess and weigh the various traits known to
be associated with leadership. Administration of such a test to all candidates for a particular position would
TABLE 2
LeadershipTraits
Physical and Constitutional
Factors
Activity, energy
Appearance, grooming
Height
Weight
Skill and Ability
Administrative ability
Intelligence
Judgment
Knowledge
Technical competence
Verbal fluency
Personality Characteristics
Achievement drive, ambition
Adaptability
Adjustment, normality
Aggressiveness
Alertness
Antiauthoritarianism
Dominance
Emotional balance, control
Enthusiasm
Extraversion
Independence, nonconformity
Initiative
Insightfulness
Integrity
Objectivity
Originality
Persistence
Responsibility
Self-confidence
Sense of humor
Tolerance of stress
Social Characteristics
Cooperativeness
Interpersonal skills,
sensitivity
Popularity, prestige
Sociability
Socioeconomic position
Talkativeness
Tact
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ARTHUR G. JAGO
318
provide a quantitative estimate of their relative leadership potential and a basis for selecting the most
promising idividtual.
Unfortunately, several features of Type I research prevent the development of such a test in spite of
readily available techniques (e.g., I.Q. tests, personality inventories) that measure those first-order traits
thought to be related to leadership. First, the relationships uncovered in Type I research are typically weak,
albeit statistically significant. There are, for example, far too many exceptions to the general rule that
effective leaders possess superior intelligence for this relationship to exhibit much practical utility.' Superior
intelligence far from guarantees leadership; average or below average intelligence in no way precludes
leadership. Although intelligence and other traits are indeed known to be related to leadership, measures of
such traits have been found to have extremely limited predictive value.
Second, the direction and strength of certain relationships apparently depend on certain situational
contingencies. Gibb [35], Stogdill [108], and others note the existence of certain inconsistencies and
contradictions among the results of Type I research that can only be explained if one rejects the notion of a
universal leadership trait (or set of traits) and accepts the fact that those who are successful leaders in some
situations may not necessarily be successful leaders in all situations. The relative importance of different
leadership traits may depend upon the organizational setting, historical precedent, the nature of the specific
goals or objectives of the group, task characteristics, and the traits and characteristics of followers. Among
prison inmates, for example, Schrag [95] reports that informal leaders tended to be homosexual, neurotic and
psychopathic-traits certainly not ascribed to all leaders or even to leaders in all prison settings [42].
Finally, there are potential problems with the most popular method for testing the existence of a
leadership trait. The ideal Type I paradigm would involve a longitudinal comparison of effective and
ineffective leaders engaged in similar if not identical leadership roles. However, unavoidable problems in the
definition and measurement of "effectiveness" have forced many researchers to instead compare the traits of
leaders to the traits of followers. The implicit assumption is that the person formally designated a leader
(e.g., manager, supervisor or foreman) has, on the average, greater leadership than does the average person
not so designated. This does, however, cause two potential problems in the interpretation of results. First,
measurable distinctions between leaders and followers may say more about an inappropriate selection
process employed to choose managers than about the nature of leadership itself. If, for example, intelligence
is one factor considered in the decision to promote an employee to a managerial position, differences in
levels of intelligence between managers and subordinates would be expected even if intelligence bears no
relationship to the ability to lead. The apparent relationship between intelligence and leadership could
merely be an artifact of an inappropriate and invalid promotion process. Second, a simple comparison of
leaders and followers does not necessarily imply that the leadership "trait" preceded selection to the
leadership role. Occupying a position of leadership may, for example, have the effect of increasing one's
self-confidence, an effect which might be expected regardless of who assumes the position. If this were the
case, the relationship between leadership and self-confidence would be spurious, each variable being affected
in a similar manner by a third variable (i.e., promotion to the position).
These and other problems led many researchers to abandon the search for a universal trait of leadership.
Nonetheless, Calder [7] has recently advanced a theory that has rekindled interest in leadership traits. When
compared to the typical Type I perspective, however, this theory takes a dramatically different approach in
specifying the acquisition and functional value of such traits.
An Attribtutioni
Theoty of Leadership
Calder suggests that leadership is indeed a disposition or trait but that it only need exist in the perceptions
of others, particularly followers. According to Calder, leadership itself is not a viable scientific construct or
variable. However, the process by which people inifer that real or imagined leadership qualities exists in
others is indeed important and is capable of scientific analysis. People label others as possessing or as not
possessing leadership qualities and the manner in which those labels become attached is the foundation of
an attribution theory of leadership. Notice that from this persepctive, the leader (or potential leader) is not
the focal person of scientific analysis. Instead, observers of the leader become focal as we attempt to
understand the psychological processes that lead to certain perceptions, labels and attributions.
The attribution of leadership begins either with the actual observation of a person's behavior and its
consequences, with second-hand accounts of the person's behavior or with "inferred observations" (i.e.,
simple observations which may imply the existence of otherwise unobserved behavior). These observations
are then accepted or rejected as evidence of leadership on the basis of four rules or criteria: (1) Is the
behavior distinctive? (2) Is the behavior typical of how leaders are expected to act? (3) Is the behavior either
consistent across time and place or extreme?, and (4) Can other dispositional (or trait) causes of the observed
behavior be rejected? Central to the process of answering these questions and to any ultimate attribution of
leadership is the observer's "implicit leadership theory" that specifies certain perceived differences between
'In their literature reviews, Stogdill [108] reports a mean correlation coefficient of 0.28 and Mann [82]
reports a median correlation coefficient of 0.25.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
319
leaders and followers. Such implicit theories may have little validity and indeed may be quite contrary to
some of the theories discussed in this review. Nonetheless these implicit theories provide the standard against
which the behavior is evaluated for evidence that it indeed represents leadership.
Attribution theory has generated substantial research interest in recent years, although most empirical
studies have been concerned with specific dispositional attributions other than leadership [64]. In a notable
exception, however, Mitchell, Larson and Green [90] reveal that an outside observer of an interacting group
will make inferred observations about the leader's behavior depending upon the perceived success or failure
of the group in accomplishing its task. Although this study falls short of fully documenting an inferred
leadership trait, the evidence suggests that a successful group can promote the inference that the leader must
indeed be doing something right-that is, something distinctive and typical of how leaders should behave.
Also noteworthy, House [53] has advanced a theory of charismatic leadership consistent with-although
not formally derived from-an attribution theory perspective. Charisma can be viewed as a special form or
type of leadership-leadership inferred from the observation of behavior that has what might be called
"charismatic effects" (i.e.. effects which represent evidence of charisma as opposed to other leadership
qualities oI traits). Similar to the more general treatment of the attribution of leadership advanced by
Calder, House places much less emphasis on defining and specifying the fundamental properties and traits
of charisma than he does on specifying: (1) the nature of charismatic effects, (2) the conditions under which
these charismatic effects will occur, and (3) the conditions under which one will be labeled as having
charismatic qualities.
4. Type II Perspectives: LeadershipStyles
After the early disappointments of Type I perspectives, research in the late 1940s began examiniing
leadership in terms of the behavioral interaction between leader and follower. Leadership was viewed as an
observable process or activity rather than an inherent, often unobservable, personal characteristic or trait.
For this Type II perspective, effective leaders were presumably distinguished from ineffective leaders not
by how they behaved on an intelligence or personality test but rather by how they behaved when interacting
with followers or potential followers. Type II research concentrated on two related issues. The first issue
concerned the dimensionality of leader behavior. What meaningful categories or factors can be used to
describe differences in leader behavior? What summary dimensions can be used to identify patterns-or
"styles"-of leader behavior? The second issue concerned the relative effectiveness of different leader
behaviors. What categories or factors seem to distinguish effective from ineffective leaders? What is the
optimal leadership style?
Considerationand Initiating Structure
To address the dimensionality issue, a questionnaire was developed that initially contained 150 items
designed to assess subordinates' perceptions of their leaders' actual behavior. Administered in both military
[45] and industrial settings [26], responses to the questionnaires were factor analyzed to empirically
determine the underlying factor structure or dimensionality of leader behavior. Two distinct factors
emerged, their independence initially confirmed by later investigations in other settings [32], [43].2
The first factor, labeled consider-ation(C), involves the degree of two-way communication and consultation, mutual trust, respect, and warmth a leader exhibits toward his followers. A leader would receive a high
score on consideration if his subordinates would agree with descriptions such as "He makes those feel at
ease when talking to them," "He is friendly and approachable," "He looks out for the personal welfare of
group members," "He puts suggestions into operation." On the other hand a leader would receive a low
score on consideration if his subordinates disagreed with these descriptions.
The second factor, labeled initiating structure (IS), involves the degree to which the leader defines and
organizes relationships among group members and establishes well-defined channels of commuinication and
methods of accomplishing the group's task. A leader would receive a high score on initiating structure if his
subordinates would agree with descriptions such as "He assigns people to particular tasks," "He schedules
the work to be done," "He asks that group members follow standard rules and regulations," "He emphasizes
deadlines." On the other hand, a leader would receive a low score on initiating structure if his subordinates
disagreed with these descriptions.
It is important to emphasize that consideration and initiating structure are not opposite ends of a single
leader behavior continuum, but are instead separate and conceptually independent dimensions. Therefore,
2On the basis of these studies the questionnaire was reduced to 40 items and named the Leader Behavior
Description Questionnaire or LBDQ [43]. Alternate forms of this instrument include the Leadership Opinion
Questionnaire or LOQ [29], the Supervisory Behavior Description Questionnaire or SBDQ [30], and the
Leadership Behavior Description Questionnaire-Form XII or LBDQ-XII [107]. These different instruments, however, have produced different results leading to some confusion in the consideration/initiating
structure literature [96].
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
320
ARTHUR G. JAGO
in addition to the possibilities of being high in C and low in IS or of being low in C and high in IS, it is
entirely possible to score high on both dimensions or low on both dimensions (or, of course, any
combination in-between). The conceptualization of leader behavior in this two-dimensional fashion has
generated far more research than a competing one-dimensional framework that referred to "employeecentered" versus "production-centered" supervision [66], [67].
The next step involved the identification of the optimal leadership style i.e., the most effective combination of consideration and initiating structure behaviors that a leader can display toward his followers. In a
number of military, educational and industrial settings, leaders' reliance on C and IS were measured and
correlated with criteria such as (1) subordinate satisfaction, (2) subordinate performance, (3) subordinate,
peer, or superior evaluations of leader performance. In a number of these studies [30], [33], [44], [48], [56],
the analyses suggested the most effective leader was one who exhibited both high consideration (C) and high
initiating structure (IS). The successful leader was one who developed good rapport and two-way communication with subordinates and who, at the same time, took an active role in planning and directing group
activities.
These results also had potentially far-reaching prescriptive implications. Whereas Type I research
attempted to identify those factors what would be useful in selecting leaders, Type II research attempted to
identify those factors that would be useful in training leaders. Unlike relatively stable traits and characteristics, behavior patterns can be presumably changed through instruction and practice. Conceptualizing
leadership in terms of the behavior patterns of the leader suggests that effective leadership is an acquired
skill and can therefore be taught.
There is indeed some evidence that attitudinal and behavioral changes do result from leadership training
involving self-assessment, lectures, groups discussions, case studies, films, role-playing and "sensitivitytraining" [8], [108]. At the same time, however, there is remarkably little evidence to suggest that such
changes have the intended effect of increasing group or organization effectiveness. Managers can indeed be
trained to behave in ways characteristic of high consideration and high initiating structure but the expected
pay-offs from such training are not necessarily realized [8].
Additional research concerning the consequences of different behavior patterns provides a potential
explanation for the disappointing results of these training efforts. A number of studies indicate that under
certain circumstances a behavior pattern emphasing both consideration and initiating structure may not
always be ideal [73], [75]. The effectivenss of C and IS may depend on: (1) follower needs and dependencies,
(2) follower ability, (3) the degree of task structure, (4) the degree of intrinsic satisfation associated with the
task, (5) task pressure, (6) job level, (7) follower expectations, and (8) leader upward influence [55], [74].
Moreover, the effectiveness of C and IS apparently depends on one's choice of criteria. Studies of
production supervisors indicate that higher initiating structure is related to higher proficiency ratings (by
upper management) but also to higher rates of grievances and turnover. On the other hand, higher
consideration is related to lower proficiency ratings but also to lower rates of grievances and turnover [31],
[32]. These results suggest the futility of attempting to identify a universally appropriate leadership style
based on the consideration and initiating structure dimensions. Initially overlooked situational contingencies
exist, rendering training in C and IS to be less applicable than first thought.
Nonetheless, one-best-way training designs having foundations in the early C and IS research persist. For
example, Blake and Mouton's [5] Managerial Grid? program trains managers to exhibit both high "concern
for production" and high "concern for people." Despite the lack of unequivocal evidence supporting the
training's effectiveness [68], the program is widely adopted. Its simplicity contributes to its intuitive appeal,
despite what others have labeled the "great hi-hi leader behavior myth" [76].
Autocracy-Democracy
At about the same time that substantial research interest was devoted to consideration and initiating
structure, other researchers taking a Type II perspective were employing an autocracy-democracy dimension
in their attempts to identify an optimal leadership style. At one end of the continuum, autocratic leadership
is characterized by highly centralized decision-making and completely concentrated power. At the other end
of the continuum, democratic leadership is characterized by highly participative decision-making and
power-equilization. Although "styles" between these extremes are often proposed [47], [109], it is worth
noting that most empirical research from this Type II perspective has treated these endpoints as if they
represented an autocracy versus democracy dichotomy.
It is also worth noting that the notion of democratic leadership overlaps somewhat with the
"consideration" dimension in the C and IS literature. Some items purported to measure consideration have a
distinctly democratic flavor: (1) "He consults the group before acting," (2) "He treats all group members as
his equals," and (3) "He gets group approval on important matters before going ahead" [29], [43]. Despite
this overlap, the dimensions are far from identical. Consideration includes a host of behaviors not
necessarily implied by a democratic leadership style (e.g., "he does personal favors for group members").
Hypotheses concerning the relative effectiveness of autocratic and democratic leadership styles rely
heavily on certain presumed benefits of participative decision-making and power-sharing. Morse and Reimer
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
321
[91] suggest that democratic leadership provides followers with the opportunity to express and fulfill
individual needs in the course of accomplishing group goals. The opportunity for regulating and controlling
their own activities enhances psychological identification with the group and its task and provides a means
for satisfying ego-esteem and self-actualization needs through the work of the group rather than at its
expense. Morale and group productivity may therefore both benefit from a democratic supervisory style.
In addition, democratic leadership is thought to directly enhance the effectiveness of managerial decisions
[81], [113]. Participative decision-making provides a vehicle for follower information, expertise and creativity
to be brought to bear on problems for which the leader's own information and knowledge may be
insufficient. Moreover, power-sharing can create a climate where constructive conflict is encouraged, thereby
insuring that important perspectives on a problem or decision are not overlooked. Aside from these benefits
to the quality of decision, follower involvement may ease the implementation of a decision. First, the need to
communicate the decision is reduced. By being actively involved in the process, followers are likely to
understand the decision and their roles in implementing its provisions. Second, potential resistance to the
decision is greatly reduced. Presumably, participation in the decision making process fosters ego involvement
in the decision itself leading to a greater sense of commitment to the course of action chosen than if the
same decision had been arrived at by more autocratic means.
If these benefits of democratic leadership do exist, the organizational implications once again involve
training. To improve leadership effectiveness, one would provide the leader with instruction and practice in
the behavioral pattern shown to be most effective-in this case, participative decision-making and powersharing.
As in the case of C and IS however, no overwhelming evidence exists to support the initial predictions of
proponents of democratic leadership. In the most comprehensive discussion to date of the relevant research
evidence, Locke and Schweiger [78] review 46 studies testing the effects of participative decision-making on
work group productivity and 43 studies testing the effects of participative decision-making on work group
satisfaction. Employing productivity as the criterion, only 10 (22%) of the studies revealed democratic
leadership to be superior to autocratic leadership. Of the remainder, 26 (56%) of the stuLdiesrevealed no
significant or no unequivocal differences and 10 (22%7)actually suggested democratic leadership to be
inferior. The results are only somewhat more supportive for the criterion of work group satisfaction.
Twenty-six (60%) of the studies support the superiority of democratic leadership; 13 (30%) reveal no
significant or no unequivocal evidence; 4 (9%) studies suggest democratic leadership to be inferior to
autocratic leadership.
Reflecting on this evidence, Locke and Schweiger (and others [25]) conclude that the effects of democratic
leadership may depend upon a number of contextual or situational variables including: (1) the extent of
leader and follower knowledge and expertise, (2) follower motivation, (3) task attributes (e.g., complexity),
(4) the degree of conflict over goals or means to attain goals, (5) leader attributes, (6) time pressures, (7)
group and organization size, and (8) environmental stability.
The similarity between these conclusions and those reached in reviewing the research of Type I and other
Type II perspectives is not coincidental. Each of these theoretical approaches has assumed the existence of a
one-best-way to lead, that is, a universally appropriate set of traits or behaviors that distinguishes effective
leaders from ineffective leaders. The empirical evidence clearly reveals the lack of a basis for such a
sweeping assumption.
Leader Behavior: Cause or Effect?
An implicit-if not explicit-assumption in Type II research is that leadership behavior causes or
determines group and organizational outcomes; among these outcomes, follower satisfaction and performance. A correlation between "consideration" and "performance," for example, is taken as evidence that
higher levels of follower performance are caused by higher levels of consideration. An intervention designed
to increase the frequency with which a manager displays considerate behavior would therefore be expected
to produce a concomitant increase in follower satisfaction.
The expectation, however, may not be fulfilled. Using sophisticated methodological techniques designed
to detect the direction of causation, several studies have challenged the conventional wisdom that leader
behavior causes follower satisfaction and performance by revealing that the opposite can and does occur. In
an elaborate simulation of an office setting, Lowin and Craig [79] report that supervisory personnel respond
to more competent employees by displaying more considerate behavior, less initiating structure and less
close supervision. As the apparent competence of the subordinate varied, so did the leadership style. Farris
and Lim [16] report that leaders who believed that their subordinates were high performers displayed greater
supportiveness and greater goal emphasis and were perceived to be more sensitive and nonpunitive than
leaders who believed that their subordinates were low performers. Herold [49] and Greene [38], [40] report
additional evidence suggesting that higher follower performance can cause increases in leader supportiveness
and consideration and cause decreases in leader punitiveness and initiating structure. Crowe, Bochner and
Clark [10] report that leaders become more democratic when followers exercise initiative, offer ideas and set
goals and become more autocratic when followers are passive, request instructions and are unquestioning.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
322
ARTHUR G. JAGO
Other studies have gone beyond the leader-follower relationship and have examined the broader social
environment for possible antecedents of leader behavior. Space prevents a detailed discussion of these
investigations. However, the collective conclusion of these efforts is that work groups [28], [32], [60],
functional departments [13], [47], hierarchical levels [6], [27], [47], [61], and entire organizations [116] develop
unique "climates" or "cultures" favoring certain leadership practices. Through selection or socialization
processes, managers entering such settings either already possess or quickly acquire the accepted leadership
style. By their behavior they "fit in" thereby sustaining the norm and reinforcing the culture. Thus the
broader context within which a leader operates may be as important an antecedent of leader behavior as his
or her own predisposition.
5. Type III Perspectives: Fiedler's Contingency Model
Disappointments with the Type I and Type II perspectives led scholars to conclude that "leadership
depends on the situation." Of course, this obvious truism could offer little insight or practical value unless
certain precise situational contigencies could be specified and validated. The exact conditions under which
different leadership traits or leadership behaviors would be effective required identification. Research of
Types III and IV attempts to provide such theoretical and empirical elaboration by offering more complex
leadership models that presumably come closer to representing the complexities of actual leadership
effectiveness.
Type III research is concerned with specifying the conditions under which certain leader traits (rather
than behaviors) are effective. However, Type III research has hardly exhausted the list of potential traits
(Table 2) that may interact with situational variables to determine leader effectiveness. In fact, for over
twenty years Type III research has been dominated by a single leadership model involving a single
leadership trait. In developing a "Contingency Model of Leadership Effectiveness," Fiedler and his
associates [19], [20], [22] argue that group productivity is dependent upon the match between: (1) a
personality trait labeled task versus relationship motivation, and (2) the "favorableness" of the leadership
situation.
Task versus relationship motivation is measured by Fiedler's Least Preferred Co-worker scale [22]. In
completing the instrument, you are asked to think of the person which whom you can work least well and to
describe this person by placing one check mark on each of 16 eight-point bipolar adjective pairs similar to
the following:
Unpleasant
:
:
:
:
:
:
:
Pleasant
4
7
1
2
5
6
3
8
Each item is scored from one to eight, with eight being the most favorable description. Your Least Preferred
Co-worker (LPC) score is simply the sum of the 16 item scores.
Interpreting your LPC score hinges on the assumption that your descriptions of your co-worker says more
aboutyou than about the person you have described. In essence, it is assumed that everyone's least preferred
co-worker is about equally "unpleasant" and that differences in descriptions of these co-workers actually
reflect differences in an underlying personality trait among the people doing the describing. Although
several competing interpretations of LPC scores exist [19], [22], [89], Fiedler has recently argued that there is
little doubt that "high LPC leaders (i.e., leaders who describe their least preferred co-worker in relatively
favorable terms) are basically more concerned with interpersonal relations, while low LPC leaders (i.e.,
leaders who describe their least preferred co-worker in relatively unfavorable terms) are more concerned
with task-relevant problems" [21, p. 48].
The distinction between relationship-oriented and task-oriented leadership is somewhat similar to the
distinction made in Type II research between considerate leader behavior and initiating structure leader
behavior. In fact, Fiedler has used the terms "considerate leadership" and "structuring leadership" to refer
to high LPC and low LPC leaders respectively [17], [18]. They are, however, far from identical concepts.
First, relationship-motivated and task-motivated leadership are proposed to be opposite ends of a single
continuum rather than distinct and independent dimensions. Second, relationship-motivated and taskmotivated leadership are thought to represent relatively stable predispositions or personality traits rather
than more transitory behavioral styles capable of change.3
Fiedler defines situation favorableness in terms of three critical dimensions listed here in their apparent
order of importance:
(1) Leader-Member Relations-the degree to which group members trust and like the leader and are
willing to follow his guidance.
(2) Task Structure-involves four elements: (a) the degree to which the requirements of the job are clearly
stated (i.e., goal clarity), (b) the number of different ways in which the job can be performed (i.e., goal-path
3Similar parallels exist with the autocracy-democracy dimension proposed in Type II research. Fiedler [18]
describes high LPC leaders as being "non-directive" and "permissive" while low LPC leaders are "directive"
and "autocratic."
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
323
multiplicity), (c) the degree to which the job provides knowledge of results (i.e., verifiability), and (d) the
degree to which there exists an optimal solution or outcome for the task (i.e., specificity).
(3) Position Power-the degree to which there exists vested authority in the leadership position giving the
leader the right to direct, evaluate, reward and punish group members.
Fiedler dichotomizes each critical dimension (good vs. poor leader member relations; structured vs.
unstructured tasks; strong vs. weak position power) thereby producing eight combinations of stituational
characteristics. The most "favorable" of these combinations is the situation characterized by good leader
member relations, a structured task and strong position power. The least "favorable" of these combinations
is the situation characterized by poor leader member relations, an unstructured task and weak position
power.
The most effective leadership trait (task-motivated vs. relationship-motivated) in various situations was
arrived at inductively by correlating LPC scores with work group productivity scores for leaders in each of
the eight combinations of situational characteristics [19]. These results, summarized in Table 3, reveal that
task-oriented leaders tend to perform best in very favorable or in very unfavorable situations and that
relationship-oriented leaders tend to perform best in moderately favorable situations. Inasmuch as these
relationships were arrived at inductively employing a measure (LPC), the meaning of which is debatable,
there are no unequivocal explanations for why the relationships exist as they do. Even without such
explanations, however, the data are convincing enough for Fiedler to conclude that "it is simply not
meaningful to speak of an effective leader or of an ineffective leader; we can only speak of a leader who
tends to be effective in one situation and ineffective in another [19, p. 261].
If the effectiveness of a leader is indeed determined by the match between his LPC score and the
favorableness of the situation, there are fewer implications for selection (as in Type I research) or for
training (as in Type II research) than there are for the placement of managers in organizations. One need not
hire (or otherwise select) only high or only low LPC people for positions requiring leadership. Nor should
one try to train leaders to be more task-motivated or relationship-motivated. (Indeed, Fiedler is highly
skeptical of the usefulness of such efforts since LPC is presumed to be governed by a stable personality trait
that resists meaningful change.) Instead one should take care in placing leaders in situations likely to fit their
leadership style: high LPC leaders in moderately favorable situations, low LPC leaders in either favorable or
unfavorable situations.
In the event that such optimal placement is impossible, an alternative is to "engineer the job to fit the
manager" [18]. On the assumption that it is easier to change the situation than to change the leadership style
of the leader, this "engineering" consists of increasing or decreasing the situational favorableness of the
situation to fit the leader's LPC. A self-paced leadership education program called Leader Match [23],
involves from four to 12 hours of self-assessment, situational diagnosis and instruction in the principles of
the contingency model. In the case of a mismatch between a leader's LPC score and the favorableness of his
leadership situation, suggestions are given for: (1) changing leader member relations (e.g., "spend more-or
less-informal time with your subordinates" [p. 154]); (2) modifying task structure (e.g., "break the job
down into smaller subtasks which can be more highly structured" [p. 155]), and (3) modifying position
power (e.g., "show your subordinates who's boss by exercising fully the powers which the organization
provides" [p. 157]; "call on members of your group to participate in planning and decision-making
functions" [p. 157]). Evidence bearing on the usefulness of such training is not unequivocal. However,
studies of the effectiveness of the Leader Match program reveal significant increases in the performance
ratings of trained personnel when compared to control groups receiving no training [1 1], [24], [77].
The contingency model has generated considerable controversy among leadership scholars [2], [71], [72],
[85], [94], [96], [97], [100], [101], [106], [111]. Although space prevents a thorough discussion of all the
conceptual and methodological issues central to this debate, it is worth noting that arguments have been
offered challenging: (1) the reliability and validity of LPC scores, (2) the measurement and evaluation of
situational favorableness, and (3) the very existence of the proposed interaction between LPC and situational
favorableness in determining effectiveness. By implication, these concerns also question the usefulness of the
Leader Match program based on the model. Despite these criticisms, Fiedler recently noted that his
contingency model is "one of the most researched and best-validated leadership theories at this time" [23, p.
3]. Even its detractors would agree, however, that the development of the contingency model challenged the
naive assumption that there exists a one-best-way to lead and that the model provided a valuable first step
toward conceptualizing leadership in terms of its situational dependencies.
6. Type IV Perspectives: Behavioral Contingencies
Like Fiedler's model, Type IV perspectives also make the assumption that effective leadership depends on
the situation. Unlike Fiedler's model, however, effective leadership is defined in terms of behaviors rather
than traits.
Whereas Fiedler's model represents an attempt to resolve certain inconsistencies and contradictions in
Type I research, Type IV research attempts to resolve similar inconsistencies and contradictions stemming
from Type II research. In fact, two of the three positions to be described in this section (path-goal theory
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
324
ARTHUR G.JAGO
-
Q
C
c
Q
S
)
0
0
o
0
0
c*
-
-
0
-
Q
-
0
Q
S
0
c
0
-
0
Q
0
2
c
*
0
.-
0
0
-
0
-
c
Q
00
2
c/-n
<0
0
0
0
0
-o
-
0
Q
Q
c
)
s
C
0
Q
S
-
0
0
U
bI
0
S
-
2
c
S
-
Q
2
0
0
S
o
0-0U
0
C/
-
U
-
0
0
0
S
c
0
2
H
H
0
___________
Q
0
0
0
S
0
2
0
.-
0
-
0
c
0
U
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
0
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
325
and the Vroon/Yetton model) are attempts to specify the situational contingencies that help to resolve some
of the problems found in the consideration and initiating structure literature and in the autocracydemocracy literature. The third position, an operant conditioning model of leadership, has its roots in
learning theory and is only indirectly related to previous leadership research.
Path-Goal Theory
Extending an initial formulation by Evans [15], House and his associates propose a path-goal theory of
leadership [52], [55], [57]. From this perspective leadership is viewed in terms of both the motivating impact
and the need satisfaction impact that the leader can have on followers; that is, effective leadership is thought
to involve behaviors that increase follower performance and/or satisfaction by means of enhancing those
psychological states that result in increased motivation or increased need satisfaction. It is labeled a
path-goal approach in that leader behavior is expressed in terms of the leader's influence in clarifying the
paths or routes followers travel toward work and personal goal attainment.4
The theory consists of two propositions:
The first proposition is that leader behavior is acceptable and satisfying to subordinates to the
extent that they see it as either an immediate source of satisfaction or as instrumental to future
satisfaction. The second proposition of the theory is that leader behavior will be motivational
(e.g., will increase subordinate effort) to the extent that (1) it makes satisfaction of subordinate
needs contingent on effective performance, and (2) it complements the environment of
subordinates by providing the coaching, guidance, support, and rewards which are necessary
for effective performance and which may otherwise be lacking in subordinates or their
environment [25, p. 254].
In essence, the leader's task is to assist followers in attaining their goals and objectives and to provide the
necessary conditions to insure that such goals and objectives are compatible with the mission of the group or
organization. The theory is a contingency theory of behavioral leadership (i.e., a Type IV theory) because
the above propositions imply that the effectiveness of various leadership behaviors depends upon characteristics of subordinates (e.g., need states, ability, tolerance for ambiguity, self-esteem) and characteristics of the
environment within which they function (e.g., task structure, role ambiguity, formal reward systems).
Although many possible predictions are suggested by path-goal theory, empirical research has concentrated on only two specific hypotheses:
(1) Leader initiating structure will contribute to the satisfaction of followers engaged in
ambiguous (i.e., unstructured) tasks and contribute to the dissatisfaction of followers engaged
in clear (i.e., structured) tasks.
(2) Leader consideration will have its most positive effect on the satisfaction of followers
engaged in clear (i.e., structured) tasks.
These hypotheses suggest that when task demands are unclear, or when formal procedures, regulations
and policies are ambiguous, structuring behavior complements the task by providing the required guidance
and instruction likely to clarify expectations and the paths to goal accomplishment. On the other hand, when
task demands are self-evident, as in the case of highly routinized or formalized work roles, followers may
resent a leader's attempt to initiate further structure. Such behavior may do little to further clarify path-goal
relationships and may be viewed as excessively directive and unnecessarily restrictive. Instead, the effective
leader in a structured situation engages in personally supportive behavior which provides a source of
extrinsic rewards for followers. Such extrinsic rewards reduce the frustration and stress that presumably
accompany a highly structured task having little challenge and few sources of instrinsic satisfaction.5
Research pertaining to these hypotheses, however, has produced mixed results. Schriesheim and Von
Glinow [98] cite six studies supporting the first hypothesis and the same number of studies failing to support
4Those familiar with theories of motivation will recognize that the term path-goal is derived from
expectancy theory [112]. Expectancy theory predicts that an individual's motivation to perform a given act is
a function of: (1) his beliefs that the act will lead to outcomes (i.e., expectancies), (2) his beliefs that these
outcomes can be instrumental to the attainment of other outcomes (i.e., instrumentalities) and (3) the
subjective value or utility he assigns to each outcome (i.e., valences). Expectancy theory provides a
conceptual base for the path-goal theory of leadership, the latter being an attempt to specify where the
leader can intervene in the motivational process to secure effort, performance and follower satisfaction.
5In a revision of the theory, House and Dessler [55] employ the labels instrulmentaland supportive
leadership rather than the conceptually similar initiating structure and consideration labels. House and
Mitchell [57] also consider the potential effects of participativeleadership and achievement-orientedleadership
as additional leader behavior constructs. Griffin [41] adds maintenance leadership, a minimum-interference
leader behavior strategy.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ARTHUR G. JAGO
326
the hypothesis. They cite five studies supporting the second hypothesis and four studies failing to support the
hypothesis.6 Explanations offered to account for such inconsistencies often center on the difficulties of
measuring the relevant variables contained in the theory and on differences in the procedures and
questionnaires employed in different studies [96], [98], [99]. Of course it is also possible that the theory is
incorrec-tor that available tests of the theory have considered too few situational variables (i.e., only task
structure) that may moderate the leader behavior-follower response relationship. With regard to this last
point, it should be noted that promising research is nlow being conducted to examine the possible
moderating effects of follower characteristics (e.g., [14]), of task dimensions other than task structure [52],
[63], and of complex combinations of task and follower characteristics [41].
Like the other Type IV approaches to leadership to be discussed, path-goal theory represents a relatively
recent contributioll to the leadership literature and is currently a subject of extensive empirical scrutiny by
leadership scholars. Should their efforts support path-goal predictions the organizational implications will be
similar to those growing out of Type II research. Leadership training (as opposed to selection or placement)
would be prescribed. However training would not only be focused on the acquisition of new behavioral
styles. Skills in diagnosing the leadership situation and rules for matching the appropriate behavior to the
situation would be emphasized. Leaders would not only be trained to exhibit initiating structure and
consideration but would also be taught the specific conditions or circumstances under which each is likely to
contribute to follower motivation and satisfaction. In essence, leaders would be taught how to adapt their
behavior to the demands of the situation.
An Operant ConditioningPrescr4iptive
Having roots in Skinnerian [104] learning theory, the operant conditioniing perspective on leadership
examines the effect that rewards and punishments can have in reinforcing particular behavior patterns of
followers. Leadership is viewed in terms of the process by which a leader or manager motivates and
"shapes" the behavior of followers or subordinates by controlling the consequences associated with various
behaviors. The perspective is a contingency theory of behavioral leadership (a type IV perspective) in that a
leader's greatest impact on followers is hypothesized to exist through the rewards and punishments he or she
makes contingent on their performance levels.7
Studies of MBA students [102], financial managers [93], hospital employees [103], retail store managers
[92], employees of a manufacturer of capital equipment [69], [70], and employees of insurance, R&D and
chemical companies [39], reveal consistent results for the effects of positive rewards (e.g., praise) but certain
inconsistencies in the effects of punitive rewards (e.g., reprimand). Making positive rewards contingent on
high levels of performance is indeed associated with higher overall levels of follower performance [39], [92],
[102], [103], and higher levels of subordinate satisfaction [39], [69], [70], [931 [103]. Making punitive rewards
contingent on low levels of performance, however, has been shown in some studies to be related to lower
overall levels of performance [92], [102] and satisfaction [39], [70] and, in other studies, to higher overall
levels of performance [39] and satisfaction [93], [103].
Although the evidence regarding punitive behavior is inconclusive, results do clearly suggest that making
positive rewards contingent on performance will motivate followers and provide for their need satisfaction.
Although few documented examples exist, training in the administration of such rewards will presumably
increase the leader's ability to control or shape follower behavior thus increasing the predictability and
desirability of follower responses to situations [46], [80]. A number of studies, for example, reveal the
potential benefits of making pay contingent on performance (e.g., [65]). Presumably applications involving
other, more informal rewards available to the leader (e.g., praise, encouragement, increased responsibility)
would similarly increase follower motivation.
It is noteworthy that the principal predictions of an operant conditioning approach to leadership can be
interpreted in terms of the path-goal theory previously discussed [54].8 From a path-goal perspective, the
leader who employs contingent rewards is clarifying paths to desired goals by making the "satisfaction of
subordinates' needs contingent on effective performance" [25, p. 254]. Both path-goal and operant conditioning approaches are remarkably similar in suggesting that the leader will have a motivating impact upoIn
followers to the extent that the leader makes rewards contingent on successful accomplishmenit of the task
and on the attainment of group goals and objectives.
6Research conducted after the Schriesheimnand Von Glinow review also presents mixed results [I 10].
7To the extent this framework deals with the effects of formal rewards and punishments, it is more
appropriately discussed as a theory of motivation rather than of leadership. Research efforts to date,
however, have not nmadean explicit distinction between the effects of formal (e.g., pay) and informal (e.g.,
praise) mechanisms and, for completeness, these efforts are discussed here.
8The reverse has also been proposed. Mawhinney and Ford [84] argue that evidence favoring path-goal
theory can be interpreted in terms of the principles of operant conditioninig.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
327
The V}roon7/YettonModel
When compared to either the path-goal or the operant conditioning theory, the Vroom/Yetton model
[118] is more explicit in its predictions but much more narrow in its focus. Although the relationship between
a leader and set of followers is recogniizedas having maniydimensions, Vroom and Yetton concenitrateonly
on the behavior of a formally designated leader in the specific decision-nmakingsituations he or she
encounters. Recognizing the lack of overwhelming evidence favoring either an autocratic or democratic
decision making style, they attempt to specify the specific situational contingencies likely to govern the
effectiveness of several decision making strategies. Following Maier [81], Vroom and Yetton propose that the
effectiveness of a decision is a function of three classes of outcomes, each of which may be expected to be
affected by the decision process used. These are:
1. The quality or rationality of the decision.
2. The acceptance or commitment on the part of subordinates to execute the decision effectively.
3. The amount of time required to make the decision.
Table 4 contains the taxonomy of decision processes available to the leader when faced with a problem or
decision.9 Each process is represented by a symbol (e.g., Al, CI, GII) which provides a convenienit method of
TABLE 4
Vroom/ YettonDecisioniMakinigProcesses
Symbol
Definition
Al
You solve the problem or make the decision yourself
using the information available to you at the present
time.
All
You obtain any necessary informationifrom subordinates,
then decide on a solution to the problem yourself. You
may or may not tell subordinates the prupose of your
questions or give information about the problem or
decision you are working on. The input provided by
them is clearly in response to your request for
specific information. They do not play a role in the
definition of the problem or in generating or
evaluation alternative solutions.
CI
You share the problem with the relevant subordinates
individually, getting their ideas and suggestionis
without bringing them together as a group. Then you
make the decision. This decision may or may not
reflect your subordinates' influenice.
CII
You share the problem with your subordinates in a group
meeting. In this meeting you obtain their ideas and
suggestions. Then you make the decision which may or
may not reflect your subordinates's influenice.
GII
You share the problem with your subordinates as a
group. Together you generate and evaluate alternatives
and attempt to reach agreement (consensus) on a
solution. Your role is much like that of chairpersoni,
coordinatinigthe discussion, keeping it focused on the
problem and making sure that the critical issues are
discussed. You can provide the group with information
or ideas that you have but you do not try to "press"
them to adopt "your" solution and are willing to accept
and implemiienit
any solutioniwhich has the support of the
entire group.
9The version of the model presented here is concerned withl what Vroom and Yettoni call "group
problems," i.e., problems affecting more than a single subordiniate of the manager. An extension of the
model to "individual problems" (i.e., problem affecting only a single subordiniate) is discussed by Vroom
and Jago [116].
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
328
ARTHUR G. JAGO
referring to it. The letters in the code signify the basic properties of the process (A stands for autocratic; C
for consultative; G for group). The roman numerals that follow the letter constitute variants on that process.
Thus Al represents the first variant on an autocratic process. All the second variant, and so on.
Choice from among these available decision processes is prescribed by the decision tree contained in
Figure 1 [118]. Arranged along the top of the tree are eight problem attributes, expressed here in the form of
simple Yes-No questions that a leader could ask himself about the decision-making situation he is presently
confronting.'0 To use the model, one starts at the left-hand side of the tree and works toward the right-hand
side, asking oneself the questions pertaining to any box that is encountered. When an endpoint on the tree is
reached, a number will be found designating the problem type and one or more decision-making processes
(called the "feasible set") considered appropriate for that problem.
Of course, the decision processes specified for each problem type are not arbitrary. The specification of
the feasible set of decision processes for each problem type is governed by a set of seven contingency rules
that serve to protect the quality and acceptance of the decision by eliminating alternatives that risk one or
the other of these decision outcomes (e.g., "If the quality of the decision is important and the leader does not
possess sufficient information or expertise to solve the problem alone, the Al is eliminated from the feasible
set."). The decision tree is merely a convenient structure for applying these rules, and, once the problem type
has been determined, the rules have all been applied. It can be seen that there are some problem types for
which only one method remains in the feasible set, and others for which two, three, four, or even five
methods remain in the feasible set.
Only two empirical investigations have examined the validity of the Vroom/Yetton model. In the first of
these studies, Vroom and Jago [117] compared the rated effectiveness of 117 managerial decisions reached
through use of a process within the problem's feasible set with the rated effectiveness of 64 other decisions
reached through use of a process outside the problem's feasible set. The evidence favored the Vroom/Yetton
model and each of its underlying tenets. This study, however, did not provide a completely independent
measure of decision effectiveness. A second, more ambitious study by Margerison and Glube [83] examined
the relationship between an index of overall conformity to the prescriptions of the Vroom/Yetton model
and the success of 45 owner-managers of identical retail franchises. Those owners exhibiting greater conformity to the Vroom/Yetton prescriptions had more economically productive franchises and had employees
who reported greaterjob satisfaction than did those owners exhibiting less conformity to these prescriptions.
The training implications of the Vroom/Yetton model are quite clear: if the model is valid, more effective
leadership in decision-making situations will result if leaders are taught to use the model to guide the choice
of the appropriate levels of subordinate participation. Vroom [114] describes a technology designed to
accomplish such learning. Prior to training, each participant completes a problem set containing from 30 to
54 hypothetical decision making situations. The trainee is asked to consider each case and to choose the level
of participation he or she would permit if the situation were real and he or she was the leader depicted.
Responses are then analyzed by a computer program that produces a three to seven page individualized
printout that compares the responses of the trainee both with the behavior prescribed by the normative
model and with the average or modal behavior of other trainees. After training in the model and practice in
its use, trainees receive their printouts and are given guidance in interpreting their scores. Although quite
preliminary, analyses of the training's effectiveness are encouraging [153], [168].
Managers' responses to the hypothetical problem set cases are also employed to generate descriptive
models of leader behavior that are then compared to the specific Vroom/Yetton normative prescriptions
[59], [116], [118]. Although these studies fall short of providing information about actual leader behavior in
real situations, the evidence does suggest that a leader's willingness to employ participative decision making
techniques is governed more by the unique characteristics of a decision making situation encountered than
by some invariant autocracy-democracy "style." Leaders are more willing to use participation in situations
which require a high quality, technically adequate, solution than in situations which seem rather trivial or
have no technical component. Leaders exhibit greater participation on those problems in which they lack
relevant information or expertise, particularly if the problem is unstructured as opposed to structured.
Leaders exhibit more participation on problems that require subordinate acceptance and commitment than
on problems that do not, especially if they lack the power to gain that acceptance from an autocratic
decision. Leaders are more participative when followers share organizational goals than when they do not
and when potential conflict among followers over preferred solutions is low rather than high.
In many ways these behavioral tendencies are quite similar to the rules underlying the Vroom/Yetton
decision tree. In fact, analyses of reported and prescribed behavior reveal that a leader's level of
participation fully conforms to the prescriptions of the normative model (i.e., falls within the prescribed
feasible set) in about two-thirds of all instances [116], [118]. Departures from the Vroom/Yetton feasible set
are more often due to violations of those rules designed to protect decision acceptance rather than due to
violations of rules designed to protect decision quality.
'0For a detailed definition of these attributes and of criteria to be used in making Yes-No judgments, see
Vroom and Yetton [118, pp. 21-31].
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
D
a.
0
.
=
U.
Z
ac/)
<,
a
\
t}
ur\
0
U
7
0
z
o,
D
$
U)
0O
H-
0
U
UL)
<
a
=
a)
0W
I
329
0
a-
Z
0
0
0
Z
I
-A
'
H-
z0
cn
v)
J
a)
0
0
L
\
c
w 1 t?0/
mc\
'?-~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~Q
u
Z
\
0
Z<CI
H0
~ ~
o~~
-
<
U)
0L
H-
LA
U)
~
Z~~
o
0
X~~~~
0
D
<~a
HH-
a)~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~L
0
m
0J0
--
>
&
X
2
CL
i-
Cr
0c
.
H
~.
0
Cr
<)0~
Cr
0z
S4
?
<
/~?\
\
0
Cr
Z
CrU)
CrL4
Cr
a
crL
Cr
0
U)
0
V
0
~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~
0
0
L
0r
JC
0LU)
LLJ
0
0
H-AJU
C
~<
0
0D
0
7
Cr
~~~~
H-~~~~~~~~~~V
U)
0~~~~~~
)
Cr
Cr
H-
LA
a)
0-
Z
-
H
<LH--0 0
0 cn
<r
L
LL
0j
U
m
z
0A
0
u
I
0~~~~~~~~~~~L
>~~~~~~~~~
~
0
H-L
7-
0)
a-
0
H-
C
0
0~~~~~~~~~~~~~~C
0t
?r
LI)
<~
0
LL
0)
o
a-<U)
o
a)
Z
U)
~. ~ H<L..
LL0
U)
<
c)
0
()
/~~~~~~<
a)_
H
O
-
<
Cr
\
)
H
U)
U) I
a
z
>- 0
-
Li-r
L)
Lu
F\
L)
U)-
LL
U)
Y
m
Cr
-
0
z
0
CC)
U
L
0L
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
330
ARTHUR G. JAGO
7.
Conclusions: Toward New Directions in LeadershipResearch
The four-fold typology employed in this review is designed to provide a useful vehicle for organizing the
dominant trends in prior leadership research. The rows and columns of the matrix (Table 1) represent the
underlying assumptions made and orientations taken by different leadership scholars. The intersection of
rows and columns produce cells in the matrix representing the different theoretical perspectives possible
when these assumptions and orientations are combined. Classification of specific theories of leadership in
the different cells provides the opportunity to compare approaches on the basis of their common sets of
assumptions (i.e., shared rows or columns) and the opportunity to contrast these same approaches on the
basis of any unique sets of assumptions (i.e., unshared rows or columns). Moreover, the typology has been
used to identify historical trends in leadership research and to indicate how certain perspectives represent
outgrowths of others.
It may seem tempting to single out one perspective on leadership (or, more narrowly, one theory of
leadership within a given perspective) as having contributed the greatest knowledge or as having the greatest
practical utility. To make such a judgement, however, would inappropriately undervalue the very real
contributions made in other approaches. Moreover, it makes little sense to view different leadership theories
as necessarily competing theories. Although sometimes implied by the proponents of an approach, no
leadership theory can rightfully claim comprehensive treatment of the entire domain of leadership phenomena. Because at least some empirical support is available for each perspective, leadership appears to be a far
more complex set of cause-and-effect relationships than suggested by any one of the comparatively simple
theoretical models offered to date.
It must also be remembered that the evidence accumulated within the boundaries of each of the four
perspectives, while somewhat supportive of propositions and hypotheses, is also to some degree either
incomplete, inconclusive, contradictory or controversial. Although it is clear that thousands of scholarly
efforts have greatly advanced our understanding of leadership phenomena, it is also clear that much is left to
be learned. Certain unanswered questions alone prevent any one perspective from standing out as singularly
superior to all others.
Much future research will undoubtedly be directed toward testing and refining existing leadership
approaches. Within the framework of a single theory, research will be designed to satisfy critics of previous
studies, to resolve certain anomalies and contradictions apparent in existing evidence, or to test yet untested
hypotheses and propositions derived from the theory. Due to measurement problems only briefly mentioned
in this review, much of this research will concentrate on methodological refinements rather than on
substantive issues.
Other research will go beyond existing theories and paradigms thereby broadening the knowledge base.
And much of this research will easily lend itself to classification in our four-fold typology. Particularly
valuable will be those approaches that extend the dimensions of leader behavior beyond the overly simple
one-dimensional or two-dimensional formulations characteristic of much of the existing Type II and Type
IV research. Also valuable will be those efforts directed at specifying the precise situational contingencies
that govern the effectiveness of various leadership traits. As the only prominent representative of Type III
research, Fiedler's "Contingency Model" has far from exhausted the list of traits for which such contingencies might exist.
Still other research, however, may not be so amenable to easy classification in our four-cell matrix. Novel
leadership perspectives with unique sets of assumptions and theoretical orientations may provide new ways
to examine and interpret leadership phenomena. To accommodate these new perspectives, additional ways
of sorting or classifying approaches may become important. If some isolated (although notable) contributions to leadership research become trends, the following conceptual distinctions may prove useful in an
eventual expansion of the dimensionality of our four-fold typology:
Leadershipvs. Stupervision
Following Jacobs [58], the definition of leadership proposed in this review suggests a distinction between
leadership processes and supervisory processes. Leadership involves the influence of group members through
interpersonal processes without resort to the authority or power derived from an employment contract.
Supervision, on the other hand, involves the influence of group members through the use of formal rewards
and punishments and through the exercise of contractual obligations. This distinction seems particularly
important if leadership as a research topic is to evolve into an area conceptually and empirically
independent of other areas of scientific inquiry (e.g., motivation, control). At present, the boundaries of
different areas often overlap, as illustrated by the heavy influence of motivation theories in both path-goal
and operant conditioning leadership theories. This has led not only to substantial confusion regarding what
leadership is and is not but also to the extreme view that definitional problems suggest leadership itself
should be abandoned as a researchable topic [86]. No opportunity to discount such arguments will be
available unless future research is more careful in defining the domain of leadership vis a vis other
explanatory constructs.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
331
Dyadic vs. GroupProcesses
Advancing this distinction between leadership and supervision, recent work by Graen and his associates
[12], [36], [37] suggest that a person in a position of formal authority may "lead" some subordinates but
merely "supervise" others. Managers differentiate subordinates into in-group members (i.e., the "trusted
cadre" who receive leadership) and out-group members (i.e., the "hired hands" who receive supervision).
Members of different subgroups respond differently to the managers and these differences can be important
determinants of the relative performance and satisfaction of group members.
Graen and his colleagues have a legitimate criticism of the majority of prior leadership research that
typically employs what is labeled the "average style" approach. Researchers using such an approach have
assumed that behavior patterns on the part of a leader are stylistic in nature and similarly applied to all
group members. Reported departures from such styles are treated as "random error" (in a statistical sense)
and explained away with cursory references to measurement error, response bias, perceptual differences, etc.
In concentrating on the "vertical dyad" (i.e., individual leader-follower relationships), Graen and his
colleagues have demonstrated that these departures from some average are far from random and, in fact, are
predictive of subsequent phenomena.
Advancing these results, future research should distinguish between leader behavior displayed only in a
one-to-one relationship with individual followers and leader behavior displayed in a one-to-many relationship with a group. Graen's research clearly indicates that there are important differences in the interpersonal
dynamics of dyadic vs. group processes yet these differences have only begun to be identified.
Appointedvs. Emergent Leadership
As suggested in the introduction of this review (and reinforced by the discussion of the empirical
evidence), the majority of research investigations have examined leadership within the context of a
managerial role in a formal organizational setting. In addition to contributing to the confounding of
leadership with supervision, this trend has produced little in the way of knowledge about the processes that
lead to the development of informal leadership, i.e., the processes by which a group member becomes
elevated to a position of status and leadership thereby attaining greater than average influence over peers.
Such processes may be quite important. It seems reasonable to speculate that many situations exist in
which an informal leader within a group has much greater influence over fellow group members than does a
formal leader appointed to a position of managerial authority over the group. Indeed, successful labor
unionization efforts are often attributed to the emergence of a persuasive and powerful leader from the ranks
of those led.
A notable exception to the absence of programmatic research on emergent leadership is work by
Hollander [50], [51]. This research documents that it is the conforming group member who is likely to attain
elevation to a position of informal leadership. It is the contributing individual who has "paid his dues" who
often becomes the high status member capable of ultimately redirecting the group and effecting changes in
the same group norms toward which prior conformity was exhibited. Clearly additional systematic research
concerning other possible determinants of emergent leadership is warranted. At the same time, care should
be taken not to confuse the processes of effective appointed leadership and what may be very different
processes of effective emergent leadership. At least one study suggests that these two types of leaders behave
in very distinct ways [9].
Objectivevs. Subjective Measures
Certainly most leadership studies have relied at least in part on either self-report measures of leadership
constructs (e.g., reports of intended behavior in hypothetical situations employed in the Vroom/Yetton
research) or on follower-report measures of such constructs (e.g., subordinate ratings of a manager's level of
Consideration and Initiating Structure). Yet recent developments in attribution theory raise some troubling
issues in the interpretation of such subjective measures of behavior. To the extent self-description and
followers' descrip ions are affected by perceptual biases, implicit theories and "inferred observations" that
are invoked as a consequence of the leader's impact or accomplishments, the evidence may be systematically
distorted by attributional processes in a direction that may artificially inflate relationships between
presumed leadership constructs and leadership consequences. Many of our "tests" of leadership theories
may have actually produced nothing more than a specification of the implicit-and perhaps fallacious"theories" adopted by our questionnaire respondents (e.g., "he is such an effective leader he must be
considerate").
Greater use of more objective measures of leader behavior, including direct and independent observation,
is certainly required if the existence of such distortions is to be fully documented and prior evidence properly
interpreted. Yet care must be taken not to rely exclusively on the experimental laboratory to provide the
setting for such measures. Although carefully designed experiments provide the opportunity for structured
observation minimally affected by observer bias, the short-term nature of the typical experiment and the
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
332
ARTHUR G. JAGO
artificial setting withiln the laboratory often limit the externial validity (i.e., generalizability) of results.
Research methodologies with the realism of the field setting and the control of the laboratory are required.
This is not to imply that subjective assessments of leadership should be abandonied. Such assessments.
however, should be recognized as containinig potential distortions due to perceptual biases, selective recall,
limited observables and inaccurate attributionial mechanisms. Building on the theoretical development of
Calder, research directed toward understanding the antecedenitsof these distortionisis particularly appropriate.
Toward NewpMethodologies
These largely uncharted areas provide significant challenges for social scientists interested in uLnderstanding the development, dynamics and effectiveness of leaderslhipprocesses. Literally thousanidsof studies have
contributed substantial knowledge having implications for the selection, placement and traininigof leaders.
However, we seem to know a great deal about only a few elements of leadership. Although existing researclh
paradigms are not to be undervalued, it is clear that new methods and measuLesare required if leadership is
to develop and mature as a subdiscipline within behavioral science. Efforts that depart from the ubiquitous
concepts of "consideration," "initiating structure," "participation," and "LPC" (and their respective penciland-paper measuremenitinstrumenits)are necessary if research is to be broadened in some of the suggested
directions.
Unfortunately, some critics of leadership research may view such efforts as further evidence of a
fragmented literature lacking consistency and a programmatic thrust (cf., [86]). The less parochial view is
that such efforts will begin to fill large gaps that exist in legitimate areas within the domain of leadership
research. What is required, of course, is both programmatic refinements to existing theory and researclhand
creative expansions into uLnexploredterritory. The former will provide depth while the latter will provide
breadth to a literature having substantial social and scholarly significance.' I
1l An expanded version of this article was presented at the TIMS XXIV Internatioiial Meeting, Honolulu,
Hawaii, June, 1979. The author appreciates the helpful comments of L. Cummings, J. Ivancevich, D.
Schweiger, A. Szilagyi, V. Vroom, and G. Wolf on a draft versioniof this manuscript.
References
3.
University of North Carolina. Chapel Hill, N.C., 1933.
Organi7ational
ASHOUR, A. S., "The Contingency Model of Leadership Effectiveness: An Evaluation?"
Behavior and Human Peiformance, Vol. 9 (1973), pp. 339-355.
J. Social Isstes, Vol. 4 (1948),
Roles of Group Members,"
BENNE, K. D. AND SHEATS, P., "FuLctionial
4.
BIRD, C., Social Psy,chology, Appleton-Century,
5.
BLAKE,
1.
2.
ALLPORT, F. M., Institutional Behaior,
pp. 41-49.
New York, 1940.
6.
J. S., The ManagerialGrid, Gulf PublishlinigCo., HoUston,Tex., 1964.
BLANKENSHIP, L. V. AND MILES, R. E., "OrganizationialStructuLeanid Maniagerial Decisioni Behavior,"
Admin. Sci. Quart., Vol. 13 (1968), pp. 106-120.
7.
CALDER,
8.
New Directions in OrganizationalBehavior, St. Clair Press, Chicago, Ill., 1977, pp. 179-204.
CAMPBELL, J. P., DUNNETTE, M. D., LAWLER, E. E. AND WEICK, K. E., Managerial Behavior,
Peiformance, and Effectiveness,McGraw-Hill, New York, 1970.
9.
CARTER,
R.
R. AND MOUTON,
B. J., "An Attribution
L. F.,
HAYTHRON,
W.,
Tlheory of Leaderslhip," in Staw, B. M., and Salanclik, G. R. (Eds.),
SHRIVER,
B. AND
LANZETTA,
J. T., "The Bellavior of Leaders and
Other Group Members," J. Abnormal Social Psvchology, Vol. 46 (1954), pp. 589-595.
10.
CROWE, B. J., BOCHNER,
S. AND CLARK,
A. W., "The Effects of Subordinates'
Belhavior on Maniagerial
Style," Human Relations, Vol. 25 (1972), pp. 215-237.
11.
12.
L. S. AND BONS, P. M. "Manipulating the Situationi to Fit the Leader's Style: Two Validation
Studies of LEADER MATCH," J. Appl. Psychology,
Vol. 63 (1978), pp. 295-300.
DANSEREAU,
F., GRAEN, G. AND HAGA, W. J., "A Vertical Dyad Linkage Approach to Leadership
Withini Formal Organizations: A Longitudinal Investigationi of the Role-Making Process," Organiza-
CSOKA,
tional Behaviorand Human Peiformance, Vol. 13 (1975), pp. 46-78.
13.
DAY,
D. R., "Basic Dimenisions
of Leadership
in a Selected
Industrial
Organizationi,"
(Doctoral
Dissertation, Olio State University), Dissertation Abstracts Internat., Vol. 22 (1961), p. 3760.
14. DESSLER, G., A Test of thlePath-Goal Theoryof Leadershlip,unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Baruch
College, City University of New York, 1973.
15.
EVANS,
16.
Relationships, unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Yale University, New Haven, Colnn., 1968.
G. F. AND LIM, F. G., "Effects of Performance oni Leadership Cohesiveness, Influenice,
FARRIS,
M. G., The Effects of Supervisory Behavior Upon?Worker Perceptions of Their Path-Goal
17.
FIEDLER,
Satisfactioni, and Subsequent Performance," J. Appl. Psvchology, Vol. 53 (1969), pp. 490-497.
F. E., "A Continigency Model of Leadership Effectiveness," in Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances
in Experimental Social Psvchologj', Academic Press, New York, 1964.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
333
F. E., "Engineer the Job to Fit the Manager," Harvard Business Rev., Vol. 43, No. 5 (1965),
18. FIEDLER,
pp. 115-122.
, A Theoryof LeadershipEffectiveness, McGraw-Hill, New York, 1967.
19.
, Leadership,General Learning Press, New York, 1971.
20.
, "Rejoinder to a Premature Obituary," in Hunt, J. G. and Larson, L. L. (Eds.), Leadership:
21.
The Cutting Edge, Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, Ill., 1977, pp. 45-51.
22.
AND CHEMERS,
M. M., Leadershipand Effective Management, Scott, Foresman, Glenview, Ill.,
1974.
,
23.
AND MAHAR,L., Improving Leadership Effectiveness: The Leader Match Concept
(revised ed.), Wiley, New York, 1977.
, MAHAR,L. AND SCHMIDT,D., Four Validation Studies of Contingency Model Training
24.
(Technical Report 75-70), Organizational Research, Seattle, Wash., 1976.
25. FILLEY,A. C., HOUSE,R. J. ANDKERR,S., Managerial Process and Organizational Behavior, Scott,
Foresman, Glenview, Ill., 1976.
26. FLEISHMAN,
E. A., Leadership Climate and Supervisory Behavior, Ohio State University Personnel
Research Board, Columbus, Oh., 1951.
, "The Measurement of Leadership Attitudes in Industry," J. Appl. Psychology, Vol. 37 (1953),
27.
pp. 153-158.
, "Leadership Climate, Human Relations Training, and Supervisory Behavior," Personnel
28.
Psychology, Vol. 6 (1953), pp. 205-222.
, "The Leadership Opinion Questionnaire," in Stogdill, R. M. and Coons, A. E. (Eds.), Leader
29.
Behavior: Its Description and Measurement, Ohio State University Bureau of Business Research,
Columbus, Oh., 1957, pp. 120-133.
, "A Leader Behavior Description for Industry," in Stogdill, R. M. and Coons, A. E. (eds.),
30.
Leader Behavior: Its Description and Measurement, Ohio State University Bureau of Business Research, Columbus, Oh., 1957, pp. 103-119.
31.
ANDHARRIS,E. F., "Patterns of Leadership Behavior Related to Employee Grievances and
Turnover," Personnel Psychology, Vol. 15 (1962), pp. 43-56.
,
ANDBURTT,H. E., Leadershipand Supervisionin Industry, Ohio State University Press,
32.
Columbus, Oh., 1955.
33.
AND SIMMONS,
J., "Relationship Between Leadership Patterns and Effectiveness Ratings
Among Israeli Foremen," Personnel Psychology, Vol. 23 (1970), pp. 169-172.
34. GIBB,C. A., "The Principles and Traits of Leadership," J. Abnormal Social Psychology, Vol. 42 (1947),
pp. 267-284.
35.
, "Leadership,"in Lindzey, G. and Aronson, E. (Eds.), The Handbook of Social Psychology (2nd
Edition), Addison-Wesley, Reading, Mass., 1969, pp. 205-282.
36. GRAEN,G. ANDCASHMAN,
J. F., "A Role-Making Model of Leadership in Formal Organizations: A
Developmental Approach," in Hunt, J. G., and Larson, L. L. (Eds.), LeadershipFrontiers, Kent State
University Press, Kent, Oh., 1975, pp. 143-165.
37.
ANDSCHIEMANN,
W., "Leader-Member Agreement: A Vertical Dyad Linkage Approach," J.
Appl. Psychology, Vol. 63 (1978), pp. 206-212.
38. GREENE,C. N., "The Reciprocal Nature of Influence Between Leader and Subordinate," J. Appl.
Psychology, Vol. 60 (1975), pp. 187-193.
, "A Longitudinal Investigation of Performance-Reinforcing Leader Behavior and Subordinate
39.
Satisfaction and Performance," in Sikula, A. F. and Hilgert, R. L. (Eds.), Midwest Academy of
Management Proceedings, 1976, pp. 175-185.
, "Questions of Causation in the Path-Goal Theory of Leadership," A cad. ManagementJ., Vol.
40.
22 (1979), pp. 22-41.
41. GRIFFIN,R. W., Leader Behavior and Task Design, unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of
Houston, Houston, 1978.
42. GRUSKY,O., "Organizational Goals and the Behavior of Informal Leaders," Amer. J. Sociology, Vol.
65 (1959), pp. 59-67.
43. HALPIN,A. W., Manual for the Leader Behavior Description Questionnaire, Ohio State University
Bureau of Business Research, Columbus, Oh., 1957.
44.
, "The Leader Behavior and Effectiveness of Aircraft Commanders," in Stogdill, R. M. and
Coons, A. E. (Eds.), Leader Behavior:Its Descriptionand Measurement,Ohio State University Bureau
of Business Research, Columbus, Oh., 1957, pp. 52-64.
ANDWINER,B. J., Studies in Aircrew Composition: The Leadership Behavior of the Airplane
45.
Commander(Tech. Report #3), Ohio State University Personnel Research Board, Columbus, Oh.,
1952.
46. HAMNER,
W. C. ANDHAMNER,E. P., "Behavior Modification on the Bottom Line, Organizational
Dynamics, Vol. 4, NO. 4 (1976), PP. 3-21.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
334
47.
48.
49.
50.
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
59.
60.
61.
62.
63.
64.
65.
66.
67.
68.
ARTHUR G. JAGO
F. A. AND YUKL, G., "Participation? Managerial Decision-Making?
Situational Varianid
ables," OrganizationalBehaviorand Human Peiformance, Vol. 4 (1969), pp. 227-241.
HEMPHILL, J. K., "Leadership Behavior Associated with the Administrative Reputations of College
Departments," J. Educational Psychology, Vol. 46 (1955), pp. 385-401.
HEROLD, D. M., "Two-Way Influence Processes in Leader-Follower Dyads," Acad. Management J.,
Vol. 20 (1977), pp. 224-237.
HOLLANDER, E. P., "Emergent Leadership and Social Influence," in Petrullo, L. and Bass, B. M.
(Eds.), Leadershipand InterpersonalBehavior, Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, New York, 1961.
AND JULIAN, J. W., "Contemporary Trends in the Analysis of Leadership Processes,"
Psychological Bull., Vol. 71 (1969), pp. 387-397.
HOUSE, R. J., "A Path-Goal Theory of Leader Effectiveness," Admin. Sci. Quart., Vol. 16 (1971), pp.
321-338.
, "A 1976 Theory of Charismatic Leadership," in Hunt, J. G. and Larson, L. L. (Eds.),
Leadership:The Cutting Edge, Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, Ill., 1977, pp. 189-207.
AND BAETZ, M. L., "Leadership: Some Empirical Generalizations and New Research Directions," in Staw, B. M., Research in OrganizationalBehavior, JAI Press, Greenwich, Conn., 1979, pp.
341-423.
AND DESSLER, G., "The Path-Goal Theory of Leadership: Some Post Hoc and A Priori Tests,"
in Hunt, J. G., and Larson, L. L. (Eds.), Contingency Approaches to Leader-ship,Southern Illinois
University Press, Carbondale, Ill., 1974, pp. 29-55.
, FILLEY A. C. AND KERR, S., "Relation of Leader Consideration and Initiating Structure to R
and D Subordinates' Satisfaction," Admin. Sci. Qluart.,Vol. 16 (1971), pp. 19-30.
AND MITCHELL, T. R., "Path-Goal Theory of Leadership," J. ContemporaryBuisiness,Vol. 5
(1974), pp. 81-97.
JACOBS, T. O., Leadership and Exchange in Formal Organizations, Humani Resources Research
Organization, Alexandria, Va., 1971.
JAGO, A. G., "Configural Cue Utilization in Implicit Models of Leader Behavior," Organizational
Behaviorand Human Performance,Vol. 22 (1978), pp. 474-496.
AND VROOM,V. H., "Perceptions of Leadership Style: Superior and Subordinate Descriptions
of Decision Making Behavior," in Hunt, J. G. and Larson, L. L. (Eds.), Leadership Frontiers. Kent
State University Press, Kent, Oh., 1975. pp. 103-120.
, "Hierarchical Level and Leadership Style," OrganizationalBehavior and Human
AND,
Petformance, Vol. 18 (1977), pp. 131-145.
JENKINS, W. O., "A Review of Leadership Studies with Particular Reference to Military Problems,"
Psychological Bull., Vol. 44 (1947), pp. 54-79.
JOHNS, G., "Task Moderators of the Relationship Between Leadership Style and Subordinate
Responses," Acad. ManagementJ., Vol. 21 (1978), pp. 319-325.
JONES, E. E., KANOUSE, D. E., KELLEY, H. H., NISBETT, R. E., VALINS, S. AND WEINER, B. (Eds.),
Attribution:Perceivingthe Causes of Behavior, General Learning Press, Morristown, N.J., 1972.
JORGENSEN, D. O., DUNNETTE, M. D. AND PRITCHARD, R. D., "Effects of the Manipulation of a
Performance-Reward Contingency on Behavior in a Simulated Work Setting," J. Appl. Psychology,
Vol. 57 (1973), pp. 271-280.
anid Mor-aleAmiion?g
KATZ, D., MACCOBY, N., GURIN, G. AND FLOOR, L., Pr-oductivity,Super-vision7
Railroad Workers,University of Michigan Institute for Social Research, Ann Arbor, Mich., 1951.
,
AND MORSE, N., Prodtuctivity,
Super-visonand Morale in an Office Situation, University
of Michigan Institute for Social Research, Ann Arbor, Mich., 1950.
KELLER, R. T., "A Longitudinal Assessment of a Managerial Grid Seminar Training Program," Grotup
HELLER,
& Organizational
69.
70.
71.
72.
73.
74.
Studies, Vol. 3 (1978), pp. 343-355.
A. D., "Employee Reactions to Leader Reward Behavior," Acad. Managemi?ent
J., Vol. 19 (1976), pp. 619-626.
, "A Longitudinal Study of Leader Reward Behavior, Subordinate Expectancies,
AND
and Satisfaction," Personnel Psychology, Vol. 31 (1978), pp. 119-129.
AND SZILAGYI,
KERR, S., "Discussant
Comments,"
in Hunt, J. G. and Larson, L. L. (Eds.), Contingency
Approaches
to
Leadership,Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, Ill., 1974, pp. 124-129.
ANDHARLAN,A., "Predicting the Effects of Leadership Training and Experience From the
Contingency Model: Some Remaining Problems," J. Appl. Psychology, Vol. 57 (1973), pp. 114-117.
ANDSCHRIESHEIM,
C., "Consideration, Initiating Structure, and Organizational Criteria An
Update of Korman's 1966 Review," PersonnielPsychology, Vol. 27 (1974), pp. 555-568.
?
Based Upon
MURPHY, C. J. AND STOGDILL,R. M., "Toward a Contingency Theory of Leadersllip
Behavior and
the Consideration and Initiating Structure Literature," Organizational
HumanPeifor?ance, Vol. 12 (1974), pp. 62-82.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
LEADERSHIP: PERSPECTIVES IN THEORY AND RESEARCH
335
76.
KORMAN, A. K., "'Consideration,' 'Initiating Structure,' and Organizational Criteria A Review,"
Personnel Psychlology, Vol. 19 (1966). pp. 349-361.
LARSON,L. L., HUNT, J. G. AND OSBORN, R. N., "The Great Hi-Hi Leader Behavior Myth: A Lesson
77.
LEISTER, A., BORDON, D. AND FIEDLER, F. E., "Validation of Contingency Model Leadership
78.
LOCKE,E. A. AND SCHWEIGER,D. M., "Participation in Decision-Making: One More Look," in Staw,
75.
from Occam's Razor," Acad. Managemnent
J., Vol. 19 (1976), pp. 628-641.
Training: Leader Match," Acad. ManagementJ., Vol. 20 (1977), pp. 464-470.
79.
B. M. (Ed.), Research in OrganizationalBehavior, Volume 1, JAI Press, Greenwich, Conn., 1978, pp.
265-339.
LoWIN, A. AND CRAIG, J. R., "The Influence of Level of Performance on Managerial Style: An
Object-Lesson in the Ambiguity of Correlational Data," Organizational BehavioracndHutmanPerfornmance, Vol. 3 (1968), pp. 440-458.
80. LUTHANS,F. AND KREITNER,R., OrganizationalBehaviorModification, Scott, Foresman and Company,
Glenview, Ill., 1975.
81. MAIER, N. R. F., LeadershipMethods and Skills. McGraw-Hill, New York, 1963.
82. MANN, R. D., "A Review of the Relationships between Personality and Performance in Small
Groups," Psychological Bull., Vol. 56 (1959), pp. 241-270.
83. MARGERISON,C. AND GLUBE, R., "Leadership Decision-Making: An Empirical Test of the Vroom and
Yetton Model," J. Management Sttudies,Vol. 16 (1979), pp. 45-55.
84. MAWHINNEY,T. C. AND FORD, J. D., "The Path Goal Theory of Leader Effectiveness: An Operant
Interpretation," Acad. Management Rev., Vol. 2, pp. 398-411.
85. MCMAHON, J. T., "The Contingency Theory: Logic and Method Revisited," Personnel Psychology,
Vol. 25 (1972), pp. 697-711.
86. MINOR, J. B., "The Uncertain Future of the Leadership Concept: An Overview," in Hunt, J. G. and
Larson, L. L. (Eds.), LeadershipFrontiers,Kent State University Press, Kent, Oh., 1975, pp. 197-208.
87. MISCHEL,W., Personalityand Assessment, Wiley, New York, 1968.
88. MITCHELL,J. O., "Assessment Center Validity: A Longitudinal Study," J. Appl. Psychology, Vol. 60
(1975), pp. 573-579.
T. R., "Cognitive Complexity and Leadership Style," J. Personality Social Psychology, Vol.
89. MITCHELL,
16 (1970), pp. 166-173.
, LARSON, J. R. AND GREEN, S. G., "Leader Behavior, Situational Moderators, and Group
90.
Performance: An Attributional Analysis," Organizational Behavior and HunmanPeiforimance,Vol. 18
(1977), pp. 254-268.
91. MORSE,N. C. ANDREIMER,
E., "The Experimental Change of a Major Organizational Variable," J.
AbnormalSocial Psychology, Vol. 51 (1956), pp. 120-129.
92. OLDHAM,G. R., "The Motivational Strategies Used by Supervisors: Relationships to Effectiveness
Indicators," OrganizationalBehaviorand HutmanPeiforinance, Vol. 15 (1976), pp. 66-86.
93. REITZ,H. L., "Managerial Attitudes and Perceived Contingencies Between Performance and Organizational Response," in Higgins, R. B., Croke, P. V. and Varga, J. F. (Eds.), Acad. Management Proc.,
1971, pp. 227-238.
94. RICE,R. W., "Psychometric Properties of the Esteem for Least Preferred Co-worker (LPC) Scale,"
Acad. Management Rev., Vol. 3 (1978), pp. 106-118.
95. SCHRAG,
C., "Leadership Among Prison Inmates," Amer. Sociological Rev., Vol. 19 (1954), pp. 37-42.
96.
97.
98.
99.
100.
101.
102.
C. A. AND KERR, S., "Theories and Measures of Leadership:
SCHRIESCHEIM,
A Critical Appraisal of
Current and Future Directions," in Hunt, J. G. and Larson, L. L. (Eds.), Leadership: The Ctutting
Edge, Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, Ill., 1977, pp. 9-45.
AND
,"R. I. P. LPC: A Response to Fiedler," in Hunt, J. G. and Larson, L. L. (Eds.),
Leadership. The Cutting Edge, Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, Ill., 1977, pp. 51-56.
AND VON GLINOW,M. A., "The Path-Goal Theory of Leadership: A Theoretical and
Empirical Analysis," Acad. ManagementJ., Vol. 20 (1977), pp. 398-405.
R. S., "Some Theoretical Insights into the Sheridan, Downey, and Slocum Test of House's
SCHULER,
Model," in Hunt, J. G. and Larson, L. L. (Eds.), LeadershipFrontiers, Kent State University Press,
Kent, Oh., 1975, pp. 126-128.
S. C., "The Contingency Model of Leadership Effectiveness: Some Implications of Its
SHIFLETT,
Statistical and Methodological Properties," BehavioralSci., Vol. 18 (1973), pp. 429-440.
, "Stereotyping and Esteem for One's Least Preferred Co-worker," J. Social Psychology, Vol. 93
(1974), pp. 55-65.
SIMS,H. P., "The Leader as a Manager of ReinforcemenitContingencies: An Empirical Example and
a Model," in HuLnt,J. G. and Larson, L. L. (Eds.), Leadership: The Cutting Edge, Soutlhern Illinois
University Press, Carbondale, Ill., 1977. pp. 121-137.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
336
ARTHUR G. JAGO
A. D., "Leader Reward Behavior and Subordinate Satisfaction and
103. SIMs, H. P. AND SZILAGYI,
Performance," OrganizationalBehaviorand Human Performance,Vol. 14 (1975), pp. 426-438.
104. SKINNER,
B. F., Contingenciesof Reinforcement:A TheoreticalAnalysis, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New
York, 1969.
105. SMITH,B. B., "The TELOS Program and the Vroom-Yetton Model," in Hunt, J. G. and Larson, L. L.
(Eds.), Crosscurrentsin Leadership, Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, Ill., 1979, pp.
39-46.
106. STINSON,J. E. AND TRAcy, L., "Some Disturbing Characteristics of the LPC Score," Personnel
Psychology, Vol. 27 (1974), pp. 471-485.
107. STOGDILL,
R. M., Manual for the Leader Behavior Description Questionnaire-FormXII, Ohio State
University Bureau of Business Research, Columbus, Oh., 1963.
108.
, Handbook of Leadership,The Free Press, New York, 1974.
R. ANDSCHMIDT,
109. TANNENBAUM,
W., "How to Choose a Leadership Pattern," Harvar-dBusiness Rev.,
Vol. 36 (1958), pp. 95-101.
110. VALENZI,
E. ANDDESSLER,
G., "Relationships of Leader Behavior, Subordinate Role Ambiguity and
Subordinate Job Satisfaction," Acad. Management J., Vol. 21 (1978), pp. 671-678.
111. VECCHIO,R. P., "An Empirical Examination of the Validity of Fiedler's Model of Leadership
Effectiveness," OrganizationalBehavior and Human Performance,Vol. 19 (1977), pp. 180-206.
112. VROOM,
V. H., Work and Motivation, Wiley, 1964.
113.
114.
115.
116.
117.
118.
, "Industrial
Social Psychology,"
in Lindzey, G. and Aronson,
E. (Eds.), Handbook of Social
Psychology, Vol. 5, Addison-Wesley, Reading Mass., 1969, pp. 196-268.
,"Can Leaders Learn to Lead?," OrganizationalDynamics, Vol. 4 (1976), pp. 17-28.
,"Decision Making In Organizations: A Case Study in Programmatic Research," Unpublished
manuscript, Yale School of Organization and Management, New Haven, 1978.
ANDJAGO,A. G., "Decision Making as a Social Process: Normative and Descriptive Models
of Leader Behavior," Decision Sci., Vol. 5 (1974), pp. 743-769.
, "On the Validity of the Vroom-Yetton Model," J. Appl. Psychology, Vol. 63
AND
(1978), pp. 151-162.
ANDYETTON,P. W., Leadership and Decision-Making, University of. Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, Penn., 1973.
This content downloaded from 77.80.49.154 on Tue, 6 Jan 2015 19:39:36 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Download