Devel.l)meplml Bra#l Re.search. 25 (1986) 71 81 Elscvicr 71 BRD 5O34O Effects of Monocular Exposure to Oriented Lines on Monkey Striate Cortex* MARY CARLSON, DAVID H. HUBEL and TORSTEN N. WIESEL** Department of Neurobiology. Harvard ~,ledical School. Boston. MA 02115 ( U. 5'. ,4. J (Accepted August 6th. 1985) Key aords." monocular exposure - - visual experience - - monkcy striate cortex - - orientation selectivity This study examines the extent to which the restriction of visual experience to lines of a single orientation influences the organization of the striatc cortex in infant monkeys (Macaca mulatta}. Previous studies of kittens raised with monocular exposure to a single line orientation have consistently shown the response preference of cells driven by that eye to be biased towards the experienced orientation. Studies of binocular exposure to restricted orientations have been equivocal. In the infant monkcv cortex responses to oricntcd lines have virtually all the specificity of responses seen in the adult animal. In an effort to clarity the phenomenon and the mechanism by which orientation bias might be obtained, we examined the effects of monocular exposure to a restricted orientation in infant macaques. Three monkeys were used. Each monkey was raised with one open eye exposed to lines of a single orientation and one cvc occluded by lid suture. As in other cases of monocular deprivation in either cat or monkey, fcw binocularly driven cclls wcrc recorded and the majority of cells were dominated by the open eye. Cells driven bv the open eye had normal rcprcscntation of all oricntation preferences and there was no overall increase in the number of cells preferring the orientation to which the eye had been exposed. The cells dominatcd by the occluded eye, howcver, showed a lack of cells responding to orientations to which thc open cyc had bccn cxposcd. These tindings suggest that a competitive mechanism operates between the two eyes to providc an orientationselective adx antagc to the open cyc. INTRODU('TION er t h e c h a n g e s reflect shifts in o r i e n t a t i o n a l l e g i a n c e of i n d i v i d u a l cells, o r r e p r e s e n t a m e r e r e d u c t i o n in T h e o r i g i n a l e x p e r i m e n t s e x a m i n i n g t h e effects of the selectivity o r r e s p o n s i v e n e s s of s o m e cells, or a early m o n o c u l a r lid s u t u r e o n t h e a n a t o m y a n d physi- b i a s i n g of t h e fate of cells t h a t at b i r t h w e r e u n c o m - ology of t h e cat visual p a t h w a y I~.> h a v e since b e e n m i t t e d . B e h i n d m o s t of t h e s e e x p e r i m e n t s h a s l u r k e d e x t e n d e d to i n c l u d e a n u m b e r of s p e c i e s a n d d i f f e r e n t t h e old c o n t r o v e r s y o v e r t h e d e g r e e to w h i c h cortical k i n d s of d e p r i v a t i o n s . O n e of t h e m o s t i n t e r e s t i n g n e u r o n a l c o n n e c t i o n s a r e d e t e r m i n e d as a r e s u l t of v a r i a t i o n s h a d its o r i g i n w i t h e x p e r i m e n t s b v H i r s c h e x p e r i e n c e , as o p p o s e d to g e n e t i c s . a n d Spinelli; a n d B l a k e m o r e a n d C o o p e r I, in w h i c h C o m p a r e d with t h e cat, t h e r h e s u s m o n k e y at b i r t h k i t t e n s h a d vision r e s t r i c t e d to s t r i p e s of a single has a far m o r e m a t u r e visual s y s t e m . Cells in t h e o r i e n t a t i o n . Since t h e n , t h e s e e x p e r i m e n t s h a v e b e e n striate c o r t e x s h o w a d e g r e e of r e s p o n s e specificity repeated with a w i d e r a n g e o f m o d i f i c a t i o n s a n d t h a t in m a n y r e s p e c t s ( i n c l u d i n g line o r i e n t a t i o n pref- widely d i v e r g i n g r e s u l t s , f r o m f a i l u r e to r e p l i c a t e t h e e r e n c e ) a p p r o a c h e s t h a t of t h e a d u l t a n d t h e s y s t e m origimtl ( a n d very d r a m a t i c ) f i n d i n g s to c o n f i r m a t i o n of o r i e n t a t i o n c o l u m n s is a l r e a d y established-'-'. Be- a n d e x t e n s i o n of t h e m (for r e v i e w s see refs. 5 a n d c a u s e of t h e s e d i f f e r e n c e s it s e e m e d w o r t h w h i l e to re- 13). T o d a y t h e r e s e e m s to be little d o u b t t h a t by re- p e a t s o m e of t h e s t r i p e - r e a r i n g e x p e r i m e n t s in m o n - stricting e a r l y vision to s t r i p e s of a single o r i e n t a t i o n keys. W e w e r e also m o t i v a t e d by s t u d i e s t h a t s h o w o n e can c h a n g e t h e r e l a t i v e n u m b e r s o f c o r t i c a l cells t h a t in m a n e a r l y a s t i g m a t i s m gives rise to a l o w e r e d t h a t r e s p o n d to t h a t o r i e n t a t i o n . It is less c l e a r w h e t h - visual acuity r e s t r i c t e d to t h e o r i e n t a t i o n s t h a t w e r e * These results were first presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience in St. Louis, MO, 1978. ** Present address: The Rockefeller University, New York, NY 10021, U.S.A. Correspondence: M. Carlson. Present address: Washington University School of Medicine, Box g07(I, St. Louis, MO 631 ll), U.S.A. 01~'~5-38116:86/$03.5II© 1986 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. (Biomedical Division) 2 out of focus 12. In the present experiments we occluded one eye by lid suture and exposed the other eye to stripes of a single orientation. Monocular closure was used because it is known to produce, in infant monkeys, dramatic physiological and anatomical changes in eye dominance, even after brief periods of deprivation 2`~,~s. We assumed that combining lid closure with orientation deprivation would provide us with two measures of the effects of the rearing procedures. MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects Three infant macaques were raised with the left eye exposed only to lines of a single orientation and the right eye sutured for the duration of the exposure. Table I presents the age, species, and the duration and orientation of exposure. Rearing apparatus and procedures The newborn monkeys, all of normal gestational age and birth weight, were removed from the mother the day after birth. Prior to surgery for right eye closure they were housed in ordinary incubators in the Primate Center nursery unit which was lighted about 12 h/day. After right-eye suture the animals were transferred to a completely darkened, light-tight room which contained the rearing apparatus. The animal was housed in a plastic cage (30.5 x 30.5 x 30.5 cm) with a thermostatically controlled electric heating pad on the floor. A molded plastic face mask with eye holes was mounted over a 7.6-cm opening in the front of the cage. When the monkey's head was properly positioned inside the mask the entire visual field was filled by a 33.0-cm translucent, mushroom-shaped glass globe. Two electrodes were mounted in the brow area of the mask which, when contacted simultaneously, activated a Kodak carousel projector. The projector, in turn, illuminated the globe and displayed stripe patterns of a single orientation for the duration of contact. When the animal remained in contact with the electrodes for a specified duration, it received a drop of milk (SMA human formula) from a nipple mounted in the mouth area of the mask. Since the animal was in total darkness except when in contact with the face mask and electrodes, this arrangement assured a fixed orientation of the monkey's head relative to the stripe pattern stimulus. On the first day in the apparatus the animal's head was placed in the mask and held against the nipple' and electrodes to assure that it learned the correct procedure to obtain reinforcement. After repeated placement in the mask on the first day, the animal learned the location of the mask and spent increasing lengths of time with its head in the mask. Over the next few days it spent increasing lengths of time with its head in the mask without assistance. During the first week it received a single drop of milk for each accumulated 2-s electrode contact and exposure to the stripe pattern. Over the next few weeks a variable duration schedule was introduced into the programming system, which required different durations of contact to obtain a single milk delivery (variable durations of 2 s-2 rain, for an average 30-s duration). The hours of exposure obtained in the apparatus tor the 3 animals are shown in Fig. 1. Regular observations of the animal during exposure confirmed that the head was upright during the projection of the stripe patterns. Through a fiber optic viewer mounted in the eye area of the mask, it was possible to verify that the left eye was open during exposure periods. The animals were taken directly from the rearing apparatus in a light-tight box to the laboratory for physiological recordings. The projector contained 80 slides of 4 different patterns of 6-8 lines which changed automatically each minute. The lines were composed of strips of stippled transparencies which diminished in density TABLE 1 Age and condition of rearing and physiological procedures Macaque Age at right eye suture Orientation exposure Age at onset of exposure Duration of exposure Age and date at physiological recordings No. 1 M. Arctoides No. 2 M. Mulatta No. 3 M. Arctoides 23 days 12 days 6days vertical vertical horizontal 41 days 13 days 7 days 15 h (37 days) 57 h (42 days) 107 h (57 days) 78 days (April 23, 1975) 55 days (June 24, 1975) 64 days (November 13, 1975) 73 Hours No. 3 100 - 75- 50 25 No I 0 Birth ]0 20 30 40 I 50 ~0 I 70 I 80 ! 90 I ]00 the end of the recording sessions. Two or 3 penetrations were made in each animal and small electrolytic lesions ( 2 ~ A for 2 s) were made at transitional points through the p e n e t r a t i o n (e.g. right eye to left eye transition, layer 1 V c - V border). A t the termination of the recording sessions animals were perfused with saline followed by formol saline. The brains were blocked and cut in the parasagittal plane, and the 25um frozen sections were stained with cresyl violet. Finally, histological reconstructions were made of electrode tracks, all of which were well within area 17. Days Fig. 1. The graph shows accumulated monocular exposure (in hours) to stripe patterns plotted relative to age (in days) for the 3 infimt monkeys. The initial point of each curve indicates that day the animal was placed under the fully restricted routine in the rearing apparatus. and width at the ends to eliminate an obvious edge. At their widest dimension the lines were 5 - 2 0 ° in width and spaced at 1-16 ° intervals, representing a variety of spatial frequencies. Half the slides consisted of light lines on a dark b a c k g r o u n d and half of dark lines on a light background. The illumination levels were in the photopic range, dark areas measured 8 cd/m 2 and bright areas 300 cd/m 2. Physiological and anatomical methods The recording m e t h o d s used in these studies have been described previously'~, 10. A n i m a l s were kept continuously anesthetized with sodium p e n t o b a r b i tal, paralyzed with a mixture of Flaxedil and D-tubocurarine and artificially ventilated during the course of the recording session. Body t e m p e r a t u r e and expirated C O , were m o n i t o r e d and kept within normal physiological range. The sutured lids were o p e n e d and the eyes refracted by means of contact lenses upon a tangent screen 1.45 m away. Unit activity was recorded through tungsten electrodes moved in steps of 2 5 - 5 0 u m in extensive tangential penetrations in area ~0,17. The size, orientation specificity and binocular interactions of receptive fields of cells were m a p p e d on the tangent screen with a hand-held projector. The location of the receptive fields was d e t e r m i n e d relative to the projected locations of the optic discs and foveas for each eye. Two of the authors (Hubel and Wiesel) did not know the rearing orientation for the 3 animals until RESULTS The 3 experimental animals were raised under the same conditions of monocular exposure to lines of a single orientation, but the age of lid suture, onset and duration of exposure differed as summarized in Table |. The rate and total accumulation of restricted visual stimulation in the rearing apparatus are illustrated in Fig. 1. M o n k e y No. 1 had normal visual experience until 23 days of age, when the right eye was closed. Over the next two weeks the animal was put into the rearing apparatus and exposed to the vertical stripe patterns for several hours a day but was otherwise kept in a normal l i g h t - d a r k environment. The fully restricted rearing routines were not begun until 41 days of age. During the next 37 days the m o n k e y exposed its left eye to a total of 15 h of vertical stripe stimulation: the rest of the time it spent in complete darkness. Some effects of both monocular deprivation and of restricted viewing of vertical orientations can be seen in this animal although it has a history of 23 days of binocular and 18 days of monocular visual exposure in the nursery before the orientation restriction procedure was begun. All the cells recorded (excluding non-oriented cells recorded in layer IVc) r e s p o n d e d normally to oriented lines. There was, however, a decline in the n u m b e r of binocular cells and the ocular dominance distribution was skewed in favor of the exposed eye (Fig. 2A). The histogram is not unlike that found in animals raised with artificial strabismus s.2,~ or after binocular closure 21. Table II shows that of the 64 o r i e n t e d cells recorded, the left (exposed) eye was d o m i n a n t for 599} (groups 1-3), whereas the right (closed) eye d o m i n a t e d in only 74 A 30-- 25~ U 20- 15"- 10- Z 5- I ii 1 I 1 2 3 Exposed I I 4 eye I 5 6 Closed 7 II eye OCULAR DOMINANCE Hot B Or. Cells Vert. +45) 20- C Or Cells (~45) 20 15 10 10 5- I 1 1 I 2 3 I 4 I 5 6 7 3 i Exposed eye OCULAR Closed DOMINANCE eye Exposed eye OCULAR I t-'r4 I 5 I 6 CIo~*ed 7 I it eye DOMINANCE Fig. 2. Monkey No. 1 experienced a normal visual environment for the first 23 days at which time its right eye was sutured, It received 15 h of restricted orientation exposure over the next 37 days. Recordings were begun at 78 days of age. A: ocular dominance histogram for all oriented cells recorded. B: ocular dominance histogram for cells with + 45 ° horizontal orientation preference. C: ocular dominance histogram for cells with _+ 45 ° to vertical orientation preference. (Definition of ocular dominance groups: cells of group 1 were driven only by the contralateral eye; for cells of group 2 there was marked dominance of the contralateral eye: for group 3, slight dominance. For cells in group 4 there was no obvious difference between the two eyes. In group 5 the ipsilateral eye dominated slightly: in group O. markedly, and in group 7 the cells were driven only bv the ipsilateral eye. ) 75 TABLE l[ Orientation pr~ference and ocular dominance RE, right eye; LE, left eye; H-OR, ceils with _+45° horizontal orientation preference; V-OR, cells with _+ vertical orientation preference: LE-DOM, cells in groups 1-3 bins in ocular dominance histograms; RE-DOM, cells in groups 5-7 bins in ocular dominance histograms; GRP 4, cells in group 4 bin, equally dominated by RE and LE; T. total. Macaque Oriented cells H-OR cells (c;~fT) V-OR cells (C'(oJT) L E - D O M RE-DOM Group 4 cells cells cells (~ofT) (~'(ofT) ( % o f T ) H-OR Cells LE ( ~ of H) RE (¢'( of H) Group 4 ('~; of H) LE RE No. 1 64 No. 2 76 96 39 (61)* 28 (37) 54 (56)* 38 (59)* 49 (64)* 66 (69)* 13 (52)* 28 (58)* 31 (74) *,~* 11 (44) 20 (42) 11 (26) 1 (4) - No. 3 25 (39) 48 (63)* 42 (44) 25 (64) ×.~ 21 (75)* ~ 35 (65)* 14 (36) 7 (25) 19 (35) 25 (39) 27 (36) 30 (31) 1 (2) - V-OR Cells - * Highest percent for single orientation. ** Highest percent of LE dominance for either orientation. 39% (groups 5 - 7 ) . W h e n the cells were divided into two groups according to orientation preference (cells favoring horizontal _+ 45 ° and those favoring vertical _+ 45°), the horizontal group showed no preference of one eye over the other, while in the vertical group, cells d o m i n a t e d by the exposed eye exceeded those d o m i n a t e d by the closed eye by nearly 2:1 (Fig. 2C; see also Table II). The second m o n k e y was raised under similar conditions having the right eye occluded by lid suture and the left exposed to vertical stripes. The main differences in this animal were that the deprivation (the monocular occlusion and stripe exposure) began at 12 and 13 days of age and that the total accumulation of visual stimulation was 57 h c o m p a r e d to 15 h in the first instance. The activity of cells in the striate cortex a p p e a r e d normal also in this animal, as in the first one, with the usual distribution of cells responding selectively to different orientations. The cells r e c o r d e d in a single, 4-ram long, tangential p e n e t r a t i o n through the superficial layers is shown in Fig. 3. T h r o u g h o u t the penetration the orientation preference shifted in small regular steps first counterclockwise, then clockwise, just as seen in normal m o n k e y s 1° and in newborn monkeys with little or no visual experience 22. In particular there was no suggestion of any compression or expansion of areas preferring horizontal or vertical orientations, as might be expected if the rearing procedure had led to a distortion of orientation columns. There was, however, a clear decrease in the repre- sentation of the occluded eye and an increase in the areas d o m i n a t e d by the exposed eye, especially in regions of vertical orientation preference. This differential distribution of orientation preferences for individual cells is shown in Fig. 4. Fig. 4A consists of cells d o m i n a t e d by the occluded eye and Fig. 4B consists of the cells preferring the exposed eye. There was a striking paucity of cells which prefer vertical orientation in the occluded eye and a rather uniform distribution of orientation preferences in the eye exposed to vertical stripes. The few binocular cells recorded had the same orientation preference in the two eyes and those with orientations a r o u n d the vertical ( m a r k e d by numbers 186, 188 and 190 in Fig. 4B) were d o m i n a t e d by the exposed eye. A n examination of the ocular d o m i n a n c e histogram of the cells recorded in the second m o n k e y also revealed this asymmetry. As in the first m o n k e y there was a decline in the n u m b e r of binocular cells and an overall dominance of the exposed eye (Fig. 5A and Table II). This dominance was slight for cells preferring horizontal orientations (Fig. 5B), but more m a r k e d for cells with orientation preference around the vertical (Fig. 5¢'). In the third case the animal was lid-sutured at 6 days of age and entered the rearing apparatus on the following day. The m o n k e y was exposed to horizontal stripes over a p e r i o d of 57 days and accumulated a total of 107 h of exposure. Thus this m o n k e y had more stimulation to a single orientation than the others and the exposures were confined to the period of 76 s 80- - 60- / 40- t 20- I 0- -20DQg \ -40- " -60" • • C 00 7" O -80- Z O m O oo o ~OOO OO I=- <c l-z LLJ -" 80- / 60- O eee O / 40- I 20- •lle• I 0- R e -20R e ', - 4 0 - ". - 60- -80Qe ee ee 0.4 ! I ! l I I I 1 I ! I I ! I ! I I I 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 1.6 1.8 2.0 2.2 2.4 2.6 2.8 3.0 3.2 3.4 3.6 3.8 4.0 TRACK DISTANCE - turn Fig. 3. Monkey No. 2 had normal visual experience until 12 days of age at which time its right eye was sutured. On the following day it was placed continuously in the rearing apparatus where it received a total of 57 h of exposure to vertical lines over the next 42 days. Recordings were begun at 55 days of age. Graph of orientation vs tract distance for cells driven by the open eye (closed circles) and for cells driven by the closed eye (open circles) recorded in a 4-ram oblique penetration in Monkey No. 2. high susceptibility to visual deprivationJt. Of the 3 monkeys, this one showed the most marked decline in the number of binocular cells and the most striking asymmetry, again in favor of the open eye (Fig. 6A). To our surprise, however, there was little difference in this asymmetry between cells favoring orientations around the horizontal axis and those favoring the vertical axis (Fig. 6B, C). The asymmetry was slightly g r e a t e r for h o r i z o n t a l , but not so great that o n e was able to guess w h a t o r i e n t a t i o n had b e e n used for the rearing ( T a b l e ll). T o s u m m a r i z e , the m o n o c u l a r s t r i p e - r e a r i n g p r o c e d u r e in t h e s e 3 m o n k e y s resulted in a decline in the n u m b e r of b i n o c u l a r cells and a decline in the influe n c e of the closed eye. T h e decline in the effectiveness of the closed eye, seen mainly for cells r e s p o n d - 77 A B ..... i Fig. 4. Monkev No. 2. Orientation of cells recorded in the oblique penetration through striate cortex illustrated in Fig. 3. ('ontinuous lines indicate monocular cells or cells preferring the designated eye. Interrupted lines indicate binocular cells or cells prcterring thc other eve. Numbers refer to micrometer depth readings (in mm × 100). A: orientation of cells driven by the closcd cvc. B: oricntation of cells driven by the exposed c\c. ing to orientations close to the one used in the rearing procedure, was statistically significant (P = 0.02, two-sample/-tests on 3 animals combined). The difference in the effectiveness of the two eyes for cells responding to orthogonal orientations was not significant (see Table ll). DIS('LISSION The results from these monkeys make it clear that preferential exposure of one eye early in life to stripes of a single orientation can change the ocular dominance distribution of cortical cells. In till 3 animals, the population of cells as a whole came to favor the open eye, and the earlier the deprivation and the longer its duration, the greater was the shift in eye preference (Figs. 2, 3.6, parts A). Not unexpectedly, the shift was greatest for the cells having orientations at and around the orientation of the stripes used in the exposure. That this difference was least striking - - and perhaps not even significant - - in the third monkey, which had the earliest and longest deprivation, was a surprisc to us and we have no ready explanation. In the case of the first two monkeys, it might at first glance seem strange that after exposure of one eve to stripes of one orientation the distribution of cells according to optimal orientation through that eye should remain unchanged (e.g. Fig. 4B) and that meanwhile the other eye should be robbed of its control over cells having the orientation used in rearhtg (Fig. 4C). This result, however, is ,just what is predicted from the earlier simpler procedures, in both cats and monkeys, of rearing in a normal visual enviro n m e n t with one eye closed, as opposed to having both eyes closed. The monocular closure is by far the more drastic procedure, and there is now an abundance of evidence that the difference is due to competitive effects between the two eyes <t~,25. In the present experiments where, say, only vertical stripes were presented to one eye, the procedure a m o u n t e d to a binocular deprivation for horizontal and monocular deprivation for vertical. In cats very similar results have been obtained in kittens in which the rearing procedures took advantage of this competition in a similar way, with selective exposure of one eye to a single orientation, the other eye being either open or closed 15. Similar results were likewise obtained when the selective depriwition was achieved by means of artificial astigmatism produced by a cylindrical lens over one eye 4. In facL studies in which competition has played a part have consistently produced striking 78 A 50- 40U~ ..4 U4 ~J 30- UJ gB 20- Z 10- ! I Exposed ! 3 2 4 5 eye 7 6 i Closed eye OCULAR D O M I N A N C E B Or, Hot. 30- C e l l s (+_45) Vert. C u~ Or. Cells (~45) 20- 25 u~ ~J u~ 20 1510 ~0Z Z 5- 5" I I 2 Exposed 3 eye OCULAR I r-v-4 5 6 Closed eye DOMINANCE I 7 I 2 Exposed 3 eye OCULAR r--J I 4 I 5 6 7 Closed eye DOMINANCE Fig. 5. M o n k e y No. 2. A: ocular dominance histogram for all oriented cells recorded. B: ocular dominance histogram for cells with _+ 45 ° horizontal preference. C: ocular dominance histogram for cells with _+ 45 ° vertical preference. 79 A 60- u~ 50- ,.i M.I U 40" 30- Z 20- t0- F--] I! 1 I 2 3 Exposed eye ; 4 ! ! II 5 6 ! I 7 Closed eye OCULAR DOMINANCE Hor. Or. B Cells Vert. (+_45) G 4o- Or. C e l l s (+-45) 40- u') "' u.,i u 3 ..a U 30- 30- 20- 20- ~D Z Z lo- ! I 2 J 3 4 5 i Exposed eye OCULAR 6 i 7 lJ Closed eye DOMINANCE i0- q 1 2 3 Exposed eye OCULAR 4 5 6 7 ii Closed eye DOMINANCE Fig. 6. Monkey No. 3 was given normal visual experience until 6 days of age when its right eye was sutured. It was placed in the rearing apparatus on the following day. Over the next 57 days a total of 107 h of restricted exposure to horizontal lines was obtained. Recordings were begun at 64 days of age. A: ocular dominance histograms for all oriented cells recorded. B: ocular dominance histogram for cells with +_ 45 ° horizontal preference. C: ocular dominance histogram for cells with _+ 45 ° vertical preference, 8(1 abnormalities in the cortical physiology, beginning the orientation orthogonal to the rearing orientation. with the original work of Hirsch and SpinelliT, in Furthermore. in sequences of cells recorded in long which goggles produced stimuli of one orientation to one eye and the orthogonal to the other, up to recent tangential penetrations we saw regular shifts m confirmation in a more strictly controlled study by Stryker et al. z:. The results of kitten experiments in which both eyes were exposed to a single orientation have been much more varied, from the profound effects seen originally by Blakemore and Cooper~ to the lack of any effects seen bv Stryker and Sherk L~. Rauschecker and Singer i4 raised kittens with cylin- orientation, similar to those seen in newborn or normally reared m o n k e y s 1<22. This indicates that the orientation columns present at birth remained intact despite orientation deprivation in one eye and pattern deprivation in the sutured eye during the first two months of life. Thus these results give no support to the suggestion that orientation preferences are induced by visual experience. drical lenses and concluded from their study that both "selective and instructive processes" may be operating in the development of orientation preference. The wide discrepancies between all these studies remain unexplained but are perhaps not surprising in view of the variations in results that can be seen in ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The monkeys used in these experiments were raised at the New England Regional Primate Center: we wish to express our appreciation for the use of a single set of experiments - - for example Figs. 2 and 5 vs. Fig. 6 of the present paper! Despite the changes reported here, the present ex- these excellent facilities and for the generous help given by Dr. P.K. Sehgal, Ellen Newhouse and many periments in macaque monkeys provided no support for the notion that it is possible to modify innately determined orientation preference of striate cortex cells by rearing animals viewing only horizontal or others. We also wish to thank Dr. F e r n a n d o Gonzalez who programmed the schedules and Dr. Jerome Blue and Mary Russell for assistance in rearing the animals. Bea Storai and Karen Larsen did the histo- vertical lines. Cells of all orientations were recorded logy of the brain tissue and Cathy Cross and Peter Peirce the photography, for which we are grateful. in the exposed eve of these monkeys and there was no obvious preponderance of cells preferring the orientation to which the eye has been exposed. In The work was supported by NIH Grants EY0605 and EY0606 to Drs. D . H . H . and T . N . W . M . C . was sup- fact, in monkeys No. 2 and No. 3 the majority of cells (63c+ and 56C+ respectively) showed preferences for ported by NS14261 during the preparation of the manuscript. REFERENCES 7 Hirsch, H.V.B. and Spinelli, D.N., Visual experience modifies distribution of horizontally and vertically oriented receptive fields in cats, Science, 168 (1970) 869-871. 8 Hubel, D.H and Wieset, T.N., Binocular interactions in striate cortex of kittens reared with artificial squint. J. Neurophysiol., 28 (1965) 1041- 1059. 9 Hubel, D.H. and Wiesel, T.N,, Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex, J. Physiol. (LondonL 195 (1968) 215-243. 10 Hubel, D.H. and Wiesel, T.N., Sequence regularity anti geometry of orientation columns in the monkey striate cortex, J. Comp. Neurol., 158 (1974) 267-294. 11 LeVay, S.. Wiesel, T.N. and Hubel. D.H.. The development of ocular dominance columns in normal and visually deprived monkeys, J. Comp. Neurol.. 191 (198(1) 1-5 I. 12 Mitchell, D.E., Freeman, R.D., Millidot, M. and Haegerstrom. G., Meridional amblyopia: evidence for modification of the human visual system by early visual experience. Vision Res., 13 (1973) 535-558. 13 Movshon. J.A. and van Sluyters, R.C., Visual neural development, Ann. Rev. P.s3,chot.. 32 (i981) 477-522, 14 Rauschecker, J.P. and Singer, W., The effects of early visu- I Blakemore, C. and Cooper, G.F., Development of the brain depends on the visual environment. Nalure (London), 228 (19711)477-478. 2 Blakemore, C., Garcy. L.J. and Vital-Durand, F., Thc physiological effects of monocular deprivation and their reversal in thc monkev's ~isual cortex, J. Physiol. (London), 283 (1978) 223-262. 3 Cw]ader. M. and Mitchell, D.E., Monocular astigmatism effects on kitten's visual development, Nature (London). 270 (1977) 177- 178. 4 Crawford, M.L.J. and von Noordcn, G.K.. The effects of short-term experimental strabismus on the visual system in Macaca rnulatta. Invest. Ophthahnol., 18 (1979) 496-505. 5 Fregnac, Y. and Imbert, M., Development of neuronal selectivity in primary visual cortex of cat, Phv,~iol. Rev.. 64 ( 1984] 325-434. 6 Guillerv, R.W. and Stelzner, D.J., The differential effects ol unilateral lid closure upon the monocular and binocular segments of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus in the cat. .I. ('omp. Neurol.. 139 (1970) 413-422. 81 al experience on the cat's visual cortex and their possible explanation by Hebb synapses, J. Physiol. (London), 310 (1981) 215-239. 15 Singer, W., Modification of orientation and direction selectivity of cortical cells in kittens with monocular vision, Brain Res.. 118 (1976) 460-468. 16 Stryker, M.P. and Sherk, H., Modification of cortical orientation selectivity in the cat by restricted visual experience: a reexamination, Science, 190 (1975) 904-9(15. 17 Stryker, M.P., Sherk, H.. Leventhal, A.G. and Hirsch, H.V.B., Physiological consequences for the cat's visual cortex of effectively restricting early visual experience with oriented contours, J. Neurophysiol., 41 (1978) 896-9(t9. 18 yon Noorden, G.K. and Crawford, M.L.J., Morphological and physiological changes in the monkey visual system after short time lid closure, Invest. Ophthalrnol.. 17 (1978) 762-768. 19 Wiesel, T.N. and Hubel, D.H., Effects of visual deprivation on morphology and physiology of cells in the cat's lateral geniculate body, J. Neurophysiol., 26 (1963) 978-993. 20 Wiesel, T.N. and Hubel, D.H., Single-cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye, J. Neurophysiol., 26 ( 1963) 1003-1 (I17. 21 Wiesel, T.N. and Hubel, D.H., Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit rcsponses in kittens, J. Neurophysiol., 28 ( 19651 1029-1(140. 22 Wiesel, T.N. and Hubel, D.H., Ordered arrangement of orientation columns in monkeys lacking visual experience. J. Cornp. Neurol., 158 (1974) 3(17-318. 23 Wiesel, T.N., Postnatal development of the visual cortex and the influence of environment, Nature (London). 299 (1982) 583-592.