CONFLICT TACTICS SCALES CTS44 Straus, Murray A. 2007. "Conflict Tactics Scales." Pp. 190 - 197 in Encyclopedia of Domestic Violence, N. A. Jackson. New York: Routledge: Taylor & Francis Group. CONFLICT TACTICS SCALES The Conllict Tactics Scales (CTS) are the most widely used instrument for identifying domestic violence. There are two main versions of tlie CTS: the CTS2 (Straus, Hamby. Boney-McCoy. and Sugarman 1996) is the version measuring violence against a partner in a dating or marital relationship. The CTS2 has scales to nieas~irevictiinizatioii and perpetration of thi-ee lactics that are onen used in conflicts between par-tners: physical assault, psychological aggression; and negotiation; and scales to measure injury and sexual coercion of and by a pal-tnei-. The CTSPC is the version of the CTS (Stl-aus, Hainby, Finkelhor. Moore. and Ruuyan 1998; Straus and Hainhy 1997) for measuring malti-eatment of a child by parents. The CTSPC has scales to measure physical assault (with subscales for corporal punishn~entand physical abuse). pspchological aggression, and use of nonviolent discipline techniques. There are also suppleilientary questions on neglect; sexual abuse. and discipline in the past week. In this article. "CTS" will be used when tlie sentence applies to all versions of the instrument. and CTS2 and CTSPC will be used to refer to those specific versions. Both tlie CTS2 arid the CTSPC have versions for child respondents and for adult recall of tactics used between their parents (Straus website, 2006). The CTS2 has been translated into many languages. Both versions of the CTS can be obtained from Western Psychological Services. Theoretical Basis of Conflict Tactics Scales Conflict theorists such as Louis Coser and Ralph Diihreiidord argue that conflict is an inevitable and valuahle aspect of all human associatio~lbecause conflict is part of the process by which inequities and problems are corrected. What is lianufi~lis not the co~iflictiiselr. but use of coercion. including force and violence, as a tactic for resolving conflicts. In order to understand this perspective, it is necessary to distinguish be~weeiitwo closely related yet clearly different phenomena, both of which are often called conflict: "conflict of interest'. and "conflict management." When conflict theorists talk about the ubiquity of conflict. they are referring t o conflict o l interest, that is. to the fact that members oS a social group. no matter how small and intimate, are each seeking to live out their lives in accordance ~ v i t hpersonal agendas that inevitably dift'er. These differences range from the trivial, such as which TV show to watch at eight. to major life events such as whether to move to a new house. On the other hand, conflict management, o r what for purposes or tlie CTS is called "conflict tactics,'. refers to ihe method used to resolve tlie conflict. Two Families can have the same conflict but differ vastly, and with profound consequences, i n how they deal with these conflicts. One I'i~inily niighl resolve the issue of which TV program to watch by rotation. another by "first one there,'' and another by threat of force by the physically strongest. Measurement Strategy of the Conflict Tactics Scales A Behuvior.al11/Ieasur.e The CTS consists of a list of behaviors directed to~varda partner or a child. It deliberately excludes attitudes, emotions, and cognitive appraisal of the behaviors. These are crucial for some research and cliiiical purposes but must be measured separately. CONFLICT TACTICS SCALES The value of a behavioral iiistr~irnentis illustrated by a study \vIiich first asked about violent hehaviors expcrienced, rind the11 asked about cognitive appraisal. It ioilnd that more than a third of womeii who reported being victims ol' one or more violent acts did not regard themselves as having experienced "physical abuse." as a "victim of violence." or as a "battered womail" (Hamby and Gray-Little 2000). This discrepalicy between the behavior and the cognitive appl-aisal of the behavior is important for understanding Family viole~iccand for designiug progralns of prevention aiid treatme~it.However. it is possible to identify the discrepancy only if there is an instrument such as the CTS which obtains the beliavioral data. The C I S 2 questions are presented in pairs. The first question in the pair asks respondelits to iiidicate how oiten they carried out each itern in the referent period. Tile second asks how often the partner- carried out each behavior. The response categories aslc for the number of times each action occurred during the past year. ranging from "Never" to "More than 20 times." The default referent period is the past twelve months. but other referent periods, sucli as "since starting in this program," call be used. The Live CTS2 scales aud cxaniplcs of a 1ili11orarid a severe questio~iin each scale are: Plij~.sicizl/i.ssuull: "I slapped my partner." "I punched or hit my partner with solnetliing that could h~irt." I~zj~~jlli.~,: "1 had a sprain. bruise, or small cut because of 21 fight with my partner." "I needed to see a doctor because o i a fight with my partner, but I didn't." P.s~chologicii1/Iggrrssioi~:"I sliouted or yelled at my partner.'' "I stomped out of the room or hoose or yard d ~ ~ r i i ai gdisagreement." Sestlul C'oercioiz: "I insisted on sex when my partner did not want to (but did not use physical force)." "I used fol-ce (like hitting. holding down, or using a weapon) to make my partiier have sex." N~goriiztiun: ''I said 1 cared about my partner even though we disagreed.'. "I suggested a comproinise to a disagreement.'' Sj,nmn~~fr.~~ of M~~a.s~~rr,nwnt Tlie CTS measures the hehaviol- of both the respondent aiid the respondent's partiier. This does not assume symmetry in the behavior: it only makes it possible to investigate the degree of symmetry o r asymmetry. Most research and clinical uses of the CTS benefit from having data on the behavior of both partners in a rclatio~iship.This applies even when it might seem that only information on the behavior of one of the partners is needed. such as when the CTS is used to 11ieasu1-e progress in a treatment program for male batterers. Research has shown that cessation of violeilce by one partner is i~ifluenced by whether the other partner also stops hitting (Feld and Straus 1989; Gelles and Stuaus 1988). Thus, when monitoring a treatment program. it is crucial to Itnow the extent to which the partner has also ceased acts of physical and psychological aggression. In addition. for reasons that are not yet understood. tlie sensitivity of the CTS (as measured by disclosure rate) is lower in studies, sucli as the Natioiial Violence against Women Survey. that used only tlie victimimtion queslio~is. Severity L P I ~ All CTS scales measuring maltreatment have subscales for less severe aiid more severe behaviors, based on the pres~i~ned greater harm resulting from acts ill the severe subscale. The distinction between minor and severe assaults is roughly parallel to the legal disti~ictioiiin the United States between "simple assault" and "aggravated assault." That coiiceptually based classification has been supported by factor analyses and by a growing recognition tliat the etiology aiid treatment of occasional minor violeiice may he quite different than the etiology of repeated severe assaults (Gelles 1991; Hollzwortllhlunroe and Stuart 1994: Johnson and Ferraro 2000: Straus and Gelles 1990). Severity of violence is also measured by the frequency of the acts and by whether an injury results. A iiational survey of Cailadians (Laroche 2005) demonstrated that tlie CTS call be used lo identify what Johnson calls the terroristic level of violelice (Johnson and Ferraro 2000). Clinical Interpretution and Nornrs The CTS is also used for clinical assesslilent (Aldarondo and Straus 1994). Because even one instance of physical assault is a behavior that calls for remedial steps, a basic clinical assessment iiidicates whether there is a score of I or higher on the physical assault scale. I11 addition, there is information for Inany clinical and general population samples ill the CTS Maiiual (Straus, tlaniby. and Warren 2003), in the core papers on the CTS. and in illally publicatioiis by others. These rates. mean scores. and standard deviations can be used to evalua~especific cases or categories of cases. In addition to the scale scores. each CTS item should be exalllined because of tlie dill'erent implications CONFLICT TACTICS SCALES or. for example, slapping as compared with piinching; or insisting on sex compared wit11 physically forcing sex. Reliability and Factor Structure Internal Consistency Relirrbility Alpha coefficients of reliability for the CTS2. reported in forty-one articles, are tabulated in Sti-aus (2005). The coefficients ranged fro1ii.34 to .94, with a mean of .77. A study of the CTS2 in se\-enteen nations found similar results (Straus 2004). The occasio~iallow alpha coeflicient occurred when the behavior measured by some of the items, such as attacking a partner wit11 a knife or gun. was absent or nearly absent in some samples. There are less data on the internal consistency reliability of the CTSPC because this instrunient is less widely used. In the seven articles which provided reliability data. the alpha cocflicients ranged from .25 Lo .92. with a rnean of .h4. The coefficients belou: the convenlio~iof .70 are for tlie severe violence subscale and rellect the near zero rate of extremely abusive acts in some samples. Tenrporal Con.sistency Temporal consistency. as measured by a testretest correlation or intraclass correlation. is argiiably the most important aspect of reliability because IOW temporal consistency imposes all upper limit for validity. However. it is rarely repor-ted. probably because it requires testing the same subjects on two closely spaced occasions. As a result. for the CTS?. test-retest correlations have been located for only two samples. The coeflicicnts for the various scales ranged froni .49 to .90 wit11 a mean of .72. For the CTSPC. no studies were located that provide data on test-retest reli;~hility.Ilo\vever, three st~idiesprovide data 011the p a r e n t child version ofthe original CTS. The coefficients range from .49 (McGuirc and Earls 1993) to .70 and .79 (Johnston 1988) to .X0 (Amato 1991). Because the CTSPC is so similar to the original CTS, those results probably apply to the CTSPC as well. Validity Contrvtt V(11illity The steps to achieving content validity included developing the questions on the basis o r qualitative interviews and suggestions and reviews hy experienced researchers and clinicians. Each question is based around an example of the behavior being measured. such as pi~~iching a partner o r a child. For punching to be invalid, it \vould he necessary to conclude that it is not an act ~Sviolence. Like most tests. the CTS includcs only a sample of the universe of possible violent acts. This is analogous to a spelling test that includes only a sample of tlie total number of words that a child in the seventh grade should kno\v how t o spcll. Although the heliaviovs in the CTS may be valid. the nlethod used to select behaviors to include in the CTS did not guarantee that they are an adequate sample oS violent behaviors. One indication that they are a n adequate sample comes Srom a study by Dobasli and Dobash (1984). who are among the ~iioststrident critics of the CTS. They used qualilative methods to identify typical violc~lt acts. Their list of violent acts is alinost identical to the items in the CTS. Sensitivity and Confbrmding vvitlz Social De,>irubility Se/~.siiisirj. An instr~~mcnt's sensitivity is its ability to detect the occurrence oSa phenomenon. Sensitivity is a critical aspect of validity. It is especially i~iiportantfor self-report measures of socially undesirable behaviors siic11 as those measured by four oS the five CTS2 scales. When the CTS is administered according to tlie standard instriictions. it oht;~i~is many times more disclosure oS violence than the most widely used measures. such as the National Crime Victimization Survey and rates of cases reported to Child Protective Services. C o f ? / ~ u f z i / i1vitl7 f ~ g Sociul D~siruhililj,. Many studies have found lo\\. correlations between the CTS and "social desirability" scales (Sugarman and Holaling 1996). These sczlles measure the degree to u:liich respondents are reluctant to disclose socially undesirable behavior. Thc Fact that there is little correlation bet~veenscores on a social desirability scale and the CTS2 \\,as confirmed by data fi-0111the International Dating Violence Study data Sor stude~itsat thirty-one universities. This study found tlml the mean correlation \vitli a social desirability scale w a s . 17 for the physical ass;iult scale (range = .03 t o . 2 3 ) and -.O9Sol-injury (range = .OO to 4 3 ) (Straus and international Dating Violence Research Consortium 2004). These relatively low correlations suggest that scores on the CTS reflect real differences in violence. rather than differences in willingness to disclose socially l~ndesirablebehavior. Nevertheless. analysis of the CTS. like analysis of all self-report data on socially undesirable behavior. should include a control for score on a social desirability scale. CONFLICT TACTICS SCALES Agreement brtt~venRespondents Because the maill threat t o the validity oS the CTS is failure to repol-t violent behaviors that actually occurred. tile degree of agrcciiient between the reports of different participants is an important type o r validity ciata. A number of studies have investigited the degree of agreement between partners in a relationship. and bet\?-eel1data provided by pal-ents about violence Lo a child and data provided by the child. A meta-analysis of agreement between partners smnmarizing results Sroni nineteen samples which obtained CTS data from both partners and forty-three samples which obtained the daia on both partners Srorn just one of the partners found coi-relations that averaged about .50 (Archer 1999). Construct Vnli~lity Construct validity refers to tlie association between the measure i i i question and other variables for which priol- research or theory predicts a relationship. It follo\vs tliat the construct validity of the CTS caii be assessed by the degree to which the CTS produces lindings that are co~isiste~il with theoretical or enipiric;il pi-opositions about the variables tlie CTS purports to measure. There are literally 1iund1.eds of studies providi~igsuch evidence. For parent-to-child violence. see Straus and Mamby (1 997). For measures of partner violence. a few examples Srom tlie National Family Violence SUI-veysand tlie I~iternatio~lal Dating Violence Study \?-illbe mentio~ied. Many hypothesized "risk P'IL~OI-s"have been found to be related to partner violence as measured by the CTS (Gelles and Straus 1988: Strails and Gelles 1990). including: - Inequality bet\?-ecn partners. and e.specially ~ n a l edoiiiina~icc 0 Poverty and unemplo)ment * Stress and lack of community ties * Youthf~~lness * Heavy drinking Experience oS corpor:~l puiiishment as children and neglectful behavior by parents \?-ere both found in a S ~ L I o~ lY~iiiversitystudents in seventeen coullirics to be independently associated with an increased probability of violence to a dating partner as measured by the CTS (Douglas and Straus 2006: Straus aiid Savage 2005). Limitations of the Contlict Tactics Scales The CTS is both the most widely used measure oS fan~ilyviolence and also the most widely criticized. Extensive critical examination is appropriate lor ;my widely used iiistrument because, i l tlie instrument is \?-song. then a great deal of research will also be wrong. 111the case of tlie CTS. however. the most frequent criticisms reflect ideological differences rather than empirical evidence. Specilically. many feminist scholars reject the CTS because studies using this instrument tiud that about the sarnc percentage oS women as men assault their partners. This co~itradictsthe feiiiinist theory that partner violelice is almost exclusively com~iiittedby men as a means to dominate women, and is thereSore taken as prima facie evidence that the CTS is iiot valid. li-onically, the Sact that the CTS has provided some of the best evidence coiifirming the Link between male dominance and partner violence and other key aspects of fe~iiiiiisttheory of partner violence (Coleman and Sti-aus 1990: Straus 1994) has not shaken the belief that the CTS is invalid. Another iroiiy is that despite these denunciaiio~is,i ~ i a ~feiiiinist iy researchers use the CTS. However, having used the CTS. they reaSSirm their Scmiiiist credentials by routinely inserting a paragraph repeating some of the erroneous criticisms. These criticisms are then cited in other articles as though tliere were empirical evidence. Anyone reviewing these studies would have the impression that tliere is a large body of empirical cvide~ice showi~igthe illvalidity or tlie CTS. whereas there is only eiidless repelition of the same u~ivalidated opinions. Bcca~iseof space limitations. only a few examples will be mentioned arid rebi~ttcd.Others are documented elsewhere (Straus and Gelles 1990). Erroneous Cr,iticisnz.s 73, C1-S !Cfeu.s~~i~e.s O12l.1 (.'(~~!flici-Rrlritcd Vio1i.11ce. Althouqli the theoretical basis of tlie CTS is conflict theory, the ilitroductory cxpl;~natio~i to participants specilically includes expre~si\~e and malicious viole~ice.It asks respondents to answer or~estio~-is about tlie times when thev and their ~-~ partners "disagree. get aiinoyed with the other person, \?.ant different things Srom each other. o r just have spats or fighls because they arc iii a bad niooci, are tired or Sor some other reason." In the past twenty-five years this criticism h;~sbeen repeated in over one hundi-ed pi~blications,giving the appearance oS a well-established Iiniitation. However. 110e~iipiricalevidence has bee11 pro\!idcd showing tliat only conflict-related violence is reported. In f:~ct; where there are both CTS data and qualimtive data. as in Giles-Sims (1983). it sho\vs that the CTS elicits iiialicious violelice as \+,ellas conflict-related violence. CONFLICT TACTICS SCALES Eqaiatev Aci.v Tliar DiJli.7 Grrurly iiz Sei.ioiisizesi.. As shown previously in this entry. exactly the opposite is a key characteristic of tlie CTS. The physical assauli scale. like all tlie CTS maltreatment scales. differentiates betwee11 less severe acts of violence, such as slapping and tiirowing things at a partlier, and more severe acts sucli as piinchiiig, kicking. and choking, and tlie CTS provides tlie opportunity to weight the scores by the frequency of these behaviors. C<~nte.si nlid Conseyiieiice.~Are ~ I I U ~ I ' L I Tile idea that the CTS physical assaillt scale is defective because it does not take into account the context. meaning. causes. and consequences of the violent acts is a~ialogousto declaring a reading ability test invalid because il does not provide data on why a child reads poorly (such as limited exposure to books at horne or lest anxiety), or foi- not measuring tlie harmful effects of reading difficulty (such as low self-esteem or dropping out of school). Context and consequences are extremely important, but they must be measured separately from the behavior they presu~iiablycause to he able to test theories about context effects. Tliis includes information on whether the assault was in selfdefense or retaliation or was provoked by domineering behavior, verbal taunting, o r other psychological aggression. For example. because the CTS has a separate measure of psychological aggression, Murphy and O'Leary (1989) were able to test the theory that psychological aggression against a partner is associated witli an increased probability of physical violence. Some Actual Lirrritations Covei..s Oiily (I Liiiiiterl Set of C'ioleizt Acts. Tlie brevity of tlie CTS makes its use possible in situations which preclude a lo~igerinstrument. However, its brevity is also a limitation because it means that the snbscales are limited to distingi~ishi~ig minor and severe levels ol'cach of the tactics. For example, witli only eight items. the psychological aggression scale cannot provide siibscales for separate dimensions such as rejecting. isolation, terrorizing. ignorilig. and corrupting. Response C~~i<,gorie.s A1.r CSireuli.srk.. Tlie CTS asks respondents how liialiy times they and their partners did each of the acts iii tlie past year (or some other referent period). Tliis is satisfactory to provide estiliiates of how rn;~ny times severe and rarely o c c u ~ ~ i nevents g such as punching a partner or a child have happened. However, for events that can occur daily or several times a week, such as spanking or slapping a cliiltl (Gilcs-Sims. Straus. and S ~ i g a m ~ a1995), n parents cannot be expected to accurately estimate how lrany times this behavior occurred in tile past year. Nevertheless, thousa~lds of responde~its arori~id the world have provided these estimates. and these data have been successfully used to identify cases which are low or high compared with other respondents. These response categories enabled Giles-Sims (1983) to estimate that women in the shelter she studied had been assaulted ari average of sixty-nine times in the preceding year. This is more than ten times greater than the six times in the previous twelve months experienced by women in the National Fa~iiilyViole~iceSurvey who had been assaulted that year (Strans and Gelles 1990). L~~iri~,rre/~cirii~zg. Although the CTS has repeatedly bee11 fo~indto uncover higher rates of partner violence than other instruments, these rates al-e nolietheless lower-bound estimates hecaiisc of underrcporting. ln addilion. a meta-analysis (Archer 1999) found that although both men and women u~iderreport,the extent of underreporting is greater for m e ~ iPerliaps . the most serious type of underreporting is by pal-tners or victims of partners who engage in repeated severe assaults [hat often produce injuries. Although such extreme violence is only a tiny percentage of partner violence, the perpetrators and the victims of such acts are the ones in most urgent need of intervention. This problem is a iimitatioii of survey research on partner violence rather than a unique problem of the CTS. Ohtoi~7.s1ZIi1lirei1tiiir+rtDuiu ,/i~rOtilj. tlie C I I ~ ~ P I I I Partner or C,.~ii.egivrr. Tlie CTS2 asks for iiiformation ahoui relationships \villi the current or most recent partner, and the CTSPC about tlie current caregiver of the child. Thus, the CTS does not provide infor~i~ation about the history of victimization or perpetration. fr[ill'.ier h'ut Dii.ectly Litilced to A,s,snttlts. The injury scale does not provide information on which assault caused each of the injuries in the scale. Research to understand tlie processes resulting in injury could obtain this information by expanding the CTS to ask each of the illjury items for each assaultive behavior reported Administration, Testing Time, and Scoring Arlministration Experience x:ith the CTS indicates low refusal rates, even iii mass surveys sucli as the 1985 CONFLICT TACTICS SCALES National Family Violence Survey. which had an 84 percent conipletion rate (Gelles and Straus 1988). Tlie CTS can be admi~iisteredin maliy ways, including in-person intervic\v, telephone interview. self-administered questionnaire, and compuleradministered questionnaire. Studies that compared in-person with telephone interviews of the CTS have found equivalent results. A study comparing paper-and-pencil self-administered questionnaires ~r:itll compi~ter-administered questionnaires also ibuiid general equivalelicc (Hamby, Sugarman. and Boney-McCoy. 2005). There is a picture-card version of tlie CTSPC for use with young childre11 (Straus website. 2006). Testing Time The testing time ibr the full CTS2 is tc\-elve to lifteel1 minutes. A sliortcr alter~iativeis to adniinistcl- only the tIil.ee corc scales (physical assault. psychological aggression. and negotiation). This produces an instru~ncntthat has the same coverage a ~ i dtakes about tlie same time as tlie o r i g i ~ ~CTS al (seven to tell miniites). It is not advisable to shot-ten the scale by including only the victimization or only the perpetration questions. This ohlains only half of the infor~iiationneeded l o ~mdersta~id partner violence. and even for that hall: it reduces the disclosure rate. A second alternative is to use the CTS2 short form (Straus and Douglas 2004), for \vliich testing lime is approximately three minutes. Both ol' these alternatives have imporlant limitations. Tlie first alternative means iio data on injury and sexual coercion. The second alternative obtains inl'ormation oil all five scales. but at the cost of detecting only about half as many cases as when the full-lengtli scales are used. Scoring Tliel-e are many Israys to score the CTS. Each is suited to different circunistalices. They arc described in a paper on scoring (Stixus \vebsite. 2006) and in the corc p~lblications on tlie CTS (Straus et al. 1996: Straus el al. 1998; Slraus and Douglas 2004). Because of space limitation. o~ily four will he nientioned. Previileizcr. For the scales with highly skewed distributions. and for which i l is important to identify even a single occurrence of the behavior, such as tlie physical assault. injury. and sexual coercion scales, the "prevalence" score o r rate is the most usual choice. This is simply an indication ol'whet11er ally one or more ol'thc acts in tlie scale have been committed. In tlie aggregate. this results in the percenlagc who were violent. i~ijureda partner, or coerced sex. I . Tliis is the nuliiber ol'tirnes the behavior occurred in the past year. A liliiitutio~iof this score is that. Ibr general popl~lationsamples. the distribution is so skewed that the meall is not an appropriate measure of central tendency. In addition, unless a normalizing tralisformation is used. the frequency score does not meet tlie assumptions of parametric statistical tests. On the other hand. a sa~iipleof known offeiidei-s or victiins will not li;~ve 85 or 95 percent with a score of zero, and the freqilency score call be very useful for measuring the chronicity of maltreatment. Sereritj- Level irr~dMzctuiilitj Tjprs. The severity level classifies each case into three categories: iiotze, iiiiiior orzlj,. o r severe. Tlie mutuality types classify each case as re.sl~n~~ilent o~ib,. purtner oizlj., or hotli. The mutuality types may be particularly useful in conples therapy because over a hundred studies ha\'e lb~rndthat \\;hen there is violence. 50 pel-cent o r more of the time it is by both partners (Archer 2000: Straus and Ramirez ill press). Contributions of the Conflict Tactics Scales to Understanding Family Violence The twentieth anniversary cornnicmorati\~eissue of the J ~ u u i i ~i~f'fii~erp~~r.su~zu/ I Vioke17ccinclizded an article entitled "Top 10 Greatest 'Hits'" (LanghinrichsenRohling 2005). The list of hits begins, "Greatest Hit Number I: He Gavc Us a Tool to Look Behinci Closed Doors." It goes on to say, "In 1979. Strails created a measure. the C:onflict Tactics Scale (CTS). which lit a fire to the domestic violence field. The CTS was revolutionary because it allowed reseal-cliers to quailtitati\,ely study evelits that had often been ignored culturally and typically took place in private." Tlie CTS made possible natioiial surveys on the prevale~iceof f ~ ~ m iviolence ly in the Uliited States and oilier countries, such as the two National Family Violence Surveys (Straus and Gellcs 19901, the National Violeiice against Wo~nenSurvey. and the National Survey ot'Cliild and Adolescent WellBeing. Between tlie first study using the CTS (Straus 1973) and 2005, about 600 research papers ;~ndat least ten books reporting results based on the CTS were published. Betweell 1995 and 2005. foul- to six ;~rticlesreporting results obtained using the CTS \?rere published every ~iionth. Every rneas~iri~iginstrurneiit has limitations and problems. and the CTS is no exception. These CONFLICT TACTICS SCALES will? Spankitig.'. Fo,iii[i. R<~/<ifio,i.i 44. no. 2 (1995): 1711 .,,,. 176 Hamby. Sherry L.. ;nid David Finkclhor. <'iioo.shig m?r, L.siiig ('/ii/i/ Mcri,iii;utio,1 Qu<,.siioiii2oircs. W,ishington, DC: Onice oS.luvciiile Justice and Deiinijuency Prevention. 2001 Harnbg. Sherry L.. a ~ i dHer~ladetteGray-Little. .'Labeling Parlncr Violciicc: Wheii Do Victims 11iffcrcilLi;ilc among Acts'?.. I;ni/<,ilce & Victir,is 15. no. 2 (2000): 173 186. Holtzivorth-Manroe. Amy. and Grcgory I.. Stuiirt. "Typologies of Male Ba1tzrc1-s:Tlhrcc Subtypes and the Diflbrencrs among Them..' P,syclio/o~icii/Ru/l<,iiii 116. ilo. 3 (1994): 476 197. .Solinson. Miciiacl P.. and Kalhleeii J. Fcii-arm "Research Scr al\o Anal?ring Incidents of Domestic Violence: on Donicstic Violencc in tlic 1990s: Making Oistincliolls." JOLO.IILI/ (!/ ;\f'll.i.i'i~~iiiiil I/!<' Foiiiiiilj 62. no. 4 The Wational Incident-Based Reporting S?dem; (2000): 948963. Measuring Domestic Violence Sohiiston. blililred E. "Correlates of Earl) Violence Esperelice amone Men Who Arc Abiisivc toward Female h.lates.'~ In Foi,liO. dbusr oiid Its C,,,i.seyiioicc.s: !'Ve~r References and Further Reading Dii.ecrio,iz,\ iii Rm.jeoi-cli, edited by G. 1'. Hot;~ling. D. Ald;~roniio.Etiony, and Murray A. Slraus. "Screening for Finkelhor. J. T. Kirkpatrick ;~ndbl. A. Str;ius. Ncabury Physical Violence in Couple Thcl-apy: Methodological. Park, CA: S:lgc. 1988. Practi~kl.and Etlhi~tlConsiderations." )ii,iii/v Pi.oce.ss I.anghinrichsen-Rolilin~, .Iciinifer. "Top 10 Greatcst 'I-lits': 33 (1994): 425 439. Iniporiani Findings and Future Direclions for Iiiliinate A~uato.Paul R. "Psychological 0ist1-essand !he Recall of Partner Violciicc Research." .Iriiiri~ii/of Iiirri.pi.r.soiio/ Cliildhood F;lmily Characteristics.' J~~o.~~(~Iqt'i/(il.i.ii!ce I 2 I . I (2005): 108 118. tt,i'/ 11w Fowi/y 53 (1991): 101 1-1019, L;~roche.Dcnis. "Aspects oftlie Content ;lild Conseij~icnccs Archer. Johli. "Assessment of lhc Rcliabilitv o l t h e Conllici ol. Uorncstic Violence--Situ;~tio~ialCoriple Violence anif . ,I I-L.~ I C.S sc.l 'i cs: A Mela-Analytic Reviciv." ./~iurtai/o/ ltitiniate Terl-orism in Canada in 1999" Governnien! OF Itilcrpn-,so,ia/ Vi,ii,/<~iic<~ 14. no. 12 (1999): 1263 1289. Quebec. 2001. "Sex Differeiices in Aggression betivean Hetcrosc\McGuil-c. Jacqueline. and Felton Earls. "Exploriiig the ~ i a lPc~rlners:A MeV,,-An:liytic Re~iew."P.s!~~~ho/ogiciil Reliahiliiy of Measures of Family Rci~lLoos. Parental R~i//l:riii126. no. 5 (2000): 611 680. Auitudes. and Parent-Child Relations in ;I Dis;id\;inColeman. Diane I-I.. and Murray I\. Straits. "Marit:il Power. taged Minority Popul,rdon.'- .lo~ii.iiiiio/'Woi.i.i<!qeorld ilir, Conilict. and Violence in a Natioiially Repi.e::cntatiw Samfiii~lih.55 (199.3): 1042-1046. ple oF Amel-icim Couples." l i i P/~y,sir.o/Ciii/<vce in A,rin-iMurphy. Christopher M.. and K. Daniel 0'1ea1-y.'Psychoro,iF~,,iii/i<~.s. Edited by M . A. S1rar1s;lnd R. J. Gelles. Nm. 1ogic;iI Aggression Predicts Pliysiciil i\ggressioii i i i Early Bronsivick. N.1: Transaction I'uhlications. 1990. Marriage." Joiri-no/ of (:~~isu/inig u,id C/itrii.~~/ P,s~~.ho/oIkililhcrg. I.ind;i L.. Susan H. Tozil. and Chrisropher tl. pi. . . 57. no. 5 11989): 579 582. Behrens .Ilct,.siii.iirg lio/<,,~c~,-Rr/iiicd ;lffiiriO<~.s, Bc/iq/.s, 1l;~thus.Jill H.. :ind Eva L. Feindlci-. -I.ssc.ce~ii.,ri~ i f P ~ l . l i > ( v oiid B<4iiii.ioi.soriioiig Y O L ~ I /.A~ S(b,irp(,iii/iit,ii : q/ .~SSC.SSVio/cricr. W;isIiiii.~ton.TIC: American Psvcliolonical - Asi i i o i i Tuo/,s. Atlanta: Ilivision of Violcncc PI-evention. socialion. 1004. National Center for In.iury Pre\ention ; ~ n dControl. Sli-;~us. Murl-;iy A. "A General Sysicms Theory ApCciilci-s Ibl- Uiseesc Conirol and Preveiilioii, 1998. proach to :i Tlicory of Violcnce betil.ecii 1;;irniIy Dobash. Eiirerson R.. a ~ i dRussell P. Dob;ish. "The Nature Members." SoriiiI .S<.iivice i,!fiil.iiiaiio,i 12. no. 3 (1973): and Anie~~edeots of Violent E~ciits."Bi.iiii/i .loui.,ai/ of I 0 5 125. ('riiiii,ro/o~,~ 24. no. 3 (1984): 269 288. Bih/io:ri~j~/z~ o,id Tcih~r/<ti. S!oiiiiioi.y ~/'Piih/icorio,u Donplas. Emily M.. and Murray .A. Stlaus. "Ass;~iiil itnd on lire Rei,i,s<,d Cii,i/licf I . S<.ii/l:.s ICTS2 oild Injury of Dating Partners by University Students in I9 C T S P C J . Durham. NH: Family Research Lahouatory. Nations and its Relatioti io Corporiil I'~i~iishmentEnpeUniversity of New Hsmpsliire. 2005. rienced as a Child.' Z~or,/,eoii ./l~lii'ilii/,;/'Ci.i,iii,io/o~j3. . ivcbsitc. lit!~:;'~,uhoa~es.~~nli.cdu.-mas2 lacccsscd . , . no. 3 (2006): 293-318. August 15. 2006). Fcld. Scott 1..atid Mui-ray A. Sli.ai~s. -'Escalation and Straus. bl'liirray 4.. and Emily M . Doiiglas. "A Short Form Desislaiice of Wife Assa~iltin Marriuee:' ('riiiiiiio/~i~s of the Revised Conilict Tactics Scalcs. arid Typologies ,,. 27. no. 1 (1989): 141-~161. for Sexerity il~ndM~ii~itlity..'I,'io/.ilce i i i i i / Vicriiir.~19. Gclles. Richard J. "Physical Vioiencc. Chili? Abuse. ;tiid no. 5 (2004): 50: 520. Chiid Homicide: A Contiiiu~irnof Vioicncc, or Distinct S t ~ i u s h.lurl-;iy . i\.. and Riciiard J. Gelles. Phj..sicii/ IV?<,/e,ii.iicr, tlclia~iors?" fl~aiirni;\'<ifiii-e2. no. I (1991): 59-72. in liiici-icuii F~iiiiiIi<,.s:Ri.s/< FOCIO~.S uiid Adc~~~iiiiioii.~ 10 Gelles. Richard J.. and Murra\. A. Straits. lirriiiiiir<.li,ii,l-io/<~,i.iii.e iu 8.145 i;ii,iri/ics. Nclv Brunsaick. NJ: Trans/~iil.e.Ncx? Yoi-k: Siiiion 6: Schusler. 1958. action Publications, 1990. Giles-Sims. Jeail. lt'ifi, BIIII?I.III~: I,. Sysro,rs Tlrrors Slrsus. MLII-rayA.. ~iiidSheri-y L. tiamby. 'Measuring 4p/roric/r. New York: Guilfoi-d Press. 1983. Phlsical and Psychological R.laltrraioisnt of Cliiiiireii Giles-Sims. Jean. Mitrl-~iyA. Straus. and David B. Sugirmaii. with the Conllicl Tactics Scales." 111 Otii qiihe D~ii~I~ireis: "Chiid. Ilatcrnsi and Family Cli;rracteristics Associated ('ii~iii~~iiporai). R<,.s<,o,*.hPci-ii,ccriac,soil F~,,,ii/j Violeiicr, limitations need to bc considered !?;hen interpreting r c s ~ ~ lSrorn t s thc CTS. or when choosing an instrument to measure hiiiily violence. Fortunately. there are comprehensive c o m p e ~ ~ dwhich ia describe over 100 measures of diffel-ent aspects of violencc (Dahlbel-g, Toal, and Behrens 1998: Halnby and Finkelhor 2001; Rathtis and Feindler 2004). They racilitate cxainini~lgalter~~atives to the CTS or choosing additional instruments to measure aspects of violence that arc not covered by it. MIJRR.AY A. STRAGS CONTROL BALANCE THEORY AND DOMESTIC VIOLENCE edited h > G .Kui~finanKuiitoc-and J. Jxsinski. Thotisand Oaks. CI\: Sage. 1997. Straus, Murriiy A , Sherry L. Hamhg. SLISIII BoncyMcCoy. and David 8. Sugarman. "Tlic Revised Conllici Ti~cticsSfales (CTSZ): Develouincni and Preliminarv Psychonictric Data." .I,,rii-~ia!qfFri,~iiljissiie.s 17. no. 3 (1996): 283 316. Straus. Mmray A,. Sherry I-. Hamby. David Finkclhor. Dayid W. Moore, and Desmond Rungan. "lde11tific;ition of C:liilil bl;~llreatme~~t with ilic Parent Child Coilllict Tactics Scalcs: Development and Psychometric Ilaia for a N;ltional Sample of American Parents.'. Ciiilii Ahiisc o,id h'<ygI~i.r22 (19'8): 249 270. Straus. Murray A,. Sherry L. H a i i ~ b >and , %'. Loaise W;urein "Statc-to-Stale DiKcrcnccs in Social Inciluality and Social Bonds in Rclalion to Asi:~ults 011 Wives in lhe United States." Jo~ir.,~oI 01' C;~i,~~,oi-utii,e Eirniili Studie.i 25. no. 1 (1994): 7 24. - . T / I PConllicr 7bctics Srol<,.s Ilcrrziihook. Los Anecles: Wcstcrn Psychologifal Services. 2003. -~ - "Cross-Cultural Reliiibility and Validity of the Revised Conilia TLlctics Scalcs: A Study of University Srudcnt D;iting Cuiiples iii 17 N;itions." (~ro.s.s-('~~Itur.~d R~scorclz38. no. 4 (2005): 407432. Str;ius, Mu1:ray A , . and I~~tcrnaiioilal Dating Violence Rcsearch Consorti~mm. "Prevalence of Violcrlce Xgainst Dating Partners hv Male acid Female Uni~ersiii.StudenlsiVorld\vide." IVi<io!e,icr ogiihisr CIIOITIEII 10..no. 7 (2004): 790 4 1 1. Stelus. Murray A.. and Isnacio 1,uis Ramirez. "Gender Svmmctry in Prcv~ilencc,Severity. and Chronicity o l Pliysicd Aggression agaiilw Dating Partners by Uiiiinsily Slodcnis in Mexico ~ l o dUSA." A,yxit.,~,sii.eB<,/iiii.ioi.,in prccs. Straus. Murray A,. and Silrah A. Savage. "Ncgiectful Bchtivior by I'al-slits in rlic Life History of University Stodents in 17 C'ounlUes and Its Rclaiion to Violei>ce a ~ a i n s t1)atine Partilcrs." Ciiil~i;I(aiti~~~iiri,ir,~t 10. no. 2 (20053: 124 135. Sugarman. Das'id B.. and Gerald T. H o l i ~ l i o ~ "Intimate . Violc~iceand Social Ilcsirability: A Meta-Analytic Rcvic\t'.-' J~,~,rno/<!/'I,!ter{><,r,?o~~o! l,'i<i/e~!cc12, 110.2 (19963: 275 290. b - - - - CONTROL BALANCE THEORY AND DOMESTIC VIOLENCE introduction Domestic violence, or intimate partner viole~lce (IPV). is a conteniporary social problcrn that has evolved from a husband's legal right to discipline his wife through physical nieans (Lutze and Syn~ons 2003: 321). Historically; thejudicial system protected the right oS the husband; however, as the ~vomcn's movement gained i~ifluence,the courts began to trcat IPV as tlic sei-ious and pervasive problem that it is (Lutze and Symo~is2003: 321.324). While st~idics sho\%:that there arc specilic g o u p s who arc victimized with greater frecluency tban others-[or example. women who arc n ~ e ~ n b e rof s minority g r o ~ p s .or those who live in urban arcas (U.S. Department of Justice 1998: 13-15). IPV is not exclusive. that is. it can affect anyone, regardless of age, sex, culture. socioeconomic status, or race. Therefore. society must continue to develop efkctive means to address violence between partners. Before solutions can be Sound. the etiology of the probleni innst be understood. In the case of this critical issue. criminological thcories should bc applied t o better understand IPV and how best to .. control it. This article applies Charles Tittle's control balance theory to occurrences of dornestic violence; in doing so. it sccks io expiain nor only inslances of IPV, bnt also victirns' responses to thc violence that thcy are experiencing and suggests possible means oS addressing IPV. Statement of Problem On March 28. 2003. in a case that garnered widespread media altcntion in Austin. Texas. Ortralla Mosley was stabbed to death on her high school campus, and her ex-boyfriend Marcus McTear was accused oS the crinle (Gilbert 2003). H e was later sentenccd to a forty-year determinate sentencc (Smith 2003). In a 2003 r l ~ , s t i ~AI?!CI.~CQIZ ! St~/e.s~iii~~! arliclc discussing IPV (Gilbert 2003). Veranda Escobar mas profiled. She survivcd her riolent rclationship. but not before it left her confined to a \\:lieelchair. In 2002. Michael Edward Hill was slabbed to death by his girlfriend in what appeared to be a n attcmpt by the isoman to defend hersclf CONFLICT TACTICS SCALES i i m i t a t i o ~ l s~ i c c dt o be coiisidercd w h e n i n t e r p r e t i n g results from t h e C T S , or w h e n c h o o s i n g an instrum e n t t o m e a s u r e family violence. F o r t u n a t e l y . t h e r e are coliipreliensive c o t n p e n d i a wliicli describe o v e r 100 measul-es oT different a s p e c t s o S violeiice ( D a h l b e r g , Toal. a n d B e h r e n s 1998; H a i n b y and F i n k e l l ~ o r2001: R a t l i u s a n d Peindiei- 2004). T h e y f,n c '~.h t a t e cxamiiiing alternatives t o t h e CTS or c h o o s i ~ i ga d d i t i o ~ i a instrtkinents l t o measure aspects of v i o l e ~ i c ct h a t arc n o t covered b y it. MVKIIAY A . STR.\I!S See ai\o Analj~ingIncidents of Domestic Violence: The National Incident-Bayed Reporting S)stem; Measuring Domestic 17iolence References and Further Reading Aldal-oiido. Etiony, 2nd Mi1r1-ayA. Stlitus. "Screeiiing ibr Physical Violence in Couple Therapy: Methodological. PI-acrical. and Ethical Consideratioils." / i i n i i / i Pi.occ.s.s 33 (1994): 425 439. Arnato. Paul R. "Psvcholo.~ic;~l llistrcss and the Rcc~tllof . Cl~ildiioodFamily CIi:iraclcristics:' .li~ui',iiiiqfJlwi-ioxe i F i r 5 ( I I I: 0 I 1019. Archer. J o h ~ i"Assessmcilt . oi'the lteli;ibilit\- o i l b e Conllict ,iitics . ' , .5..i'i I,.. t\. A Meta-Analytic Revieiv." J~,ri,rii-,iiilof l ~ ~ i ~ ~ r / ~ ~ ~Viol<,!ic', r , ~ , i t z14, i i / no. I2 (1999): 126.3 ~1289. -. "Sex Differences iii Agpressioii between Heterosesu;rl Partners: A Meta-Analytic Rcvien,.'. P\.i~c/ro/o,yini/ B u / / c ~ i i126. i ~ no. 5 (2000): 651~~680. Colcmaii. Dialie 1-1.. and Murmy A. Straus. marital Poiver. Conllict. and Violence in a Nation;illy Rcpl-eszntativc Sanipie o l Amzrican Coitples." In P i ~ ~ . s i rlio/<,,ice ul in .-lnirr-icuii lis?zi/i<~.s, Editcd by M. A. Stvaus and It. J. Gelles. New Bn~nswick.NJ: Transaction Publications, 1990. Dahlbcrg. Linda L.. Susan B. Toiil. ;riid Christopher 13. Hclircns. .M<raiii-hi,y V i . i o / ~ ~ i ~ r - R cAiriiiiil~~.s. / ~ , r ~ ~ ~ Bc/ic/.\. / =, oiid Bclitiviol.; oitioiix youth.^: .4 <h,,iix~ir</iroizi!/'.l.rsrs.rtitriil Toii/.s. Atlcinta: I>ivirion of Violeiicc Prc~cntion. Nitional Center fol- Injury Preveiilioil and Coiltroi. Centers lor Discasc Control and I'revciition. 1998. Dohasli. Emerson R.. and Russell P. Doh;~ili.'Tlie Nature riiid Anteceilents of Violent Events." Bi-iii.sll .lo~ii.,iii/(if Ci.i~i~i,iii/ii,yj, 24. no. 3 (1984): 269-288. Douglas. Emily M., and Murray A. Slraus. ~'.\ssaiiIl anil li~.ii~ry ol.Dating Partners by Llni~crsityStudents i n I9 Nations and Its Relation to Corporal Puiiisl~mei~l Espericnced as a Child." Eni-oiicoii Jo~ii7iii/U/ Ci.ii~iiz010gj..3. no. 3 (2006): 293-3 18. Feld. Scott I... and Murray A. Straus. "Escalatioii and Desistance or Wife Assault in Marriapc..' Ci.ii,iXio/oci. " .,. 27. no. 1 (1989): 141-161. Gellcs. Richird J. "Phi.sica1 Violence. Child Abuse. and Child Homicide: ,2 ~ o n t i n u i i mof Violmce. or Distinct Beha\-iors?" ffiiiiiiiii ~ V t i i ~2.o .110. ~ i (19911: 59 72. Gclles. Richiird J.. and Murfiiy ,I. Stiaus. Iiithiiutc Vi<>/i.i~c.r. Ncii- York: Simon & S~.liusrer. 19x8. Gilcs-Sims. Jean. Wife Boiici-iiig .A S~.sie,.i,i.s Tiieoi). Ap,~rooch. New Sorl<:Guilli~i-dPress. 1983. Giics-Sinis. Jcan, bturruy A . Straus. and David B. Sugarmaii. -'Child. Mateniai iind Fxniiy Cliar;rcterislius Associlitcd with Spanl~in::' Fc,irii/j Rc1~riioii.s 44. no. 2 11995): i 70 176 Hiirnby. Sherry L.. tilid David Finkclhor. (,'/~oo.si,ig iiilii C,:shig C'iriid T~'icti,iikiiii~iiiQric,siionniiii.<,.s.Washingloi~, DC: Oflice ol~.luvcnilcJustice and Dclinqucncy I'revention. 2001 Hamby. Sherry L.. and Bernadette Gray-Little. "Labeling I'ariner Vioicilcc: Wlrrii D o Victims Difiereniiatc among Acts?" Violiizc(, cY l'irti,~i.s 15. nil. 2 (2000): 173-1 86. Holtru.orth-Munroe. Aiiiy. :ind GI-egory L. Stuart. "Typologics of Male Battereis: Three Subtypes and the Differ-. ences among Them." P ~ ~ i . / i o / o , q i iBu//eiiii ~u/ 116. no. 3 (1994): 476 497. Johnson. Michael P.. and Kathlccil J. Fcrraro. 'Reseal-c11 oil Llomcsric Violence i n rhc 1990s: Making Dislinctioils." .IOlli.ilii/ O/ Iri(ii.i.i(rg(, iiiid tire F'ii~ii/y 62. no. 4 (20001: 948- Yh3. Johnston. Mildred E. "Correlates of E:irly I'iolence Expercncc arnonf Mcn Wlio Arc Abusive toa.al-d Fernale Mares." In Fu,i?ih. l h i i ~ euni/ Its C~iii.s?i/iieiir.<i:Xwt. I1irri.iioir.s i ~ iRr,seoi-d~,edited by G . T. Hoialing. D. Finkclhor. J. T. Kirkniltrick and M.i\. Slraris. Newbun. Cry: Sage. 1988. Laneliiiiricliscii-Rol~li~~e. Jennifer. .'Toi> I0 Gre:ttest 'Hits': " Important Findings and Futuis Dircciioris for lnlimate Pi~rtrier Violence Research." Jo~rr.tio/"f liiirrpei..so,io/ t"io/nicc 20. no. 1 (2005): 108 118. I,ai-oche. Denis. "Aspects o l t h c Contest and Conscilucnces o f Domestic Violellee--Sililatio~i;~lCouple Violeiice anil Illtimate Terrorism in C,iriada in 1999." Government of Quebec. 2005. McGuir. Jacilueiine. iind Felton Earls. "Exploring liie Reliahilily o l Mcasures o i F:~mily Rcltitons. I'arental Atlitudcs, and Parent-Child Relations in a Disadv.rilcaged Minority Popul;~tion.".Iniii.iirrii~/'.Wiii.i~i~igc mid !lie Fri,iii/)' 55 (1993): 1042l016. Murph). Christopher M.. ii~idK. Daniel O'Leary. "Psychologiml Agpression Predicts Physical 4ggrcssion i n Early Marriage." Ji>~ii.,zo/of <:oiii~tiihigoticl C/inicu/ P l i . c / i o / ~ c j , 5:. no. 5 (1989): 579 5 8 1 l < ~ t t l ~ Jill i ~ s ,H., and Evil I-. Feintllcr. A,~,s~~.s,s~~i~~rii ofPuri,ier Violiiici~.W~~sliington. 13C: A~liel-icanPsychological 4 s sockation. 2004. S t s r A. "A Geocfiil Systems Theory Approasli to a rhcory of Violence betu.eeil Family Members." Social S<.ioiccIii/,,niiiiiioii 12. inn, 3 (1973): - ., .-... Il>i~-l?i , B i / ~ / i ~ ~ ~ uric/ r c ! [TUIIL~/U,. ~ i i ~ S r t , ~ , m , ~of j l't~/~ii~~~lio~~. ihr R<,i,isni Coiiflici 7iicii<:i Scu/~~.sICTS? iind CT.YP(,'j. Durham. NH: Family Rese;ircIi Lc~boratory. oii Uilivtrsily of New H~~mpsliire. 2005. wehsitc. Iirrp:~.1~iih~,agc~.~nh.cduI-niits2 (acccsscd August 15. 2006). Strnus. Murray A.. and Emily M. Ilougias. ' A Sliol-t Form ol' the Rcviscd Conllict Tactics Sc.iles. and T ~ n o l "o ~ i e s for Se\'eriry 2nd Muruality.' I,'io/~.iiceii,ri/ lVitiii~i.s 19. 110. 5 (2004): 507 520. Stiaus. Murray A . :ind Kicli:~rdJ. Gelles. P/ij~.sicoil"io/i,izc~ iii i,iiwicnii F i i ~ ~ i i / i <Ri,s/i ~ s : Fncior.~uiid ,zl~/~ij,iiiiioii.s lo i,'iolei1cr in 8.145 F c ~ ~ ~ i i /N i ~c~n. sBrunsivick. . N.1: Tra~isacrio:~Publications. I99O. Straus, Mi~rfily A . and Sherry L. Hamby. "Measuriiig Physical and Psychologiciil M;~llreatment oS Childre~i with the Colillict T:idics Scales." In Oui i,/'ihr Doi-hn~.r.s: < ' ~ ~ ~ i r ~ ~ ~Rcreorch i i ~ x ~ i . uP~~i-,sjircrii~~.s r,~ o,, l-iiiijili. Vio/<,iicr,