Polish Academy of Sciences, Institute of Genetics and Animal Breeding, Jastrzębiec, Poland Animal Welfare Improving welfare status of animals in different phases of production chain Impact of handling, attitude to animals and personal traits of the stockman on animal welfare and human safety Definition of handling According to Webster`s Encyclopedic Unabridged Dictionary of the English Language (1989) the term “handling” may be defined as “the manner of treating or dealing with something”. In farm animal husbandry this term is relevant when close relation between domestic animal and the stockman takes place. Generally, there are direct and indirect relations between humans and farm animals. The narrower meaning of “handling” (direct relations) refers to the physical contact initiated by the stockman when manipulating, touching, stroking, or during veterinary interventions etc. Handling is indispensable in all phases of animal production chain. Humans not only have impact on animals due to physical contact, but also through designing of animal facilities and planning husbandry systems. Each type of housing system is related to specific handling. The direct human involvement in animal handling is not always necessary. In modern husbandry systems, mechanical, automatic or electronic devices are being developed to replace more and more the stockman at the laborious or time consuming husbandry activities (mechanical manure removal, automatic feeding and watering, milking robots, etc.). Some kinds of handling (e.g. health care, animal marking) need tools or devices of special design. Tendency in modern animal husbandry In modern animal husbandry stockpeople/farmers have more animals to care of and less time to spend taking care of individual animals due to increased working load and other activities. On the other hand, farm animals are less dependent of and accustomed to direct contact with humans and become less familiar to the human presence and handling. Main husbandry procedures in which handling problems may occur involve: milking, restraining an animal to the veterinary intervention, loading and unloading animals for transportation, pre-slaughter handling, moving large groups of domestic herbivores to the pasture, training of horses etc. Fear of humans The handling is very important for animal welfare, because the presence of human in close proximity to animal and human’s behaviour directed to animal can be aversive 1 Polish Academy of Sciences, Institute of Genetics and Animal Breeding, Jastrzębiec, Poland or positive to animal. Through the domestication process the fear of humans was generally attenuated in domesticated animal (Belaev, 1969) but this opinion is only partially true. In fact, humans may easily evoke fear in domestic animals particularly when animals are raised in groups with the little or no contact with man. This fear of humans occurs with various intensity (from restlessness to panic escape). Experiments have shown that direct human contact of the stockman with animals can be very important for the development of young animals. Even a brief but systematic contact of the stockman with pre-weaned or weaned young animals, including stroking, talking to and gentle handling of animals, often results in better growth rate, better cognitive abilities (e.g. learning capacity) and more balanced reaction to subsequent stressors (Hemsworth, 2004). On the base of the kind, intensity and frequency of previous contacts with human, an animal can differently perceive humans i.e.: as danger or predator eliciting flight/fight reaction) as food- and water-giving environmental object as social partner to whom an animal is trying to establish social dominance or becomes submissive. • as indifferent environmental object which is ignored by animal (Estep, Hetts, 1992) • • • The idea of the stockperson acting as a social partner for farm animals is based on two concepts. (1) The concept of social dominance According to most practitioners, the animal handler should behave and act as the dominant member of the animal herd or the leader of the herd. According to a more anthropomorphic approach, the handler should be perceived by animals rather as the “friend” than dominant herd-mate. (2) The concept of imprinting or socialization Imprinting can be considered as an attachment phenomenon that will be created during sensitive period in animal’s early life. This attachment is expressed later on, as filial or sexual preferences. Originally, the concept of imprinting was developed by K. Lorenz (1935) and concerned birds. The imprinting process was viewed as irreversible. For mammals a concept of socialization was proposed instead of imprinting. Socialization creates reversible social relationship (including the human partner) and is mainly based on the process of attachment and adjustments to the other member of the social group, especially in particular sensitive periods of the animal’s life. Socialization will occur mostly in orphan young animals which are fed by stockman from bottle. The phenomenon of socialization may have both positive effect (no fear of human, ease of handling) and negative effect (too much bond to humans, ignoring 2 Polish Academy of Sciences, Institute of Genetics and Animal Breeding, Jastrzębiec, Poland herdmates of animal’s own species, and no respect of human, dangerous behaviours towards humans). The fear of humans may be differentiated among animal species and is especially expressed in poultry, pigs and sheep (Fraser, Broom, 1997.) The fear reaction is often aggravated by other environmental stimuli interpreted by animal as fear-provoking. Not only painful experiences but all the housing conditions and husbandry procedures which are perceived as aversive and stressful by the livestock, often evoke the serious troubles during handling. Animals often demonstrate fear of novelty. Measuring fear in animals Some tests have been developed for measuring the fear reaction of animals towards human (Boivin et al, 2003): (1) (2) (3) Recording reactions of animal to the man staying motionless (approach or no approach, and latency to approach are noted. Recording of animal’s reaction when it is restraint and the man is approaching the animal (estimating stress hormones and heart rate). Estimation of the “flight distance” as the radius of space around an animal which intrusion provokes flight reaction (Fraser, Broom, 1997). Environmental and genetic factors of fear reactions The results of test measuring fear of man in animals are influenced by genetic and environmental factors. Reaction to humans may be enhanced by novelty of situation or by stress evoked by social isolation. The flight distance in beef cattle is longer than in dairy cattle which is probably caused by genetic factors, and, dairy cattle kept under traditional system seemed to be less fearful towards man than animals kept on pasture which can be attributed to environmental factors (Murphy et al., 1981, Boivin et al., 1991). The inter-breed differences and genetic variability as well as a possibility of selecting animals that are better adapted to being handled, should be taken into consideration for improving handling and welfare of animals during handling. However, the estimated heritability coefficients for traits related to “docility” were rather moderate (h2 = 0.2 - 0.3). Despite of thousands years of domestication and elimination of the most reactive animals, a substantial variability still exists in traits related to reaction of animals to humans. The interaction between rearing environment (individual experience) and genetic make up is probably responsible for the great variability of behavioural traits related to reactivity of animals to humans. 3 Polish Academy of Sciences, Institute of Genetics and Animal Breeding, Jastrzębiec, Poland Effects of handling There are many experimental data showing negative effects of harsh or brutal treatment of animals on their productivity. Aversively handled pigs have a lower pregnancy rate and lower weight gains. On the other hand, the corticosteroids level was higher when animals were handled in a harsh way (Hemsworth and Barnett, 1987). The way animals are handled, can have not only impact on animal performance or fear of people. Inconsistent of brutal treatment of animals (especially males) can trigger aggressivity towards stockpeople. Vaarst (1997) found that the incidences of kicking where stockpeople were harmed by cows, varied between 3 and 29% in 10 dairy herds in Denmark. The differences in the quality of human behaviour towards animal were probably very important factor in this case. Personality traits of a good stockman Positive influence of stockperson on the milk yield in dairy cows was reported as early as in the beginning of seventies (Seabrook, 1972). The same author evaluated the optimal personality traits of the stockperson, which may positively influence animal productivity (Seabrook, 2000). A good stockman should act consistently, rewarding wanted behaviour of an animal and providing pleasant physical contact with it. As a whole, the action of the stockperson directed to cattle should have a form of positive active interaction „involving use of the voice in a calm manner, patting, stroking and touching of the animal, particularly the head area and around the ears”. This behaviour has the positive effect on the milk production. The stockman should be: emotional stable, confident, independent, satisfied with his/her job, motivated, having high self-esteem, relaxed, rather introvert and shrewd. Calm and self confident stockperson scarcely faces defensive reaction from animals. An unsure, inexperienced or nervous person may have difficulties when handling animals. If a stockperson is excitable, unbalanced or has a tendency to be violent, he/she has a greater chance of inducing dangerous reactions from animals. The importance of the “body language” of animals The prominent researcher of animal handling problems T. Grandin (2004a), on the basis of own investigations and experiences, prepared guidelines for better handling of the domestic livestock in various situations. She stressed the fact, that animal’s experience in new surroundings should be positive one. The stockperson should not appear suddenly to the animal, avoiding startle reaction. For effective and safe handling of animals, a stockperson should know the so called “body language” of animals (postures and gestures signalling animal’s intentions towards man or animal’s mood). 4 Polish Academy of Sciences, Institute of Genetics and Animal Breeding, Jastrzębiec, Poland Animals can precisely signal if their attitude to the approaching person is: • • • • • aggressive, submissive, friendly, curious, being afraid. Each animal species has its own repertoire of body language signals. Forms of animal learning playing a role at handling As results of a systematic handling throughout longer period of time, behaviour of animals changes gradually due to the process of learning. There are two main types of learning that take place during handling: habituation and operant conditioning. Habituation takes place when animals are exposed many times to a stimulus that appears in neutral context. The frightful reaction to this stimulus will gradually extinguish, if this stimuli is neither aversive nor positive to the animal. As a result e.g. human who is evoking frightful reaction may become to an animal simply a part of its environment and the fear reaction may extinct. (2) Operant conditioning takes place when a desirable reaction or behaviour of an animal is reinforced by rewarding the animal (e.g. giving a titbit or by pleasant contact with man) or undesirable reaction or behaviour is punished. (1) Usually, habituation and operant conditioning are appearing not in their pure forms but are mixed. Some advices for proper handling To control animal’s movement on the pasture, the stockman should know the flight distance of the livestock. He should put himself on the edge of animal flight distance zone. Then, he would adjust his position–approach animals or withdraw to move or stop them. When moving animals, direction of the handler path should be opposite to the desired direction of the animals (Grandin 2004a). There are also recommendations concerning grouping animals before some handling procedures (Grandin, 2004b). For example, crowd pen linked with the chute should be filled only up to three quarters enabling animals to perform turning movements. Characteristics such as vision and flight distance must be also taken into account in this case. Hoof stock is very sensitive to moving objects that appear in the visual field, avoids shadows and the strong light. These environmental stimuli evoke the balking and backing reactions. Thus, special design of facility is needed with the curved corridor and its solid sides reducing visual field in animal and sufficiently separating from the human handler who works on the catwalk. Similar principles should be taken into account when designing squeezing chutes, loading ramps and corrals. Safety at animal handling 5 Polish Academy of Sciences, Institute of Genetics and Animal Breeding, Jastrzębiec, Poland Farm animals can be dangerous to stockpeople due to their body mass, strength, inconsistency in behavioural reactions and unpredictability. Females can be as dangerous as males when their young are threaten. On the base of case reports on accidents classified as dangerous to stockpeople, the following reactions of animals can be expected: Cattle: especially adult bulls: can hit the stockman with horns, push or press against the wall. Because of their weight, strength and inconsistent temperament bulls require extra caution. Even a bull’s playful activity can easily injure or kill a person. Bulls require special facilities that allow them to feed, drink, exercise and breed without direct contact with handlers. Cows can kick the stoclman/milker, mostly with hind legs. Horses of both sexes can strongly kick both with front and hind legs and bite severely. Stallions are very excitable when in proximity of a mare in heat. Pigs: adult boars and sows defending piglets can bite severely. Goats and sheep: adult males can hit with horns or even kill a man when they are provoked by stockman’s unusual behaviour (accidental falling down or squatting). Each animal has its own personality; however certain animal behaviours are predictable: • • • • • • • • • • • • Most animals respond positively to calm, gentle and consistent handling, Livestock become uneasy or skittish when their ordinary routines or familiar surroundings change. Do not alter the daily routine Animals have defined social order. Dominant animals have first choice of feed, location and direction of travel. Crowding a subordinate animal against a dominant one during handling may disrupt their social structure and cause an unpredictable and dangerous response, Domestic livestock especially cattle, sheep and horses are herd animals. They may become agitated or stressed when isolated and will try to return to the group, Livestock detect people by their movement which is much more important to animals than what is moving or the location, colour, or identity of the moving object. A handler’s excited or aggressive movements may cause animals to stop and watch the activity rather respond to handling. It is important to move calmly and steadily when handling animals, Always leave an escape route when working closely to animals, Avoid startling an animal especially a horse. Make it aware of your approach before getting too close to it, Yards, sheds should be suitable in size and strength for the animal, Avoid blind covers and sharp turns in the design of the yard, Keep the walkways dry and non-slip, Make sure your gates, footholds and access ways are well positioned, Keep all equipment in good repair. 6 Polish Academy of Sciences, Institute of Genetics and Animal Breeding, Jastrzębiec, Poland Literature 1. Belaev D.K. (1969): Domestication of animals. Science Journal 1, 47-52. 2. Boivin X., Le Neindre P., Boissy A., Lensink J., Trillat G., Veissier J. (2003): Eleveur et grands herbivores: une relation a entretenir. INRA Productions Animales, 16, 101-115. 3. Duncan J.J., Slee G.S., Kenlewell P., Berry P., Carlisle A.J. (1986): Comparison of the stressfulness of harvesting broiler chickens by machine and by hand. British Poultry Science 27, 109-114. 4. Estep D.Q., Hetta S. (1992): Interaction, relationship and bonds: the conceptual basis for scientist-animal relations. [In:] The Inevitable Bond: Examining Scientist-Animal Interactions. H. Davis and D. Balfour (Eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 6-26. 5. Fraser A.F., Broom D.M. (1997): Farm Animal Behaviour and Welfare. CABI Publishing, Wallingford, New York. 6. Grandin T. (2004a): Principles for Handling Grazing Animals. [In:] The Well-Being of Farm Animals. G.J. Benson & B.E. Rollin (Edsl), Blackwell Publ., Ames, 119144. 7. Grandin T. (2004b): Principles for the Design of Handling Facilities and Transport Systems. [In:]The Well-Being of Farm Animals. G.J. Benson & B.E. Rollin (Eds), Blackwell Publ., Ames, 145-166. 8. Gray J.A. (1987): The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. 9. Hemsworth P.H. (2004): Human –Livestock Interaction. [In:]The Well-Being of Farm Animals. G.J. Benson & B.E. Rollin (Eds), Blackwell Publ., Ames, 21-38. 10. Hemsworth P.H., Barnett J.L. (1987): Human-Animal Interactions. [In:] The Veterinary Clinics of North America, 3, 2, Farm Animal Behaviour 339-356, Philadelphia, Saunders. 11. Murphy R.M., Moura-Duarte F.A., Torres-Penedo M.C. (1981): Response of cattle to humans in open spaces: breed comparison and approach-avoidance relationship. Behavioural Genetics 11, 37-48 12. Seabrook M.F. (1972: A study to determine the influence of the herdsmen personality on milk yield. Journal of the Agricultural Labour Science 1, 45-59. 7 Polish Academy of Sciences, Institute of Genetics and Animal Breeding, Jastrzębiec, Poland 13. Seabrook M.F. (2000): The effect of the operational environment and operating protocols on the attitudes and behaviour of employed stockperson. The 4th NAHWOA Workshop, Clermont-Ferrand 14. Vaarst M. (1997): Introduction to milking and udder health in 1st lactation cows after calving-management routines in organic herds [In:] Organic milk production with focus on management, animal health and welfare. Internal Report no 97, Danish Institute for Agricultural Sciences43/49, 63-67. 8