Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 www.elsevier.com/locate/foodqual Exploring consumersÕ perceptions of local food with two different qualitative techniques: Laddering and word association Katariina Roininen *, Anne Arvola, Liisa Lähteenmäki VTT Biotechnology, P.O. Box 1500, FIN-02044 VTT, Finland Received 28 September 2004; accepted 29 April 2005 Available online 28 June 2005 Abstract In recent years, a growing number of consumers in Finland have started to show interest in the origin of the foods they eat. Although the concept of local food has been launched to describe food produced near the consumer, it is not yet well-defined and consumers may understand it in different ways. The aim of the study was to establish the personal values, meanings and specific benefits consumers relate to local food products by comparing two different qualitative interview techniques: laddering and word association methods. Product names, presented as cards for participants, were used as stimulus material. In the word association (n = 25), four product categories (general term, fresh pork meat, marinated pork slices, and pork sausage) and of four types of production method or production location (locally, organically, conventionally and intensively produced) were presented. In laddering (n = 30), the production methods were the same as in the word association method, with the exception that there were only two product categories, instead of four. The content analysis of the participantsÕ responses resulted in very similar categories in both studies, such as ‘‘quality’’, ‘‘locality’’, ‘‘vitality of rural areas’’, ‘‘short transportation distances’’, ‘‘freshness’’, and ‘‘animal wellbeing’’. Only the laddering study, however, revealed cognitive structures, i.e., links between such constructs as ‘‘short transport’’ and ‘‘animal welfare’’. Word association was found to be an efficient and rapid method for gathering information on consumer perceptions of local foods. Laddering interviews, which were time-consuming and required laborious analysis, provided us with important information on the relationship between perceived attributes and the reasons for choices. 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Local; Organic; Laddering; Word association 1. Introduction Interest in the origin of foods and in the production method has increased among Finnish consumers in recent years. The concept of local food has been launched to describe local food systems or short food chains where the food is produced near the consumer; this could contribute to rural development and labor markets to promote local economies (Urban–Rural Interaction, 2001). This concept is not, however, well-defined * Corresponding author. Tel.: +358 9 708 58724; fax: +358 9 708 58212. E-mail address: katariina.roininen@vtt.fi (K. Roininen). 0950-3293/$ - see front matter 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2005.04.012 yet, due to which it can mean different things to different consumers. It is not clear what consumers exactly appreciate in local foods and whether these valuations differ from those that they associate with organic foods. In order to promote marketing possibilities for local foods, it is important that we understand how consumers perceive the concept of local food and what advantages or disadvantages and values they relate to the concept. In the case of a new concept, qualitative methods are suitable tools for revealing how consumers view and perceive that concept. Word association techniques, which are commonly applied in psychology, are qualitative methods that could serve as quick and convenient K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 tools in exploring consumer perceptions for new and undefined concepts such as local food. Word association may be less laborious than many other qualitative methods, such as personal interviews. Most importantly, indirect associative techniques are able to grasp affective and less conscious aspects of respondentsÕ mindsets better than methods that use more direct questioning (Szalay & Deese, 1978). Based on the expectancy-value theories of Ajzen and Fishbein (1980), the most salient associations or beliefs that the consumers has about the attitude object in question are the best predictors of the consumerÕs behaviour related to that attitude object. Thus, the associations that first come to the respondentÕs mind are the ones that should be the most relevant for consumer choice and product purchase. Slovic et al. (1991) and Benthin et al. (1995) have demonstrated an application of a word association technique where respondents are asked, not only about their associations, but also to score their responses as regards to their valuations. Thus, the method applied in this fashion provides both a qualitative understanding of the beliefs behind the attitudes as well as quantitative estimates of the attitude valence (unfavourable/favourable). At its best, this method could provide fast and convenient tool for exploring the motives behind food choice. At its worst, though, it could provide results that are shallow and difficult to interpret. Laddering interviews are another qualitative method that can provide both the perceptions of the local food concept and a comprehensive investigation of the structure of the concepts that are relevant for the respondent. It provides a method that can capture the salient attributes of product choices, which then lead to the benefits and values that these attributes signify to a person. It is based on the means-end theory, which is a model of the consumersÕ cognitive structures that focuses on how product attributes (the ‘‘means’’) are linked to self-relevant consequences and personal values (the ‘‘ends’’) (Olson, 1989; Reynolds & Gutman, 1988). Laddering provides a rich and useful understanding of consumers perceptions of products and the basis for their purchase decisions. The advantage of the laddering technique over other qualitative approaches is that the meanings in a means-end chain are personally relevant; therefore, laddering could provide results that are more closely related to preference and choice behaviour (Olson, 1989). This approach has previously been shown to be useful tool in analysing consumer behaviour in the food domain (Baker, Thompson, & Engelken, 2004; Grunert et al., 2001; Makatouni, 2002; Nielsen, Bech-Larsen, & Grunert, 1998; Roininen, Lähteenmäki, & Tuorila, 2000; Urala & Lähteenmäki, 2003). The aim of this study was to establish the personal values, meanings and specific benefits consumers relate to local food products by comparing word association and laddering methods as elicitation techniques. 21 2. Materials and methods 2.1. Participants This study consisted of two parts: word association and laddering interviews. Both parts were conducted in two different locations, Mikkeli and Espoo, in Finland. Mikkeli is a small town in eastern Finland and Espoo is part of the Helsinki Metropolitan Area. Espoo was chosen to represent an urban area that is far from production and Mikkeli to represent as location that is in the middle of a rural, agricultural area. For the word association interviews (n = 25; 15 participants from Espoo and 10 from Mikkeli; 40% of which were males and 60%, females), the mean age of the participants in Espoo was 39 (range: 19–68) and in Mikkeli, 42 (range: 18–68). In the laddering interviews (n = 30; 15 participants in Espoo and 15 participants in Mikkeli; 54% of which were males and 46%, females), the mean age of the participants in Espoo was 49 (range: 18–67) and in Mikkeli, 44 (range: 20–64). 2.2. Word association The applied method relies on the word association method demonstrated by Slovic et al. (1991) and Benthin et al. (1995). It involves presenting subjects with a target stimulus and asking them to provide the first thoughts or images that come to mind. In this study, the target stimuli were written descriptions of pork meat products. The descriptions were combinations of three types of products with varying levels of processing (fresh pork meat, marinated pork slices, and pork sausage) and of four types of production method or production location (locally, organically, conventionally and intensively produced), such as ‘‘locally produced sausage’’ or ‘‘conventionally produced marinated pork slices’’. In addition, the four production methods were presented as general descriptions without any reference to a specific product, e.g., ‘‘organically produced food’’. Each of the (n = 25) respondents were shown (4 · 4) 16 cards, one at a time, in a random order. The respondents were asked to write down the first four images, associations, thoughts or feelings that came to mind. After going through the 16 stimulus descriptions, the participant was asked to rate each association that he or she had written on the questionnaire on a scale from 1 (very negative/bad) to 5 (very positive/good). At the end, the participants answered a few background questions about their age, gender, education, and familiarity of terms local and organic food (on scale from 1 = very unfamiliar to 5 = very familiar). The data were collected in Espoo and Mikkeli during autumn 2003. In both places, the interviews took place in coffeehouses. After completing the task, each respondent received a gift voucher, ranging in value from EUR 22 K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 5 to 8, depending on the place, to the coffeehouse where the interview took place. 2.3. Laddering interviews In laddering interviews, the types of production methods or production location (locally, organically, conventionally and intensively produced) were the same as those in the association task, with the exception that only two product categories were used (fresh pork meat and marinated pork slices). Both product categories were presented to each participant. The order of the food category was randomised. The one-to-one interviews were divided into two parts: sorting (part A) and the main interview (part B). The session started with an explanation of the study tasks that emphasised that ‘‘there were no right or wrong answers’’ to the questions, and that researchers were only interested in the participantsÕ opinions. In part A, participants were instructed to sort the foods by their choice priority. In the main interview (part B), participants were asked about the reasons for their first choice compared to other choices. They were then further asked ‘‘why is this reason important to you?’’ The laddering process continued in this manner until the participant could not produce any further information. This was then repeated for their second, third and last choice. After part B, the participants were asked to fill out a background questionnaire on their age, gender, education, and their use frequencies of the target foods. The interviews took between 30 min and 1 h to complete. The participantsÕ responses were recorded on pre-prepared answering sheets by the interviewer during the course of the interview. In addition, the interviews were tape-recorded. In Espoo, the interviews took place in two coffeehouses and in Mikkeli, in a room provided by the YTI Research Centre. After completing the task, each respondent received a small gift (in Mikkeli, which was worth EUR 6), a cinema ticket (worth EUR 7) or a gift voucher (worth EUR 6) to the coffeehouse where the interview took place (in Espoo). 2.4. Data analysis 2.4.1. Word association Quantitative estimates of the attitudes towards the stimuli were obtained by averaging the ratings of the four images that the respondent had provided for each stimulus. Normality of the distributions was tested by one sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality test. All variables used in the analyses were normally distributed based on this test. Therefore, parametric tests could be used for these analyses. Repeated measure analyses of variances were used to compare these attitudes (or image ratings) between the stimuli. Comparisons were made (independent within subjects variables) between the production methods/production distance (locally produced, organic, conventionally produced, intensively produced) and product type (raw pork meat, marinated pork slices, sausage), with the interview location (Espoo, Mikkeli) acting as the subject variable. In addition to mean ratings, we also calculated the percentages of associations that gave a positive or negative score for each stimulus. The software used for the quantitative analyses was SPSS.12.0 for Windows (2003). The elicited associations were qualitatively analysed by means of content analysis. The analysis proceeded sequentially from the initial search of recurrent themes to the classification of each elicited association into one of the 32 categories. Finally, these categories were reduced to the 18 categories presented in this article. Ten was used as the reduction cut-off, i.e., the association need to be mentioned at least 10 times when all the products were combined. 2.4.2. Laddering interviews Information collected during the interviews was content analysed. In the content analysis, the attributes and consequences having the same meaning were grouped together and each group was labelled. These groupings formed a scheme that was used to categorise the attributes and consequences. Once the content analysis had been performed, the laddering data, e.g., the attributes and their consequences, were aggregated and interpreted through hierarchical value maps (HVM), which are graphical representations of the most frequently mentioned links summed across all subjects. These graphic maps were generated using MecAnalyst software (Skymax Inc., Italy) which is tailored to analysing means-end chains. The number of links between the concepts reflects the complexity of the map and the strength of the line illustrates the number of participants that mentioned the link. The software forms individual chains and then analyses the number of links across the study population. A cut-off level of four was chosen, except in the case of conventionally produced food, where a cut-off level three was selected. A cut-off level of four means that a link is drawn between two concepts if at least four of the participants mentioned one concept as a direct or indirect link to another concept. 3. Results 3.1. Word association In word association, term local food was regarded familiar (values 4–5 on familiarity scale) by 80% of participants from Mikkeli and 20% by participants from Espoo. There was no difference in familiarity of organic food between the two locations. K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 For all product categories, locally, organically and conventionally produced foods received mostly positive associations. On the other hand, intensively produced food evoked mostly negative associations. Freshness, short transport, security as well as contribution to local economy and viability were associated with local food production. The transparency of locally produced food 23 was considered good. This was expressed in associations such as ‘‘knowing what theyÕre getting’’, ‘‘knowing where the food came from’’, and ‘‘trustworthiness’’. The only negative association that was related to locally produced food was price, which was considered to be high. Expensive pricing was, however, more often associated with organically produced than locally produced Table 1 Association categories and examples of individual associations Category Examples Local Organic Conventional Intensively Total Wholesome Healthy (l, o), low-fat (l, o), unhealthy (c, i), high-fat (c, i) 6 1; 0; 5 16 0; 1; 15 13 10; 2; 1 10 10; 0; 0 45 21; 3; 21 Clean No additives (l, o, c), no preservatives (o), additives or preservatives can be added (c, i), more additives (i), preservatives (i) 5 0; 2; 3 32 0; 2; 30 11 6; 3; 2 15 13; 2; 0 63 19; 9; 35 No pesticides No pesticides (o), pesticides can be used (c), may contain antibiotic substances (i) 0 0; 0; 0 12 0; 1; 11 3 2; 1; 0 13 0; 0; 13 28 2; 2; 24 Organic, natural Organic (l, o), natural (o), not organic (c), unnatural (i) 7 0; 2; 5 17 0; 1; 16 5 1; 3; 1 7 7; 0; 0 36 8; 6; 22 Quality, taste Do not affect taste (l), good taste and quality (l, o, c), conventional (c), bad taste and quality (i) 38 4; 2; 32 49 0; 1; 48 44 14; 12; 18 43 34; 3; 6 174 52; 18; 104 Fresh Fast delivery (l), fresh (l, o, c) 20 0;0; 20 3 0; 0; 3 2 1; 0; 1 0 0; 0; 0 25 1; 0; 24 Conflict between processing and production The idea of local product is spoiled by the marinade (l, o), marinated product cannot be organic (o), negative effect of marinade (c) 4 1; 3; 0 13 5; 8; 0 5 1; 4; 0 0 0; 0; 0 22 7; 15; 0 Inform and security Trust (l, o), origin is known (l), good quality control (c), lack of confidence (i) 16 0; 5; 11 1 0; 0; 1 9 4; 2; 3 13 9; 2; 2 39 13; 9; 17 No added value of production method Does not affect buying decision or food choice (l, i), quality product is enough (o) 15 1; 5; 9 2 1; 0; 1 4 0; 4; 0 2 1; 1; 0 23 3; 10; 10 Availability, selection Availability not good (l, o), not many alternatives (o), conventional food (c, i), limited selection (o) 14 3; 4; 7 16 9; 4; 3 17 2; 4; 11 7 0; 4; 3 54 14; 16; 24 Price (cheap) Expensive (l, o), good buy (c, i), special offer product (i) 11 8; 2; 1 34 28; 6; 0 18 3; 4; 11 18 0; 2; 16 81 39; 14; 28 Animal welfare Animal welfare (l, o), awareness of animal treatment (l, o, c), animal welfare is ignored (i) 8 0; 0; 8 19 0; 0; 19 2 0; 0; 2 19 19; 0; 0 48 19; 0; 29 Affects viability of countryside Affects viability of countryside (l), good for economy (l), creates unemployment (i) 20 0; 1; 19 0 0; 0; 0 0 0; 0; 0 1 1; 0; 0 21 1; 1; 19 Transport, costs Low transport costs (l), long transport (i) 11 1; 0; 10 0 0; 0; 0 0 0; 0; 0 1 0; 0; 1 12 1; 0; 11 Local Local product (l), produced locally (l, o), industrial product (i) 29 0; 2; 27 3 0; 0; 3 0 0; 0; 0 0 0; 0; 0 32 0; 2; 30 Large production unit Large production units (l, o, i), profit seeking (i) 2 0; 0; 2 0 0; 0; 2 18 2; 11; 5 60 38; 14; 8 80 40; 25; 15 Small production unit, traditional Homelike (l, o), self-produced (o, c), traditional (o, c) 19 0; 2; 17 32 0; 3; 29 24 0; 3; 21 0 0; 0; 0 75 0; 8; 67 Products Products of Portti in Mikkeli (l), vegetables (o), convenience foods (c), chicken (i) 8 0; 3; 5 8 0; 0; 8 16 3; 5; 8 13 8; 1; 4 45 11; 9; 25 In each column, below the total number is number of negative, neutral and positive associations. (l) = local, (o) = organic, (c) = conventional and (i) = intensive. 24 K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 food. In general, organically produced food aroused more associations such as the purity (no added additives or very few) of the product and the production method, wholesomeness, good taste and quality, animal welfare and small-scale production than locally produced food. Good quality, affordable price, easily available, unhealthy, impure and industrially produced were associations that were more often connected with conventionally produced food than locally produced foods. Most of the terms evoked by intensively produced food were considered negative such as cruelty to animals, pesticides, unhealthy, unnatural and large-scale production. Intensively produced food was, however, considered to be cheaper and better in quality than local food (Table 1). Value scores were significantly different for the different production methods when the all 16 terms were included in the analysis (F(3, 69) = 69.4; p < 0.001) (Fig. 1). Although the number of associations linked to the organically produced food was higher than those for locally produced food, the value scores of organically produced food were lower compared to locally produced food generally speaking. Interaction between the production method and degree of processing was observed (F(6, 138) = 4.9; p < 0.001). The value scores of organically produced food were lower when the degree of production was higher (F(2, 46) = 8.6; p < 0.001). In the case of intensively produced food, foods that were more processed received higher value scores than foods that were not processed (F = (2, 46) = 4.3; p < 0.05). The degree of processing did not significantly affect the scores of locally and conventionally produced foods and no difference was observed between pork sausage and marinated pork slices. The interview location did not significantly influence the association value ratings. Locally produced Organically produced 3.2. Laddering In laddering interviews, term local food was regarded familiar (values 4–5 on familiarity scale) by 67% of participants from Mikkeli and 40% by participants from Espoo. Whereas, 73% of participants from Mikkeli and 67% of participants from Espoo regarded organic food familiar. Choices in the sorting part (A) varied between different production methods and interviewing locations but not between products. Therefore, the choice data of products is combined. In Mikkeli 33% of participants chose local food, 55% organic, 13% conventional and 3% intensively produced food as their first choice. Whereas, in Espoo 30% of participants chose local food, 33% organic, 33% conventional and 3% intensively produced food as their first choice. The maps of locally produced food noticeably differed between the two locations (Fig. 2a and b). In both, the short transportation distance was given as a reason for preference, leading to fresh product quality and the Finnish origin of products creating a sense of security. In Mikkeli, however, the local food was mostly considered a way to support local production and to create economic welfare in the area; whereas, local food was linked with animal welfare, environment and health in Espoo, which was quite similar to their perception of organic products (Fig. 3a). Moreover, the attribute-consequence links were more concrete in Mikkeli than in Espoo; for example, in Mikkeli, short transportation distance was related to good taste, lower price, freshness and saving money. In Espoo, this was related to animal welfare and respect for nature. Local food was perceived similarly to organic food in Espoo (Figs. 2a and 3a); in Mikkeli, the perception was Conventionally produced Intensively produced 5 4.5 Value score 4 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 1 General term Fresh pork meat Marinaded pork slices Pork sausage Fig. 1. Affect scores of four types of production methods (locally, organically, conventionally and intensively produced) with four types of products (general term, fresh pork meat, marinated pork slices, and pork sausage). K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 25 Cut-Off=4 Well-being Environment stays clean Creates no waste Avoid diseases Quality of life Animal welfare Does not spoil Creates employment Respect for nature Good for health Creates security Like to support Stays fresh Can do alternative activies Short transport Finnish Familiar a Cut-Off=4 Common good Subsistence wages Creates employment Affects viability of countryside Stays fresh Tastes good Creates security Like to support Avoid diseases Lower price Local Short transport Finnish Saves money Improves quality b Fig. 2. Hierarchical value map of locally produced foods (data from fresh pork meat and marinated pork slices combined): (a) for participants from Espoo and (b) participants from Mikkeli. close to that of conventional production (Figs. 2b and 4b). The overall picture of organic products in the hierarchical value maps is similar in the two towns with animal welfare, good taste and respect for nature included on the map. The ladders describing conventional production are, however, dissimilar in the two locations. For Mikkeli, the value map is a complex picture with many attributes that link conventional production to health, nature and well-being and the map is comparable to that of local food. In Espoo (please note the cut-off point of 3), the conventional production is considered to be practically neutral, not expensive and an easily available option. Intensive production is perceived with parallel negative attributes in Espoo and Mikkeli (Fig. 5a and b). Intensive production means cruelty to animals and is bad for health. The hierarchical value maps also have very simple structures indicating that respondents perceived intensive production in a stereotypic manner. 26 K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 Cut-Off=4 Balance of nature Respect for nature Well-being Recreactional value of environment remain Enjoys Avoid diseases Good for health Animal welfare Creates no waste Chemicals do not cumulate Fresh Environment stays clean Tastes good Improves quality Animalfriendly Organic animals a Cut-Off=4 Quality of life Improves quality Well-being Happiness Pleasure Enjoys Will not purchase Avoid diseases Animal welfare Chemicals do not cumulate Tastes good Good physical condition Expensive Organic animals Clean Organic Good quality b Fig. 3. Hierarchical value map of organically produced foods (data from fresh pork meat and marinated pork slices combined): (a) for participants from Espoo and (b) participants from Mikkeli. 3.3. Comparison of word association with laddering results Both the association and laddering methods gave similar descriptions for locally produced food such as positive effects on food quality, locality, viability of local areas, short transportation distances, freshness, and animal well-being. With word association, the number of consequences from eating and buying these foods were fewer than in laddering and the links between attributes and consequences, e.g., association between short trans- portation distances and animal well-being, were only obtained with the laddering technique. 4. Discussion In the present study, locally produced food was considered to support the local economy, was related to short transport distance, freshness and trustworthiness of its origin regardless of the elicitation method. The results are congruent with a previous focus group study K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 27 Cut-Off=3 Can do alternative activities Saves time Easy Can afford Saves money Price not expensive Good availability a Cut-Off=3 Pleasure Can do alternative activies Respect for nature Well-being Enjoys Can afford Animal welfare Avoid diseases Saves money Good for health Familiar Awareness of animal treatment Know what you get Is important Lower price Clean Safe Animalfriendly Price not expensive Large production unit Good quality b Fig. 4. Hierarchical value map of conventionally produced foods (data from fresh pork meat and marinated pork slices combined): (a) for participants from Espoo and (b) participants from Mikkeli. about ecolabel prototypes that was carried out in three locations in Iowa, USA. In that study, it was found that for consumers, ‘‘freshness’’ and ‘‘supporting family farmers’’ were the two most important reasons for buying local foods (Pirog, 2003). Organically produced food evoked more associations related to purity (no added additives or very few) than locally produced food did. Getting clean food or food free from pesticides or other chemicals have also been found to be common benefits associated with organic food in earlier studies (Baker et al., 2004; Grunert & Juhl, 1995; Huang, 1996; Mathisson & Schollin, 1994; Tregear, Dent, & McGregor, 1994). Moreover, healthi- ness, and animal welfare were more associated with organically than with locally produced food. Organically produced food, however, evoked more negative associations, such as that it was more expensive than locally produced food. In addition, processing was not considered appropriate in the case of organically produced food. Moreover, in many other studies, the main benefits consumers have been found to associate with organic foods (or indicate as their reasons for buying them) are related to health, taste and environment; these are among the most commonly mentioned characteristics or purchase motives of organic foods, regardless of the country that the study took place in (Baker 28 K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 Cut-Off=4 Animals are stressed Will not purchase Bad for health Creates suspicion Taste deteriorates Cruelty to animals Not animalfriendly Not clean a Cut-Off=4 Develop diseases Taste deteriorates Animals are stressed Chemicals cumulates Cruelty to animals Profitseeking Not animalfriendly Deteriorates quality Not clean Large production unit b Fig. 5. Hierarchical value map of intensively produced foods (data from fresh pork meat and marinated pork slices combined): (a) for participants from Espoo and (b) participants from Mikkeli. et al., 2004; Granqvist & Biel, 2001; Hill & Lynchehaun, 2002; Magnusson, Arvola, Koivisto-Hursti, Åberg, & Sjöden, 2001, 2003; Roddy, Cowan, & Hutchinson, 1996; Saba & Messina, 2003; Tregear et al., 1994; Wandel & Bugge, 1997; Zanoli & Naspetti, 2002). Differences between local and organically produced foods are congruent with an earlier study by Winter (2003). This study consisted of face-to-face interviews with 736 residents from five regions in England and Wales. Fifty four percent of the respondents claimed that they made regular, weekly purchases of foods produced by local farms. The reasons that they gave for the purchases of local food were related to supporting the local farmers and the local economy, freshness and knowing where their food was coming from. On the other hand, health, food safety and environment were stressed more often as the reasons for buying organic food. In the laddering results, a huge difference between the constructs of the hierarchical value maps were observed between participants from the rural area (Mikkeli) and participants from the urban area (Espoo). Participants from the rural area were more interested in supporting the local economy than participants from the urban area were. This is in accordance with earlier findings in the UK (n = 734), where rural consumers gave higher priority to ‘‘civic’’ issues in food choice and showed higher interest in local foods than urban consumers did K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 (Weatherell, Tregear, & Allinson, 2003). The reason for this might be that participants from the rural area are closer to sources of food production. Therefore the concern of local economic issues might be greater. Both association and laddering methods gave similar descriptions for the foods produced. As expected, word association provided a useful approach for examining the images and outcomes participants associated with local food. This is in accordance with the study of Benthin et al. (1995), who found word association to be a useful method for eliciting positive and negative affects associated with the behaviour of adolescents. The laddering technique, however, provided an overview of the characteristics of locally and organically produced foods as well as information on why these characteristics are important to the participants. Laddering has been found to be a very useful method in understanding the self-relevant consequences consumers attach to food products (Baker et al., 2004; Grunert, Grunert, & Sørensen, 1995; Grunert et al., 2001; Nielsen et al., 1998). Moreover, the differences between rural and urban participants were mainly observed by the laddering technique. Differences between the degree of processing was observed when the association technique was used; the same was not observed with the laddering technique. In the laddering the participants sorted different production methods by their choice priority and this was done separately for both products that different in processing level therefore they might paid more attention in comparison of different production methods than comparison of different level of processing. Word association was found to be an efficient and rapid method to gain information about consumer perceptions of local foods. Product attributes may, however, not be sufficient by themselves and the link they have to one or more desirable or undesirable consequences is more important. Laddering interviews were time-consuming and the analysis of laddering data was a laborious and relatively slow task. Laddering is, though, a useful tool when the purpose is to get self-related information on the attribute–consequence–value associations that consumers hold about a product. The limitation of this study is that only small number of respondents were used in both methods. Therefore, generalization of the views consumers have about local and organic foods is complicated. However, this study gives good insight about advantages and disadvantages of associations and laddering methods when these methods are used for getting information about the new concepts. The association method was found to be fast and useful in situations when information about product attributes is sufficient themselves. When association method was used, it was not possible to get values and links between attributes, consequences and values. Because association method was faster, more respondents could be used, and this would make the results more eas- 29 ily generalized. Laddering, in turn, was found to be more time-consuming than association method. However, it gave more information than association method. Te use of laddering would be recommended in the situation when deep information about the concept is needed. Acknowledgements This study is part of the Consumers, decision makers and local or organic foods. Possibilities for SMEs project supported by the Ministry of Agriculture and Forestry of Finland. References Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting behavior. Engelwood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. Inc. Baker, S., Thompson, K. E., & Engelken, J. (2004). Mapping the values driving organic food choice: Germany vs the UK. European Journal of Marketing, 38(8), 995–1012. Benthin, A., Slovic, P., Moran, P., Severson, H., Mertz, C. K., & Gerrard, M. (1995). Adolescent health-threatening and healthenhancing behaviors: a study of word association and imagery. Journal of Adolescent Health, 17, 143–152. Granqvist, G., & Biel, A. (2001). The importance of beliefs and purchase criteria in the choice of eco-labeled food products. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21, 405–410. Grunert, K.G., Grunert, S.C., & Sørensen, E., (1995). Means-end chains and laddering: An inventory of problems and an agenda for research (Working paper No. 34). Aarhus: The Aarhus School of Business, MAPP. Grunert, S. C., & Juhl, H. J. (1995). Values, environmental attitudes, and buying of organic foods. Journal of Economic Psychology, 16, 39–62. Grunert, K., Lähteenmäki, L., Nielsen, N. A., Poulsen, J. B., Ueland, O., & Åström, A. (2001). Consumer perceptions of food products involving genetic modification—results from a qualitative study in four Nordic countries. Food Quality and Preference, 12, 527–542. Hill, H., & Lynchehaun, F. (2002). Organic milk: attitudes and consumption patterns. British Food Journal, 104(7), 526–542. Huang, C. L. (1996). Consumer preferences and attitudes towards organically grown produce. European Review of Agricultural Economics, 23, 331–342. Magnusson, M., Arvola, A., Koivisto-Hursti, U.-K., Åberg, L., & Sjöden, P.-O. (2001). Attitudes towards organic foods among Swedish consumers. British Food Journal, 103(3), 209–226. Magnusson, M. K., Arvola, A., Koivisto Hursti, U.-K., Åberg, L., & Sjödén, P.-O. (2003). Choice of organic foods is related to perceived consequences for human health and to environmentally friendly behaviour. Appetite, 40, 109–117. Makatouni, A. (2002). What motivates consumers to buy organic food in the UK? Results from a qualitative study. British Food Journal, 104(3/4/5), 345–352. Mathisson, K., & Schollin, A., (1994). Konsumentaspekter på ekologiskt odlade grönsaker—en jämförande studie (Consumer aspects on organic vegetables—a comparative study) (No. 18). Uppsala: Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet, Institutionen för växtodlingslära. Nielsen, N. A., Bech-Larsen, T., & Grunert, K. G. (1998). Consumer purchase motives and product perceptions: a laddering study on vegetable oil in three countries. Food Quality and Preference, 9(6), 455–466. 30 K. Roininen et al. / Food Quality and Preference 17 (2006) 20–30 Olson, J. C. (1989). Theoretical foundations of means-end chains. Werbeforschung & Praxis Folge, 5, 174–178. Pirog, R. (Ed.). (2003). Ecolabel value assessment consumer and food business perceptions of local foods. Iowa: Leopold Center for Sustainable Agriculture and the Iowa State University Business Analysis Laboratory. Reynolds, T. J., & Gutman, J. (1988). Laddering theory, method, analysis, and interpretation. Journal of Advertising Research, 2, 11–31. Roddy, G., Cowan, C. A., & Hutchinson, G. (1996). Consumer attitudes and behaviour to organic foods in Ireland. Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 9(2), 41–63. Roininen, K., Lähteenmäki, L., & Tuorila, H. (2000). An application of means-end chain approach to consumersÕ orientation to health and hedonic characteristics of foods. Ecology of Food Nutrition, 39, 61–81. Saba, A., & Messina, F. (2003). Attitudes towards organic foods and risk/benefit perception associated with pesticides. Food Quality and Preference, 14, 637–645. Slovic, P., Layman, M., Kraus, N., Flynn, J., Chalmers, J., & Gesell, G. (1991). Perceived risk, stigma and potential economic impacts of a high-level nuclear waste repository in Nevada. Risk Analysis, 11, 683–696. Szalay, L., & Deese, J. (1978). Subjective meaning and culture: An assessment trough word associations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tregear, A., Dent, J. B., & McGregor, M. J. (1994). The demand for organically-grown produce. British Food Journal, 96(4), 21–25. Urala, N., & Lähteenmäki, L. (2003). Reasons behind consumersÕ functional food choices. Nutrion & Food Science, 33(4), 148–158. Urban–Rural Interaction (2001). Report of the Working Group of Urban–Rural Interaction, Ministry of the Interior, Helsinki. Wandel, M., & Bugge, A. (1997). Environmental concern in consumer evaluation of food quality. Food Quality and Preference, 8(1), 19–26. Weatherell, C., Tregear, A., & Allinson, J. (2003). In search of the concerned consumer: UK public perceptions of food, farming and buying local. Journal of Rural Studies, 19, 233–244. Winter, M. (2003). Embeddedness, the new food economy and defensive localism. Journal of Rural Studies, 19, 23–32. Zanoli, R., & Naspetti, S. (2002). Consumer motivations in the purchase of organic food. A means-end approach. British Food Journal, 104(8), 643–653.