Editorial Experimental Psychology Volume 50, No. 1, 2003 Experimental Psychology: Quo Vadis? By Karl Christoph Klauer Upon completion of the first volume of Experimental Psychology, it is appropriate to draw a brief résumé of journal operations and contents. Considering the first 12month period of effective operations, between 1 June 2001 and 31 May 2002, we received 63 manuscripts, of which three (5%) were accepted, 34 (54%) were accepted conditionally upon revision, and 26 (41%) were rejected. The rate of submissions steadily increased in this 12-month period. The mean processing time of these manuscripts, from date of submission until editorial decision, was 33 days ( SD = 12.9), and it ranged from 2 to 61 days. In 51 of 63 cases, we were able to keep the six-week deadline that is our target for manuscript processing time. Within this 12month period, 28 authors resubmitted revisions of their conditionally accepted manuscripts. The mean processing time of these papers was 15 days and thereby substantially shorter as sometimes the changes required by the reviewers were of a minor nature and a further review process was not needed. Many authors provided laudatory feedback about what they perceived to be a fast and efficient review process. Not only do we aim to publish manuscripts rapidly in print, but we also set ourselves the aim of putting the manuscripts online in the form of PDFs as soon as possible after the acceptance date. This means that individual subscribers are able to read prepublished papers online sometimes several weeks before they receive their print copies. This service was implemented in the course of 2002, and for Issues 3 and 4 the average time it took to put manuscripts online was 12.55 weeks. Another feature that was also introduced in the course of last year was a fully linked online full-text version of the journal as part of the Hogrefe & Huber PsyJOURNALS service. Further information about this new fully searchable journal articles database can be found at www.psycontent.com. Manuscripts were received from many parts of the world and were reviewed by an equally international set of reviewers whose names are listed in the fourth issue of the 2002 annual volume. Experimental Psychology attracted submissions from many different areas of psychology. Four areas received particular attention. These were memory, learning, reasoning and decision making, and social cognition. From different perspectives, a number of authors addressed memory phenomena. Vaterrodt-Plünnecke, Krüger, and Bredenkamp (2002) looked at the stochastical relationship between consciously controlled and automatic process in an implicit memory task, and Zimmer, Steiner, and Ecker (2002) addressed the related question of how far implicit color memory is independent of explicit recollection. Czienskowski and Giljohann (2002) studied the self-reference effect in memory in relation to the effects of concreteness and imageability of to-be-remembered material, and Blank (2002) was concerned with categorical relationships across the members of stimulusresponse pairs of successive lists in paired-associate learning and their effects on retroactive and proactive interference. Learning was also a frequent topic for authors publishing in Experimental Psychology, and papers addressed diverse forms of learning, ranging from relatively simple learning paradigms to learning in multimedia environments. Zirngibl and Koch (2002) manipulated response mode (manual vs. vocal) in sequence learning in a serial reaction time task and found different effects of response mode on implicit and explicit learning. Two papers focused on causal learning (De Houwer & Beckers, 2002; Shanks, 2002). De Houwer and Beckers (2002) demonstrated the phenomena of higher-order unovershadowing and higher-order backward blocking in human causal learning, and Shanks (2002) conducted a critical test of the power PC theory of causal induction (Cheng, 1997). Acquiring and generalizing mental transformations were the phenomena tackled by Müller (2002) and Müller and Gehrke (2002). Müller (2002) investigated transfer between different uses of function concepts and the effects of practice of a specific kind of use on transfer, while Müller and Gehrke (2002) assessed the role of different kinds of representations such as rules, mental models, instances, and chunks in the acquisition of mental operators. Learning in multimedia contexts was the topic of two papers, both of which built on cognitive-load theory (Sweller, 1988): Vollmeyer and Burns (2002) contrasted conditions with specific goals and with nonspecific, exploratory goals in learning the contents and use of a hypermedia program about the outbreak of World War I. A computer-based training program about the human cardiovascular system was used by Brünken, Steinbacher, Plass, and Leutner (2002), who assessed cognitive load during learning by means of dual-task methodology. Another class of papers turned to processes of reasoning, problem solving, and decision making. A case in point is the paper by Roberts (2002) demonstrating an inverted matching bias in disjunctive reasoning. The interpretation of conditional statements was the focus of a paper by Dieussaert, Schaeken, and d’Ydewalle (2002). Turning to decision making, Västfjäll and Gärling (2002) looked at affective reactions in a decision-making context. Nückles and Bromme (2002) studied how Internet experts plan explanations for laypersons as a function of the addressee’s intention and prior knowledge. Many authors tackled research questions from the area of social cognition. One issue was the cognitive antecedents and consequents of stereotyping (Araya, Akrami, Ekehammar, & Hedlund, 2002; Heredia & Blumentritt, 2002). Araya et al. (2002) demonstrated that unobtrusively raising the accessibility of the concept of self-control reduced prejudice in judging a member of a stereotyped group. Heredia and Blumentritt (2002) studied how far stereotype contents (e.g., bomb) mentioned in spoken language prime a stereotypically related person or category (e.g., Arab) under different temporal and contextual conditions. Another issue was biases and heuristics: Mussweiler (2002) took a new look at the anchor heuristic on the basis of his selective accessibility model (Mussweiler, 1997), whereas Pohl, Bender, and Lachmann (2002) investigated hindsight bias in different cultural contexts. In a study by Gawronski (2002), the convergent and discriminant validity of two IAT measures of ethnic prejudice was assessed, and De Houwer and Randell (2002) discussed another potential indirect measure of affect and prejudice, namely affective priming. They identify conditions under which an affective-priming effect is observed in the naming task. Other topics addressed by the authors of the first volume comprised priming effects on word identification under degraded viewing conditions (Eichstaedt, 2002), the application of social-cognitive theories to perceived personal vulnerability to sexual assault (Bohner, Danner, Siebler, & Samson, 2002), national differences in children’s event recall and suggestibility (Roebers, Bjorklund, Schneider, & Cassel, 2002), the comprehension of metaphoric reference (Stewart & Heredia, 2002), and pain sensitivity as a function of perceived control (Rothermund, Brandstädter, Meiniger, & Anton, 2002). A methodological contribution by Reips (2002) proposed standards for internet-based experiments. Forthcoming issues further expand the range of topics with papers on visual and auditory perception and attention. It is probably unavoidable and indeed desirable that a journal’s contents come to be focused around a few key topics in the course of time, giving the journal a distinctive profile, and Experimental Psychology was lucky to attract papers from core areas of experimental work in psychology. The trends that emerged for the first volume should not, however, discourage prospective authors from submitting papers from other areas of psychology. Experimental Psychology welcomes manuscripts reporting experimental work from any area of psychology. References Araya, T., Akrami, N., Ekehammar, B., & Hedlund, L.-E. (2002). Stereotype inhibition through priming of control-related words. Experimental Psychology, 49, 222-227. Blank, H. (2002). The role of horizontal categorization in retroactive and proactive interference. Experimental Psychology, 49, 196-207. Bohner, G., Danner, U. N., Siebler, F., & Samson, G. B. (2002). Rape myth acceptance and judgments of vulnerability to sexual assault: An Internet experiment. Experimental Psychology, 49, 257-269. Brünken, R., Steinbacher, S., Plass, J. L., & Leutner, D. (2002). Assessment of cognitive load in multimedia learning using dual-task methodology. Experimental Psychology, 49, 109-119. Cheng, P. W. (1997). From covariation to causation: A causal power theory. Psychological Review, 104, 367-405. Czienskowski, U., & Giljohann, S. (2002). Intimacy, concreteness, and the “selfreference effect”. Experimental Psychology, 49, 73-80. De Houwer, J., & Beckers, T. (2002). Second-order backward blocking and unovershadowing in human causal learning. Experimental Psychology, 49, 27-33. De Houwer, J., & Randell, T. (2002). Attention to primes modulates affective priming of pronunciation responses. Experimental Psychology, 49, 163-170. Dieussaert, K., Schaeken, W., & d’Ydewalle, G. (2002). The relative contribution of content and context factors on the interpretation of conditionals. Experimental Psychology, 49, 181-195. Eichstaedt, J. (2002). Measuring differences in preactivation on the Internet: The content category superiority effect. Experimental Psychology, 49, 283-291. Gawronski, B. (2002). What does the Implicit Association Test measure?: A test of convergent and discriminant validity of prejudice-related IATs. Experimental Psychology, 49, 171-180. Heredia, R. R., & Blumentritt, T. (2002). On-line processing of social stereotypes during spoken language comprehension. Experimental Psychology, 49, 208-221. Müller, B. (2002). Single-use versus mixed-use learning of transformations: Evidence for conceptual integration. Experimental Psychology, 49, 45-56. Müller, B. & Gehrke, J. (2002). Acquisition and use of mental operators: effects of type of practice. Experimental Psychology, 49, 141-152. Mussweiler, T. (1997). A selective accessibility model of anchoring: Linking the anchoring heuristic to hypothesis-consistent testing and semantic priming. Lengerich, Germany: Pabst. Mussweiler, T. (2002). The malleability of anchoring effects. Experimental Psychology, 49, 67-72. Nückles, M., & Bromme, R. (2002). Internet experts’ planning of explanations for laypersons: A Web experimental approach in the Internet domain. Experimental Psychology, 49, 292-304. Pohl, R. F., Bender, M., & Lachmann, G. (2002). Hindsight bias around the world. Experimental Psychology, 49, 270-282. Reips, U.-D. (2002). Standards for Internet-based experimenting. Experimental Psychology, 49, 243-256. Roberts, M. J. (2002). The elusive matching bias effect in the disjunctive selection task. Experimental Psychology, 49, 89-97. Roebers, C. M., Bjorklund, D. F., Schneider, W., & Cassel, W. S. (2002). Differences and similarities in event recall and suggestibility between children and adults in Germany and the United States. Experimental Psychology, 49, 132-140. Rothermund, K., Brandstädter, J., Meiniger, C., & Anton, F. (2002). Nociceptive sensitivity and control: Hypo- and hyperalgesia under two different modes of coping. Experimental Psychology, 49, 57-66. Shanks, D. R. (2002). Tests of the power PC theory of causal induction with negative contingencies. Experimental Psychology, 49, 81-88. Stewart, M. T., & Heredia, R. R. (2002). Comprehending spoken metaphoric reference: A real-time analysis. Experimental Psychology, 49, 34-44. Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257-285. Västfjäll, D., & Gärling, T. (2002). The dimensionality of anticipated affective reactions to risky and certain decision outcomes. Experimental Psychology, 49, 228238. Vaterrodt-Plünnecke, B., Krüger, T., & Bredenkamp, J. (2002). Process-dissociation procedure: A testable model for considering assumptions about the stochastic relation between consciously controlled and automatic processes. Experimental Psychology, 49, 3-26. Vollmeyer, R., & Burns, B. D. (2002). Goal specificity and learning with a hypermedia program. Experimental Psychology, 49, 98-108. Zimmer, H. D., Steiner, A., & Ecker, U. K. H. (2002). How “implicit” are implicit color effects in memory? Experimental Psychology, 49, 120-131. Zirngibl, C., & Koch, I. (2002). The impact of response mode on implicit and explicit sequence learning. Experimental Psychology, 49, 153-162.