Split Coordination in the History of English Ann Taylor and Susan Pintzuk, University of York In this paper we investigate a construction which is often referred to descriptively as “split coordination”. “[O]ne of the most characteristic features of Old English syntax” (Perez Lorido 2009), it affects every type of coordinated constituent in OE: subject and object DPs, adnominal genitives, predicative and attributive ADJPs, ADVPs, PPs, and DP objects of P. An example of an OE split coordinated subject is given in (1). This is not just an OE phenomenon, however; it, or a superficially similar construction, occurs continuously, albeit with decreasing frequency, throughout the attested period up to the present day, as the parallel example in (2) from Present-Day English (PDE) demonstrates. Split coordination in PDE is most frequently analyzed as a type of Gapping known as Stripping or Bare Argument Ellipsis (BAE; see Johnson 2006 for an overview). Although the OE and PDE examples appear superficially similar, a closer look at the data suggests that we are dealing with two different phenomena. In PDE the 2nd conjunct is almost always clausefinal, but this is not the case in the early stages of English. Typical OE and Middle English examples are given in (3) and (4), with the 2nd conjunct positioned between the finite and non-finite verbs (3), and immediately after the non-finite verb but not clause-final (4). BAE cannot derive cases like those in (3) and (4), where verbs, arguments and adjuncts appear after the 2nd conjunct. Despite its synchronic range and diachronic persistence, split coordination has received surprisingly little attention in the diachronic literature (but cf. Sielanko 1994, Perez Lorido 2009); no empirical corpus-based studies of its use exist for any stage of the English language. Its occurrence in OE is often mentioned (Traugott 1972, Kohonen 1978, among many others); but beyond Perez Lorido’s (2009) limited study of split subjects in eight OE texts, it hasn’t been rigorously investigated. The reason for this lack may be the low frequency of split coordination throughout the history of English: while the number of cases is not negligible, it is small enough to necessitate annotated corpora and sophisticated search tools to collect sufficient examples for robust qualitative and quantitative results. We suggest a new analysis of split coordination in the early stages of English: Second Conjunct Stranding (SCS). When the coordinated constituent moves from its merged position, e.g. from Spec,vP to Spec,TP, the 2nd conjunct can be stranded either at the merge site or at any subsequent landing site. This analysis is reminiscent of quantifier stranding, and a similar analysis is used in Milićev and Milićević (2012) for split appositives in OE and Serbian. Unlike BAE, SCS accounts for the full range of positions of the split 2nd conjunct. Two basic diachronic trends can be seen in the history of English: first, the frequency of split coordination decreases over time, as shown in Table 1 for subjects. Although it is possible that the declining frequency of this construction is simply an external effect, perhaps the result of standardization and/or a prescription against split coordination in writing, a more plausible scenario is that BAE and SCS are indeed different processes, both of which exist in the early stages of English. BAE derives split coordination with the 2nd conjunct in clausefinal position; SCS derives split coordination with the 2nd conjunct either in clause-internal position or in clause-final position, depending upon the merge position of the coordinated constituent. Over time SCS is lost, leaving only BAE still in operation. This scenario is supported by the second diachronic trend: the frequency of clause-final 2nd conjuncts increases over time, as shown in Table 2. We suggest that BAE is and has always been a low frequency, possibly genre-sensitive, process, while SCS in the early stages of English is influenced by other factors (e.g. information structure and discourse/performance constraints) that serve to rearrange sentence constituents in the early language. (1) oðþæt þæt ad wæs forburnen, and ealle þa tunnan until the pile was burned and all the casks ‘until the pile and all the casks were burned up’ (YCOE: coaelive,+ALS_[Julian_and_Basilissa]:332.1143) (2) "Fear possessed me, and the certainty of war'', he has related. (BROWN) (3) þæt he sceolde & his ofspring his wed healdan that he should and his offspring his oath hold ‘that he and his offspring should hold his oath’ (YCOE: cocathom1,+ACHom_I,_6:225.49.1090) (4) Jhesus was clepid and hise disciplis to the weddyngis (PPCME: CMNTEST,II,1.122) Jesus was summoned and his disciples to the wedding ‘Jesus and his disciples were summoned to the wedding’ Table 1: The frequency of split coordinated subjects over time Period Split Total % Split coordinated subjects coordinated subjects Old English 437 2609 16.8% Middle English 264 2292 11.5% Early Modern 191 2963 6.5% English Early Modern 12 1442 0.8% British English Present Day English 6 2336 0.3% Table 2: The frequency of split coordinated subjects with clause-final 2nd conjuncts over time; the data are controlled for finiteness, type of conjunction and type of split Period Split coordinated Total split % Clausesubjects with clause- coordinated subjects final final 2nd conjuncts Old English 275 437 62.9% Middle English 191 264 72.3% Early Modern 154 191 80.6% English Early Modern 5 12 [41.7%] British English Present Day English 6 6 100.0% References Johnson, K. 2006. Gapping. In The Blackwell Companion to Syntax, (Eds, Everaert, M. & van Riemsdijk, H.) Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 407-435. Kohonen, V. 1978. On the development of English word order in religious prose around 1000 and 1200 AD. Åbo Akademi Foundation, Åbo. Milićev, T. & Milićević, N. 2012. Leftward movement with discontinuous appositive constructions. Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 59, 205-220. Perez Lorido, R. 2009. Reconsidering the role of syntactic “heaviness” in Old English split coordination. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia, 45, 31-56. Sielanko, E. 1994. Split coordinated structures in Late Old English. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia, 29, 57-72. Traugott, E.C. 1972. A History of English Syntax: A transformational approach to the History of English sentence structure. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.