THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIAL CIRCUMSTANCES AND RECOUNTED MUSICAL EXPERIENCES Naomi Ziv Yizre’el Valley College, Israel ABSTRACT The subjective perception of music and its effect on the individual depend not only on musical structure, but on the specific circumstances and the social situation in which it is heard. Music is integrated into an on-going construction of a personal narrative and receives meaning through its combination with other situational factors. The present study examined narratives of events in which music played an integral part. Participants were divided into three groups, according to the circumstances under which music was heard: alone, with one close person, and with a larger group of people. Results show differences between the subjective quality of experience with music in the three situations. When listening to music alone, participants were concentrated on themselves and their emotions. When listening with a close person, attention was turned to the relationship and communication with the other. When listening with a group of people, participants felt an enhancement of the self. Although results suggest the situation determined perception of the music, in participants’ narratives, the relationship was reversed, and music was seen as the cause of subjective experience of the situation 1. BACKGROUND Although much of the time music is part of the background and does not receive our full attention, it sometimes has the power to affect our sensations and perceptions very strongly. After a relatively long period in which music psychology was mainly interested in cognitive aspects of perception of the musical form, research seems to have shifted today towards more subjective aspects of the individual’s experience with music. Numerous studies have been conducted in an attempt to understand the way music influences the individual. On the one hand, there are those that search for a relationship between musical structures or styles and various stable personality traits (e.g.: Litle & Zuckerman, 1986; Rawlings et al., 1995; & Gosling, 2003), types of activities (North & Hargreaves, 1996; Kellaris & Robert, 1992), and physiological effects (Krumhansl, 1997; Sloboda, 1992; Panksepp, 1995). These studies seem to suggest generalizations about what music creates the various effects. A second group of studies look more closely at the subjective nature of personal experience with music, beyond specific styles. In this category are studies on reasons for listening to music (Sloboda et al. 2001), the role music plays at adolescence (Arnett, 1995; Frith, 1983), and the effect of music on self-development and identity (Macdonald et al., 2002). ISBN 1-876346-50-7 © 2004 ICMPC 571 Several studies analyze personal emotional reactions to or uses of music in real life (Sloboda et al., 2001; De Nora, 1999, 2000) or different factors of emotional reaction to music in peak experience (Gabrielson, 2001). These aim at generalization concerning the how of musical effect. While the above studies reveal the complex interaction participants have with music, and the various roles music plays in people’s lives, they do not tell us when particular aspects of the subjective experience are dominant. It seems logical to suppose that the nature of the interaction with music is not determined solely by listener characteristics or type of music, but is also influenced by other external variables, such as the particular social circumstances. Depending on whom we are with, we are more open to some effects and more closed to others. 2. AIMS The aim of the present study was to examine personal narratives about experiences with music and compare three types of situations: alone, with one other person in an intimate relationship, and in a larger group. It was hypothesized that each circumstance would give rise to a qualitatively different experience. More specifically, it was thought that music would serve to accentuate, or allow the expression and communication of, different emotional aspects in the three situations. Looking at narratives of personal events involving music, it should be possible to characterize the dominant aspects of each type of situation. 3. METHOD Participants Fifty-four participants took part in the study (13 males, 41 females, mean age: 27.4). All were first year students of psychology participating in obligatory research. None of the participants were musicians (i.e., all had less than eight years formal musical training). Materials Three questionnaires were used. ICMPC8, Evanston, IL, USA August 3-7, 2004 A. A Musical Preferences and Listening Habits questionnaire was developed for the study. Participants were asked yes/no questions regarding their liking for four categories of music (rock/pop, classical, world, techno).They were also asked questions on Likert-type scales regarding their listening habits: the frequency they listen to music alone, with other people, in the background, or as a main activity. Yes/ no questions were also asked regarding the general effect which music has on them (‘calms me’, ‘affects my mood’, ‘has a spiritual effect’). B. An Experience With Music questionnaire was also developed for the study. Participants were asked to recount an event in which music played an integral part, and were asked yes/no questions about their feelings: whether they felt love, excitement, elation, depression. They also indicated whether they felt ‘connected’, which they could answer ‘No’, ‘To myself’, or ‘To someone else’. C. A shortened version of the Eysenck Personality Inventory, measuring extraversion-introversion and neuroticism-stability (Ziv, 1986), was also administered. Procedure Participants were individually interviewed on general listening habits and musical preferences. They were asked to recount an event they remembered, in which music played an integral part. They were then asked to fill out the shortened Eysenck questionnaire. Interviews were recorded on tape. The questionnaires were completed by the experimenter as the participants talked. Thus, data included both quantitative material and a personal narrative. 4. RESULTS Participants were divided into three groups. Group 1 comprised participants who talked about an experience with music in which they were by themselves (7 males, 14 females); Group 2 comprised participants who talked about an experience in which they were with one partner (boyfriend, girlfriend, husband, wife) (2 males, 13 females); Group 3 comprised participants who talked about an experience in which they were with a group of people (a group of friends, at a concert, a wedding, etc.) (4 males, 14 females). This differentiation became the main independent variable in subsequent analyses. Chi-square tests were carried out to investigate differences in the three groups on the various quantitative measures. No differences were found between the three groups regarding listening habits, musical preferences and reasons for listening to music, or personality. However, in regards to the experience of music, two significant differences were found. Participants who talked about an experience in which they were by themselves tended to talk more about negative events, and felt more depressed than subjects in the other two groups (chi square=11.976, p=0.26). Eight of the 21 participants in this group felt very depressed, and two a little. Another significant difference was in relation to feeling “connected” to someone or to oneself (chi square=30.941, p<.001). Participants who were alone felt mostly connected to themselves, 572 participants who were with one other person felt connected to that person, and participants who were with a group of people were distributed more or less equally between the categories. Table 1. Group differences in feeling “connected” in the recounted event Group 1 (alone) Group 2 (with someone) Group 3 (with a group) total No To myself 3 2 17 0 To someone else 1 13 5 5 8 10 22 22 On the basis of recurrent themes within the narratives, eight new variables were created, within three major categories. Three independent judges rated the stories on yes/no scales on these variables. The first category was called ’Mental Changes’ and included 1. Improvement of mood: whether the participant felt his mood got better through listening to music; 2. Change of state of mind: whether there was a change in awareness; 3. Reminiscence: whether the event elicited memories of someone from the past, or some situation from the past, and 4. Emotional release: whether through listening to music, the participant felt he expressed his own emotions. The second category was labeled ‘Relationships’, and included 1. Communication of emotion: whether the music allowed the expression or communication of emotions to someone else; 2. Intensification of emotion: whether the music intensified a relationship; and 3. Symbolization of relationship: whether the music served to symbolize a relationship. The third category, ‘Enhancement of Self’, included 1. Part of something big: whether the participant felt through the music that he was a part of a larger scene or event. (Examples of excerpts for each variable are shown in appendix 1). Again using chi-square, all new variables produced significant differences between the three groups, as described below, and summarized in Table 2. Improvement of mood (chi square=7.644, p=.022). Music improved the mood of 6 out of the 21 participants who were alone. Of the other groups, music improved mood for only one participant from the group describing an event in which they were with a group of people. Change of state of mind (chi square=15.464, p<.001). The strongest change was in participants in the group which was alone in the event. In the group with one other person, no one felt such a change, and in the group with a group of people, only three participants felt this effect. Reminiscence: Participants who talked about an experience in which they were by themselves most strongly felt the effect of music in evoking past memories (chi square=7.347, p=.025). This effect was not found at all in subjects who talked about an ICMPC8, Evanston, IL, USA August 3-7, 2004 experience in which they were with one other person, and only in three participants who were in a group. about. However, the subjective impression was that music was the means through which these elements were best expressed. Emotion release: Participants who were alone in the recounted event felt the music helped them express the emotion they were feeling at the time, more than participant in the other two groups (chi square=10.389, p<.001). It is significant that personality and listening habits or musical taste were not related to the various recounted memories. This tends to suggest that in principle we may all experience a large variety of effects through music, and it is the particular circumstances, and our particular needs and emotional states, which influence the events’ shades and colors. Communication of emotion: Was music a means of communicating to someone? Participants in the group who talked about an experience with a boyfriend or girlfriend experienced this effect the most (chi square=17.476 p=.<.001) Intensification of emotion: Did music intensify the emotion felt? This effect was strongest in Group 2, who talked about an experience with one close person (chi square=22.612,p<.001). Part of something big: the feeling of being a part of something larger than oneself (chi square=20.28 p<.001). This effect was strongest for participants who were with a group of people. Table 2.Group differences in eight new variables Improvement of mood Change of state of mind Reminiscence Emotional release Communicating emotion Intensify emotion Symbolize relationship Part of something big 6. REFERENCES 1. Arnett, J.J. (1995), Adolescents’ uses of media for self-socialization, Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24(5), 519-533. 2. DeNora, T. (1999), Music as a Technology of the Self, Poetics,27, 31-56. 3. DeNora, T. (2000), Music in Everyday Life, Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press. 4. Frith, S. (1983), Sound Effects: Youth, Leisure, and the Politics of Rock, London: Constable. Group 1 N=21 6 Group 2 N=15 0 Group 3 N=18 1 13 0 5 5. Gabrielsson, A. (2001), Emotions in Strong Experiences with Music, in Juslin, P.N. & Sloboda, J. (eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 431-449. 8 11 0 0 2 8 4 2 2 6. Kellaris J. J. & Robert J. K. (1992), "The Influence of Music on Consumers’ Temporal Perceptions: Does Time Fly When You’re Having Fun?" Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1 (4), 365-376. 0 6 11 7 5 3 7. Krumhansl, C.L. (1997), an exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology, Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51(4), 336-353. 1 1 11 8. Litle, P. & Zuckerman, M. (1986), Sensation seeking and music preferences, Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 575-577. 5. CONCLUSIONS The study shows that the perceived dominant effect of music changes with the social circumstances in which individuals find themselves, at least as the events are recalled and recounted. The data suggest that, when alone, music allows us to enter more deeply into our idiosyncratic world, to explore our emotions and feelings, to deal with and cope with our problems, and perhaps to arrive at a better understanding of ourselves. In intimate relationships, music facilitates emotional communication, and may help us clarify our feelings towards the other person. When we are with a larger group of people, music may allow us to transcend ourselves, and to assume an enlarged perspective in which we are part of something bigger than ourselves. We may presume that it is not necessarily the music subjects listened to in the recounted events that produced these different effects, but the particular state of mind subjects were in at the time, which made them prone to feel the effects they talked 573 9. Macdonald, R.A.R.., Hargreaves, D.J. & Miell, D. (eds.), (2002), Musical Identities, Oxford: Oxford University Press. 10. North, A.C. & Hargreaves, D.J. (1996), Responses to music in aerobic exercise and yogic relaxation classes, British Journal of Psychology, 87, 535-547. 11. Panksepp, J. (1995): The emotional sources of “chills” induced by music, Music Perception, 13(2), 171-207. 12. Rawlings, D., Hodge, M., Sherr, D. and Dempsey, A. (1995). ‘Toughmindedness and preference for musical excerpts, categories and triads’, Psychology of Music, 23: 63-80. 13. Rentfrow, P.J. & Gosling, S.D. (2003), The Do Re Mi’s of everyday life: the structure and personality correlates of music preferences, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(6), 1236-1256. ICMPC8, Evanston, IL, USA August 3-7, 2004 14. Sloboda, J.A.(1992), Etudes empiriques de la réponse émotionnelle à la musique, Analyse Musicale, 14, 14-21. 18. number in this style). Indented 1.0 cm on the left, but don’t indent on the right. Reference text should be justified. 15. Sloboda, J. & O’Neill, S.A. (2001), Emotions in Everyday Listening to Music, in Juslin, P.N. & Sloboda, J. (eds.) (2001), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 415-429. 19. Lyon, R.F., and Mead, C. “An Analog Electronic Cochlea,” IEEE Trans. ASSP 36: 1119–1134, 1988. 16. Sloboda, J., O’Neill, S.A. & Ivaldi (2001), Functions of Music in Everyday Life: An Exploratory Study Using the Experience Sampling Method, Musicae Scientiae, 5, 9-32. 17. Ziv, A. (1986), Personality and Sense of Humor, New York: Springer. 20. Lee, K.-F., Automatic Speech Recognition: The Development of the SPHINX SYSTEM, Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston, 1989. 21. Rudnicky, A. I., Polifroni, Thayer, E H., and Brennan, R. “Interactive problem solving with speech”, J. Acoustic. Soc. Amer., Vol. 84, 1988, p S213(A). 7. APPENDIX Examples of new variables Variable Mood improvement Change in state of mind Reminiscence Emotional release Emotional communication Emotional intensification Symbolof relationship Being part of something big Examples s.22: “this song helped me…it helped me concentrate on how to get out of this situation”, s.53: “I felt calmer, like things got back into proportion”. s.30: “I felt like I was in a picture, it’s really not like me…it gets you out of the daily life”. s.18: “… it was kind of amusing, it’s like you’re looking at yourself from the outside, and the rain, and the music…”. s.33: “I connect to the memory of her. For a moment it’s like I’m with her”. s.91: “It like takes me back to the long-gone days…” “Music helped me liberate my feelings, the words say what I want to and can’t”; “It like brings everything out. It makes it easy for me to write”. s.13: “Music passes currents that are expressed by touch.” s.23: “It like expressed how we were in the same ‘groove’ somehow”. s.16: “We would combine the music with what we were doing…and it connected us”. s.66: “There was a piano solo that both me and my girlfriend connected to together.” s.3: Her wedding song: “The song really spoke to me. It’s our song”. s.23: at a club: “I watched her while we danced… and thought this really symbolizes our connection”. s.46: “The combination of the scenery, all those people who smoke all those things…and what connected people coming together.” s.57: “everyone who was there identified with everyone, got into the music”. 574